Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-cphqk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-01-15T06:37:46.528Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Hominin cognition: The null hypothesis

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 January 2025

Duncan N. E. Stibbard-Hawkes*
Affiliation:
Department of Anthropology, Durham University, Durham, UK duncan.stibbard-hawkes@durham.ac.uk
*
*Corresponding author.

Abstract

The target article explores material culture datasets from three African forager groups. After demonstrating that these modern, contemporary human populations would leave scant evidence of symbolic behaviour or material complexity, it cautioned against using material culture as a barometer for human cognition in the deep past. Twenty-one commentaries broadly support or expand these conclusions. A minority offer targeted demurrals, highlighting (1) the soundness of reasoning from absence; and questioning (2) the “cognitively modern” null; (3) the role of hunter-gatherer ethnography; and (4) the pertinence of the inferential issues identified in the target article. In synthesising these discussions, this reply addresses all four points of demurral in turn, and concludes that there is much to be gained from shifting our null assumptions and reconsidering the probabilistic inferential links between past material culture and cognition.

Type
Author's Response
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2025. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Albessard-Ball, L., & Balzeau, A. (2018). Of tongues and men: A review of morphological evidence for the evolution of language. Journal of Language Evolution, 3(1), 7989.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
al-Nahar, M., & Olszewski, D. I. (2016). Early Epipaleolithic lithics, time-averaging, and site interpretations: Wadi al-Hasa region, Western Highlands of Jordan. Quaternary International, 396, 4051.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ames, C., Riel-Salvatore, J., & Collins, B. (2013). Why we need an alternative approach to the study of modern human behaviour. Canadian Journal of Archaeology, 37(1), 2147.Google Scholar
Anderson, A., Chilczuk, S., Nelson, K., Ruther, R., & Wall-Scheffler, C. (2023). The myth of man the hunter: Women's contribution to the hunt across ethnographic contexts. PLoS ONE, 18(6), e0287101.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Backwell, L., Bradfield, J., Carlson, K. J., Jashashvili, T., Wadley, L., & d'Errico, F. (2018). The antiquity of bow-and-arrow technology: Evidence from Middle Stone Age layers at Sibudu Cave. Antiquity, 92(362), 289303.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bagshawe, F. (1925). The peoples of the Happy Valley (East Africa) the aboriginal races of Kondoa Irangi. Part II: The Kangeju. Journal of the Royal African Society, 24, 117139.Google Scholar
Bahuchet, S. (1990). Food sharing among the pygmies of Central Africa. African Study Monographs, 11(1), 2753.Google Scholar
Baquedano, E., Arsuaga, J. L., Pérez-González, A., Laplana, C., Márquez, B., Huguet, R., … Higham, T. (2023). A symbolic Neanderthal accumulation of large herbivore crania. Nature Human Behaviour, 7(3), 342352.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barham, L., Duller, G. A. T., Candy, I., Scott, C., Cartwright, C. R., Peterson, J. R., … Nkombwe, P. (2023). Evidence for the earliest structural use of wood at least 476,000 years ago. Nature, 622(7981), 107111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barker, J. L., Power, E. A., Heap, S., Puurtinen, M., & Sosis, R. (2019). Content, cost, and context: A framework for understanding human signaling systems. Evolutionary Anthropology, 28(2), 8699.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bar-Yosef, O. (1997). Symbolic expressions in later prehistory of the Levant: Why are they so few? In Conkey, M. W., Soffer, O., Stratmann, D. & Jablonski, N. G. (Eds.), Beyond art: Pliestocene image and symbol (pp. 161188). California Academy of Sciences.Google Scholar
Bastos, A. P. M., & Taylor, A. H. (2020). Macphail's null hypothesis of vertebrate intelligence: Insights from avian cognition. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Belfer-Cohen, A., & Hovers, E. (2010). Modernity, enhanced working memory, and the middle to upper Paleolithic record in the Levant. Current Anthropology, 51(S1), S167S175.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Belitzky, S., Goren-Inbar, N., & Werker, E. (1991). A middle Pleistocene wooden plank with man-made Polish. Journal of Human Evolution, 20(4), 349353.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ben-Oren, Y., Kolodny, O., & Creanza, N. (2023a). Cultural specialization as a double-edged sword: Division into specialized guilds might promote cultural complexity at the cost of higher susceptibility to cultural loss. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 378(1872), 20210418.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ben-Oren, Y., Strassberg, S. S., Hovers, E., Kolodny, O., & Creanza, N. (2023b). Modelling effects of inter-group contact on links between population size and cultural complexity. Biology Letters, 19(4), 20230020.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berbesque, J. C., Wood, B. M., Crittenden, A. N., Mabulla, A., & Marlowe, F. W. (2016). Eat first, share later: Hadza hunter-gatherer men consume more while foraging than in central places. Evolution and Human Behavior, 37(4), 16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berger, L. R., Hawks, J., Fuentes, A., van Rooyen, D., Tsikoane, M., Ramalepa, M., … Molopyane, K. (2023). 241,000 to 335,000 years old rock engravings made by Homo naledi in the rising star cave system, South Africa. eLife, 12, 140.Google Scholar
Bird, R. B., & Codding, B. F. (2015). The sexual division of labor. In Scott, R. A. & Kosslyn, S. M. (Eds.), Emerging trends in the social and behavioral sciences (pp. 116). Wiley.Google Scholar
Blessing, M. A. (2023). Technological complexity as an indicator of behavioural modernity a case study on middle Palaeolithic birch tar production and South African microlithic technology. PhD thesis, University of Tübingen.Google Scholar
Blessing, M. A., Conard, N. J., & Bader, G. D. (2023). Cultural developments between the final MSA and the Robberg at Umbeli Belli, KwaZulu-Natal, South Africa. Journal of Paleolithic Archaeology, 6(1), 31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blurton Jones, N. (1982). Origins, functions, development, and motivation: Unity and disunity in the study of behavior. Journal of Anthropological Research, 38(4), 333349.Google Scholar
Blurton Jones, N. G., & Konner, M. J. (1989). !Kung knowledge of animal behavior. In Johannes, R. E. (Ed.), Traditional ecological knowledge: A collection of essays (pp. 2129). The World Conservation Union.Google Scholar
Blurton-Jones, N. G., & Marlowe, F. W. (2002). Selection for delayed maturity. Human Nature, 13(2), 199238.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boeckx, C. (2023). What made us “hunter-gatherers of words”. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 17, 112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boesch, C. (2002). Cooperative hunting roles among Taï chimpanzees. Human Nature, 13(1), 2746.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Borgerhoff Mulder, M., & Schacht, R. (2012). Human behavioural ecology (pp. 110). John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Botha, R. (2008). Prehistoric shell beads as a window on language evolution. Language & Communication, 28(3), 197212.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Botha, R. (2010). On the soundness of inferring modern language from symbolic behaviour. Cambridge Archaeological Journal, 20(3), 345356.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (2022). Large-scale cooperation in small-scale foraging societies. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews, 31(4), 175198.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Breyl, M. (2021). Triangulating Neanderthal cognition: A tale of not seeing the forest for the trees. WIREs Cognitive Science, 12(2), e1545.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Broesch, T., Crittenden, A. N., Beheim, B. A., Blackwell, A. D., Bunce, J. A., Colleran, H., … Mulder, M. B. (2020). Navigating cross-cultural research: Methodological and ethical considerations. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 287(1935), 20201245. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2020.1245CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brumm, A., & Moore, M. W. (2005). Symbolic revolutions and the Australian archaeological record. Cambridge Archaeological Journal, 15(2), 157175.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bruner, E. (2010). Morphological differences in the parietal lobes within the human genus: A neurofunctional perspective. Current Anthropology, 51(S1), S77S88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buckley, C. (2023). A template for recording material culture, and a comparison of four Oceanic societies. Retrieved from https://osf.io/preprints/psyarxiv/eu82yCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buckner, W. (2022). The intelligence of African hunters, and the ignorance of popular hereditarians. Retrieved from https://traditionsofconflict.substack.com/p/the-intelligence-of-african-huntersGoogle Scholar
Campbell, M. C., & Ranciaro, A. (2021). Human adaptation, demography and cattle domestication: An overview of the complexity of lactase persistence in Africa. Human Molecular Genetics, 30(R1), R98R109.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Castrillon, G., Epp, S., Bose, A., Fraticelli, L., Hechler, A., Belenya, R., … Riedl, V. (2023). An energy costly architecture of neuromodulators for human brain evolution and cognition. Science Advances, 9(50), eadi7632.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Codding, B. F., Bird, R. B., & Bird, D. W. (2011). Provisioning offspring and others: Risk-energy trade-offs and gender differences in hunter-gatherer foraging strategies. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 278(1717), 25022509.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, E., Davis, A. J., & Taylor, J. (2023). Interdependence, bonding and support are associated with improved mental wellbeing following an outdoor team challenge. Applied Psychology: Health and Well-Being, 15(1), 193216.Google ScholarPubMed
Cohen, E. E. A., Ejsmond-Frey, R., Knight, N., & Dunbar, R. I. M. (2009). Rowers’ high: Behavioural synchrony is correlated with elevated pain thresholds. Biology Letters, 6(1), 106108.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Collard, M., Buchanan, B., Morin, J., & Costopoulos, A. (2011). What drives the evolution of hunter-gatherer subsistence technology? A reanalysis of the risk hypothesis with data from the Pacific Northwest. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 366(1567), 11291138.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Collard, M., Kemery, M., & Banks, S. (2005). Causes of toolkit variation among hunter-gatherers: A test of four competing hypotheses. Canadian Journal of Archaeology, 29, 119.Google Scholar
Conard, N. J. (2015). Cultural evolution during the Middle and Late Pleistocene in Africa and Eurasia. In Henke, W. & Tattersall, I. (Eds.), Handbook of Paleoanthropology (pp. 24652508). Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Conde-Valverde, M., Martínez, I., Quam, R. M., Rosa, M., Velez, A. D., Lorenzo, C., … Arsuaga, J. L. (2021). Neanderthals and Homo sapiens had similar auditory and speech capacities. Nature Ecology & Evolution, 5(5), 609615.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coolidge, F. L., Haidle, M. N., Lombard, M., & Wynn, T. (2016). Bridging theory and bow hunting: Human cognitive evolution and archaeology. Antiquity, 90(349), 219228.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crema, E. R., Bortolini, E., & Lake, M. (2024). How cultural transmission through objects impacts inferences about cultural evolution. Journal of Archaeological Method and Theory, 31, 202–226. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10816-022-09599-xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crittenden, A. N., & Zes, D. A. (2015). Food sharing among hadza hunter-gatherer children. PLoS ONE, 10(7), e0131996.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cronk, L. (1991). Human behavioral ecology. Annual Review of Anthropology, 20(1), 2553.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cunningham, J. J. (2003). Transcending the “obnoxious spectator”: A case for processual pluralism in ethnoarchaeology. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology, 22(4), 389410.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
David, B., Mullett, R., Wright, N., Stephenson, B., Ash, J., Fresløv, J., … Matheson, C. D. (2024). Archaeological evidence of an ethnographically documented Australian aboriginal ritual dated to the last ice age. Nature Human Behaviour, 8, 14811492.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davidson, D. S. (1936). The spearthrower in Australia. Proceedings of the American Philosophical Society, 76(4), 445483.Google Scholar
de León, M. S. P., Bienvenu, T., Akazawa, T., & Zollikofer, C. P. E. (2016). Brain development is similar in Neanderthals and modern humans. Current Biology, 26(14), R665R666.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dennett, D. C. (1983). Intentional systems in cognitive ethology: The “Panglossian paradigm” defended. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 6(3), 343355.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
d'Errico, F. (2003). The invisible frontier. A multiple species model for the origin of behavioral modernity. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews, 12(4), 188202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
d'Errico, F., Backwell, L., Villa, P., Degano, I., Lucejko, J. J., Bamford, M. K., … Beaumont, P. B. (2012). Early evidence of San material culture represented by organic artifacts from Border Cave, South Africa. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 109(33), 1321413219.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
d'Errico, F., & Stringer, C. B. (2011). Evolution, revolution or saltation scenario for the emergence of modern cultures? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 366(1567), 10601069.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Détroit, F., Mijares, A. S., Corny, J., Daver, G., Zanolli, C., Dizon, E., … Piper, P. J. (2019). A new species of Homo from the Late Pleistocene of the Philippines. Nature, 568(7751), 181186.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dibble, H. L., Holdaway, S. J., Lin, S. C., Braun, D. R., Douglass, M. J., Iovita, R., … Sandgathe, D. (2017). Major fallacies surrounding stone artifacts and assemblages. Journal of Archaeological Method and Theory, 24(3), 813851.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elkins, C. (2022). Legacy of violence: A history of the British empire. Knopf Doubleday Publishing Group.Google Scholar
Fajardo, S., Kozowyk, P. R. B., & Langejans, G. H. J. (2023). Measuring ancient technological complexity and its cognitive implications using Petri nets. Scientific Reports, 13(1), 14961.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Falk, D. (2024). The botanic age: Planting the seeds of human evolution. University of Toronto Press.Google Scholar
Fisher, S. R. A. (1928). Statistical methods for research workers (1st ed.). Oliver and Boyd.Google Scholar
Foecke, K. K., Queffelec, A., & Pickering, R. (2024). No sedimentological evidence for deliberate burial by Homo naledi – A case study highlighting the need for best practices in geochemical studies within archaeology and paleoanthropology. PaleoAnthropology, 122.Google Scholar
Frantz, L. A. F., Bradley, D. G., Larson, G., & Orlando, L. (2020). Animal domestication in the era of ancient genomics. Nature Reviews Genetics, 21(8), 449460.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
French, J. C. (2024). Sex, gender, and the division of labour in the European Middle and Upper Palaeolithic. In Moen, M. & Pedersen, U. (Eds.), The Routledge handbook of gender archaeology (pp. 120). Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781003257530-14Google Scholar
Fuentes, A., Kissel, M., Spikins, P., Molopyane, K., Hawks, J., & Berger, L. R. (2023). Burials and engravings in a small-brained hominin, Homo naledi, from the late Pleistocene: Contexts and evolutionary implications. eLife, 12, RP89125. https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.89125.1Google Scholar
Gabora, L., & Smith, C. (2019). Exploring the psychological basis for transitions in the archaeological record. In Henley, T. B., Rossano, M. J. & Kardas, E. P. (Eds.), Handbook of cognitive archaeology: Psychology in prehistory (pp. 220240). Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gaine, S. F. (2021). Did Christ die for Neanderthals? New Blackfriars, 102(1098), 225238.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gold, K. C., & Watson, L. M. (2018). In memorium: Koko, a remarkable gorilla. American Journal of Primatology, 80(12), e22930.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gosselain, O. P. (2016). To hell with ethnoarchaeology!. Archaeological Dialogues, 23(2), 215228.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Graeber, D., & Wengrow, D. (2021). The dawn of everything: A new history of humanity. Penguin UK.Google Scholar
Gray, P. (2009). Play as a foundation for hunter-gatherer social existence. American Journal of Play, 1(4), 476522.Google Scholar
Grove, M. (2009). Hunter-gatherer movement patterns: Causes and constraints. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology, 28(2), 222233.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gurven, M. D. (2018). Broadening horizons: Sample diversity and socioecological theory are essential to the future of psychological science. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 115(45), 1142011427.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haidle, M. (2009). How to think a simple spear? In De Beaune, S., Coolidge, F. L. & Wynn, T. (Eds.), Cognitive archaeology and human evolution (pp. 5773). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Haidle, M. N. (2016). Lessons from Tasmania – cultural performance versus cultural capacity. In Haidle, M. N., Conard, N. J. & Bolus, M. (Eds.), The nature of culture: Based on an interdisciplinary symposium “the nature of culture”, Tübingen, Germany (pp. 717). Springer Netherlands.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harari, Y. N. (2014). Sapiens: A brief history of humankind. Random House.Google Scholar
Harari, Y. N., Vandermuelen, D., & Casanave, D. (2020). Sapiens: A graphic history: The birth of humankind (Vol. 1). Harper Collins.Google Scholar
Hardy, B. L., Moncel, M.-H., Kerfant, C., Lebon, M., Bellot-Gurlet, L., & Mélard, N. (2020). Direct evidence of Neanderthal fibre technology and its cognitive and behavioral implications. Scientific Reports, 10(1), 4889.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). The weirdest people in the world. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33(2–3), 6183.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henrich, J., Kline, M., Muthukrishna, M., Shennan, S., Thomas, M. (2016). Appendix to understanding cumulative cultural evolution: A reply to Vaesen, Collard, et al. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 113(44), 137.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henshilwood, C. S., & Dubreuil, B. (2009). Reading the artifacts: Gleaning language skills from the Middle Stone Age in Southern Africa. In Knight, C. & Botha, R. (Eds.), The cradle of language (pp. 4161). Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henshilwood, C. S., & Dubreuil, B. (2011). The still bay and Howiesons Poort, 77–59 ka: Symbolic material culture and the evolution of the mind during the African Middle Stone Age. Current Anthropology, 52(3), 361400.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hodgson, G. M. (2012). On the limits of rational choice theory. Economic Thought, 1, 94108.Google Scholar
Hoffman, J., Farquharson, K., & Venkataraman, V. (2023). The ecological and social context of women's hunting in small-scale societies. Journal of Hunter-Gatherer Research, 7(1–2), 131.Google Scholar
Hoffmann, D. L., Standish, C. D., García-Diez, M., Pettitt, P. B., Milton, J. A., Zilhão, J., & Pike, A. W. (2020). Response to White et al.'s reply: “Still no archaeological evidence that Neanderthals created Iberian cave art”. Journal of Human Evolution, 144, 14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hopkinson, T. (2011). The transmission of technological skills in the Palaeolithic: Insights from metapopulation ecology. In Roberts, B. W. & Vander Linden, M. (Eds.), Investigating archaeological cultures: Material culture, variability and transmission (pp. 230244). Springer.Google Scholar
Ho, S. Y., Larson, G., Edwards, C. J., Heupink, T. H., Lakin, K. E., Holland, P. W., & Shapiro, B. (2008). Correlating Bayesian date estimates with climatic events and domestication using a bovine case study. Biology Letters, 4(4), 370374.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hovers, E., & Belfer-Cohen, A. (2006). “Now you see it, now you don't” – Modern human behavior in the Middle Paleolithic. In Hovers, E. & Kuhn, S. L. (Eds.), Transitions before the transition: Evolution and stability in the Middle Paleolithic and Middle Stone Age (pp. 295304). Springer US.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hovers, E., & Belfer-Cohen, A. (2024). A Pleistocene record of making symbols. In Overmann, K. A. & Coolidge, F. L. (Eds.), Oxford handbook of cognitive archaeology (pp. 485504). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hussain, S. T., & Will, M. (2021). Materiality, agency and evolution of lithic technology: An integrated perspective for Palaeolithic archaeology. Journal of Archaeological Method and Theory, 28(2), 617670.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ichikawa, M. (2021). Elephant hunting by the Mbuti hunter-gatherers in the eastern Congo Basin. In Konidaris, G. E., Barkai, R., Tourloukis, V. & Harvati, K. (Eds.), Human-Elephant interactions: From past to present (pp. 455467). Tübingen University Press.Google Scholar
Jorstad, N. L., Song, J. H. T., Exposito-Alonso, D., Suresh, H., Castro-Pacheco, N., Krienen, F. M., … Bakken, T. E. (2023). Comparative transcriptomics reveals human-specific cortical features. Science, 382(6667), eade9516.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kelly, R. L. (2013). The lifeways of hunter-gatherers: The foraging spectrum. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kelly, R. L., Mackie, M. E., & Kandel, A. W. (2023). Rapid increase in production of symbolic artifacts after 45,000 years ago is not a consequence of taphonomic bias. Journal of Archaeological Science, 160, 105885.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kent, S. (1991). The relationship between mobility strategies and site structure. In Kroll, E. M. & Price, T. D. (Eds.), The interpretation of archaeological spatial patterning (pp. 3359). Springer US.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kent, S. (1996). Hunting variability at a recently sedentary Kalahari village. In Kent, S. (Ed.), Cultural diversity among twentieth-century foragers: An African perspective (pp. 513536). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Killin, A., & Pain, R. (2021). Cognitive archaeology and the minimum necessary competence problem. Biological Theory, 18(4), 269283.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kissel, M., & Fuentes, A. (2021). The ripples of modernity: How we can extend paleoanthropology with the extended evolutionary synthesis. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews, 30(1), 8498.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klein, R. G. (1995). Anatomy, behavior, and modern human origins. Journal of World Prehistory, 9(2), 167198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klein, R. G. (2017). Language and human evolution. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 43, 204221.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kline, M. A., & Boyd, R. (2010). Population size predicts technological complexity in Oceania. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 277(1693), 25592564.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kochiyama, T., Ogihara, N., Tanabe, H. C., Kondo, O., Amano, H., Hasegawa, K., … Akazawa, T. (2018). Reconstructing the Neanderthal brain using computational anatomy. Scientific Reports, 8(1), 6296.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ku, P. W., Steptoe, A., Lai, Y. J., Hu, H. Y., Chu, D., Yen, Y. F., … Chen, L. J. (2019). The associations between near visual activity and incident myopia in children: A nationwide 4-year follow-up study. Ophthalmology, 126(2), 214220.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuhlwilm, M., & Boeckx, C. (2019). A catalog of single nucleotide changes distinguishing modern humans from archaic hominins. Scientific Reports, 9(1), 8463.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuhn, S. L., & Stiner, M. C. (2007a). Body ornamentation as information technology: Towards and understanding of the significance of early beads. In Mellars, P. A., Bar-Yosef, O. & Stringer, C. B. (Eds.), Rethinking the human revolution: New behavioural and biological perspectives on the origins and dispersals of modern humans (pp. 4554). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kuhn, S. L., & Stiner, M. C. (2007b). Paleolithic ornaments: Implications for cognition, demography and identity. Diogenes, 54(2), 4048.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Labra, N., Mounier, A., Leprince, Y., Rivière, D., Didier, M., Bardinet, E., … Balzeau, A. (2024). What do brain endocasts tell us? A comparative analysis of the accuracy of sulcal identification by experts and perspectives in palaeoanthropology. Journal of Anatomy, 244(2), 274296.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lacy, S., & Ocobock, C. (2024). Woman the hunter: The archaeological evidence. American Anthropologist, 126(1), 1931.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lane, P. (2014). Hunter-gatherer-fishers, ethnoarchaeology, and analogical reasoning. In Cummings, V., Jordan, P., & Zvelebil, M. (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of the archaeology and anthropology of hunter-gatherers (pp. 104150). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Lang, M., & Kundt, R. (2023). The evolution of human ritual behavior as a cooperative signaling platform. Religion, Brain & Behavior, 123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Langley, M. C., Clarkson, C., & Ulm, S. (2011). From small holes to grand narratives: The impact of taphonomy and sample size on the modernity debate in Australia and New Guinea. Journal of Human Evolution, 61(2), 197208.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lavi, N., Rudge, A., & Warren, G. (2024). Rewild your inner hunter-gatherer: How an idea about our ancestral condition is recruited into popular debate in Britain and Ireland. Current Anthropology, 65(1), 7299.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Layton, R. (2001). Hunter-gatherers, their neighbours and the nation state. In Panter-Brick, C., Rowley-Conwy, P. & Layton, R. (Eds.), Hunter-Gatherers: An interdisciplinary perspective (pp. 292321). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Leder, D., Lehmann, J., Milks, A., Koddenberg, T., Sietz, M., Vogel, M., … Terberger, T. (2024). The wooden artifacts from Schöningen's Spear Horizon and their place in human evolution. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 121(15), e2320484121.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, M. D., & Vanpaemel, W. (2018). Determining informative priors for cognitive models. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 25(1), 114127.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, R. B. (2011). Eating Christmas in the Kalahari. In Boyd, C. E. & Lassiter, L. E. (Eds.), Explorations in cultural anthropology (pp. 4148). AltaMira Press.Google Scholar
Lemoine, N. P. (2019). Moving beyond noninformative priors: Why and how to choose weakly informative priors in Bayesian analyses. Oikos, 128(7), 912928.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leonardi, M., Gerbault, P., Thomas, M. G., & Burger, J. (2012). The evolution of lactase persistence in Europe. A synthesis of archaeological and genetic evidence. International Dairy Journal, 22(2), 8897.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lew-Levy, S., Andersen, M. M., Lavi, N., & Riede, F. (2022a). Hunter-gatherer children's object play and tool use: An ethnohistorical analysis. Frontiers in Psychology, 13, 824983.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lew-Levy, S., Bombjaková, D., Milks, A., Kiabiya Ntamboudila, F., Kline, M. A., & Broesch, T. (2022b). Costly teaching contributes to the acquisition of spear hunting skill among BaYaka forager adolescents. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 289(1974), 20220164.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lew-Levy, S., Boyette, A. H., Crittenden, A. N., Hewlett, B. S., & Lamb, M. E. (2020). Gender-typed and gender-segregated play among Tanzanian Hadza and Congolese BaYaka hunter-gatherer children and adolescents. Child Development, 91(4), 12841301.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lew-Levy, S., Reckin, R., Lavi, N., Cristóbal-Azkarate, J., & Ellis-Davies, K. (2017). How do hunter-gatherer children learn subsistence skills?: A meta-ethnographic review. Human Nature, 28(4), 367394.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lewontin, R. C. (1998). The evolution of cognition: Questions we will never answer. In Osherson, D. N., Sternberg, S. & Scarborough, D. (Eds.), An invitation to cognitive science (pp. 107132). The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Lieberman, D. E., Bramble, D. M., Raichlen, D. A., & Shea, J. J. (2007b). The evolution of endurance running and the tyranny of ethnography: A reply to Pickering and Bunn (2007). Journal of Human Evolution, 53(4), 439442.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lieberman, D., Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (2007a). The architecture of human kin detection. Nature, 445(7129), 727731.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liu, C., & Stout, D. (2023). Inferring cultural reproduction from lithic data: A critical review. Evolutionary Anthropology, 32(2), 8399.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lombard, M. (2016). Mountaineering or ratcheting? Stone Age hunting weapons as proxy for the evolution of human technological, behavioral and cognitive flexibility. In Haidle, M. N., Conard, N. J. & Bolus, M. (Eds.), The nature of culture: Based on an interdisciplinary symposium “the nature of culture”, Tübingen, Germany, vertebrate paleobiology and paleoanthropology (pp. 135146). Springer Netherlands.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mace, R. (2014). Human behavioral ecology and its evil twin. Behavioral Ecology, 25(3), 443449.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Macphail, J. G. S. (1930). The Bandala method of hunting elephant on foot. Sudan Notes and Records, 13(2), 279283.Google Scholar
Macphail, E. M., Barlow, H. B., & Weiskrantz, L. (1985). Vertebrate intelligence: The null hypothesis. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. B, Biological Sciences, 308(1135), 3751.Google Scholar
Macphail, E. M. (1987). The comparative psychology of intelligence. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 10(4), 645656.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Macphail, E. M., & Bolhuis, J. J. (2001). The evolution of intelligence: Adaptive specializations versus general process. Biological Reviews, 76(3), 341364.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maher, L. A. (2019). Persistent place-making in prehistory: The creation, maintenance, and transformation of an Epipalaeolithic landscape. Journal of Archaeological Method and Theory, 26(3), 9981083.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maher, L. A., & Conkey, M. (2019). Homes for hunters? Exploring the concept of home at hunter-gatherer sites in Upper Paleolithic Europe and Epipaleolithic Southwest Asia. Current Anthropology, 60(1), 91137.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marlowe, F. W. (2004). Mate preferences among Hadza hunter-gatherers. Human Nature, 15(4), 365376.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marlowe, F. W. (2010). The Hadza: Hunter-gatherers of Tanzania. University of California Press.Google Scholar
Martinón-Torres, M., Garate, D., Herries, A. I. R., & Petraglia, M. D. (2023). No scientific evidence that Homo naledi buried their dead and produced rock art. Journal of Human Evolution, 195, 103464.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mazza, P. P. A., Martini, F., Sala, B., Magi, M., Colombini, M. P., Giachi, G., … Ribechini, E. (2006). A new Palaeolithic discovery: Tar-hafted stone tools in a European Mid-Pleistocene bone-bearing bed. Journal of Archaeological Science, 33(9), 13101318.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McBrearty, S. (2013). Advances in the study of the origin of humanness. Journal of Anthropological Research, 69(1), 731.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McBrearty, S., & Brooks, A. S. (2000). The revolution that wasn't: A new interpretation of the origin of modern human behavior. Journal of Human Evolution, 39(5), 453563.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McBrearty, S., & Stringer, C. (2007). The coast in colour. Nature, 449(7164), 793794.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCall, G. S. (2000). Ju/’hoansi adaptations to a cash economy. African Sociological Review, 4(1), 138155.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCauley, B., Collard, M., & Sandgathe, D. (2020). A cross-cultural survey of on-site fire use by recent hunter-gatherers: Implications for research on Palaeolithic pyrotechnology. Journal of Paleolithic Archaeology, 3(4), 566584.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDowell, W. (1984). A brief history of the Mangola Hadza. Technical report, Mbulu District Development Directorate.Google Scholar
McElreath, R. (2016). Statistical rethinking: A Bayesian course with examples in R and Stan. Chapman and Hall/CRC.Google Scholar
McElreath, R., Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (2003). Shared norms and the evolution of ethnic markers. Current Anthropology, 44(1), 122130.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGrew, W. C. (2010). In search of the last common ancestor: New findings on wild chimpanzees. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 365(1556), 32673276.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meneganzin, A., & Currie, A. (2022). Behavioural modernity, investigative disintegration & Rubicon expectation. Synthese, 200(1), 47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meneganzin, A., & Killin, A. (2024). Beyond reasonable doubt: Reconsidering Neanderthal aesthetic capacity. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 133.Google Scholar
Metcalfe, T. (2023). Earliest evidence of wooden construction uncovered. Retrieved from https://www.scientificamerican.com/article/earliest-evidence-of-wooden-construction-uncovered/Google Scholar
Milks, A. (2020). A review of ethnographic use of wooden spears and implications for Pleistocene hominin hunting. Open Quaternary, 6(1), 120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Milks, A., Lehmann, J., Leder, D., Sietz, M., Koddenberg, T., Böhner, U., … Terberger, T. (2023). A double-pointed wooden throwing stick from Schöningen, Germany: Results and new insights from a multianalytical study. PLoS ONE, 18(7), e0287719.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, J. M., & Wang, Y. V. (2022). Ostrich eggshell beads reveal 50,000-year-old social network in Africa. Nature, 601(7892), 234239.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mithen, S. (2024a). How Neanderthal language differed from modern human – they probably didn't use metaphors. The Conversation. Retrieved from https://theconversation.com/how-neanderthal-language-differed-from-modern-human-they-probably-didnt-use-metaphors-229942Google Scholar
Mithen, S. (2024b). This is what a Neanderthal conversation would have sounded like. Psyche. Retrieved from https://psyche.co/ideas/this-is-what-a-neanderthal-conversation-would-have-sounded-likeGoogle Scholar
Moreau, L. (2020). Social inequality before farming? Multidisciplinary approaches to the study of social organization in prehistoric and ethnographic hunter-gatherer-fisher societies. McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research.Google Scholar
Morgan, I. G., French, A. N., & Rose, K. A. (2018). Intense schooling linked to myopia. BMJ, 361(June), 23.Google ScholarPubMed
Morin, E., & Winterhalder, B. (2024). Ethnography and ethnohistory support the efficiency of hunting through endurance running in humans. Nature Human Behaviour, 8(6), 10651075.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moritz, J. M. (2012). Human uniqueness, the other hominids, and “anthropocentrism of the gaps” in the religion and science dialogue. Zygon®, 47(1), 6596.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moritz, J. M. (2015). Does Jesus save the Neanderthals? Theological perspectives on the evolutionary origins and boundaries of human nature. Dialog, 54(1), 5160.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mounier, A., Noûs, C., & Balzeau, A. (2020). Palaeoneurology and the emergence of language. Bulletins et Mémoires de la Société d'Anthropologie de Paris, 32(3–4), 147157.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mountjoy, E., Davies, N. M., Plotnikov, D., Smith, G. D., Rodriguez, S., Williams, C. E., … Atan, D. (2018). Education and myopia: Assessing the direction of causality by Mendelian randomisation. BMJ (Online), 361, 111.Google ScholarPubMed
Neubauer, S. (2014). Endocasts: Possibilities and limitations for the interpretation of human brain evolution. Brain, Behavior and Evolution, 84(2), 117134.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neyman, J., & Pearson, E. S. (1928). On the use and interpretation of certain test criteria for purposes of statistical inference: Part I. Biometrika, 20A(1/2), 175240.Google Scholar
Noetling, F. (1911). Notes on the hunting sticks (lughkana), spears (perenna), and baskets (tughbrana)of the Tasmanian Aborigines. Papers and Proceedings of the Royal Society of Tasmania, 64106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nowell, A. (2013). Cognition, behavioral modernity, and the archaeological record of the middle and early upper paleolithic. In Hatfield, G. & Pittman, H. (Eds.), Evolution of mind, brain, and culture (pp. 235262). University of Pennsylvania Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ocobock, C., & Lacy, S. (2024). Woman the hunter: The physiological evidence. American Anthropologist, 126(1), 718.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O'Connell, J. F. (1995). Ethnoarchaeology needs a general theory of behavior. Journal of Archaeological Research, 3(3), 205255.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O'Connell, J. F., Hawkes, K., & Jones, N. B. (1991). Distribution of refuse-producing activities at Hadza residential base camps. In Price, T. D. (Ed.), The interpretation of archaeological spatial patterning (pp. 6176). Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oktaviana, A. A., Joannes-Boyau, R., Hakim, B., Burhan, B., Sardi, R., Adhityatama, S., … Aubert, M. (2024). Narrative cave art in Indonesia by 51,200 years ago. Nature, 631(8022), 814818.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ozaita, J., Baronchelli, A., & Sánchez, A. (2022). Ethnic markers and the emergence of group specific norms: An experiment. Scientific Reports, 12(1), 5068.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Padilla-Iglesias, C., & Bischoff, R. J. (2024). Hunter-gatherer mobility patterns influence the reconstruction of social networks from archaeological assemblages. Journal of Archaeological Science: Reports, 59, 104798.Google Scholar
Padilla-Iglesias, C., Blanco-Portillo, J., Pricop, B., Ioannidis, A. G., Bickel, B., Manica, A., … Migliano, A. B. (2024). Deep history of cultural and linguistic evolution among Central African hunter-gatherers. Nature Human Behaviour, 8(7), 12631275.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Page, A. E., & French, J. C. (2020). Reconstructing prehistoric demography: What role for extant hunter-gatherers? Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews, 29(6), 332345.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paige, J., & Perreault, C. (2024). 3.3 million years of stone tool complexity suggests that cumulative culture began during the Middle Pleistocene. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 121(26), e2319175121.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Panofsky, A., Dasgupta, K., & Iturriaga, N. (2021). How White nationalists mobilize genetics: From genetic ancestry and human biodiversity to counterscience and metapolitics. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 175(2), 387398.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pargeter, J., MacKay, A., Mitchell, P., Shea, J., & Stewart, B. A. (2016). Primordialism and the “Pleistocene San” of Southern Africa. Antiquity, 90(352), 10721079.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pascual-Garrido, A., & Almeida-Warren, K. (2021). Archaeology of the perishable ecological constraints and cultural variants in chimpanzee termite fishing. Current Anthropology, 62(3), 251388.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Patterson, F. G., & Cohn, R. H. (1990). Language acquisition by a lowland gorilla: Koko's first ten years of vocabulary development. WORD, 41(2), 97143.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pauly, R., Johnson, L., Feltus, F. A., & Casanova, E. L. (2024). Enrichment of a subset of Neanderthal polymorphisms in autistic probands and siblings. Molecular Psychiatry, 29, 3452–3461. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41380-024-02593-7CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pepperberg, I. M. (2020). The comparative psychology of intelligence: Some thirty years later. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pernet, C. (2016). Null hypothesis significance testing: A short tutorial. F1000Research, 4, 621.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peterson, N. (1993). Demand sharing: Reciprocity and the pressure for generosity among foragers. American Anthropologist, 95(4), 860874.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pickering, T. R., & Bunn, H. T. (2007). The endurance running hypothesis and hunting and scavenging in savanna-woodlands. Journal of Human Evolution, 53(4), 434438.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pollom, T. R., Cross, C. L., Herlosky, K. N., Ford, E., & Crittenden, A. N. (2021). Effects of a mixed-subsistence diet on the growth of Hadza children. American Journal of Human Biology, 33(1), 15.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Powell, A., Shennan, S., & Thomas, M. G. (2009). Late Pleistocene demography and the appearance of modern human behavior. Science, 324(5932), 12981301.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Premo, L. S. (2015). Mobility and cultural diversity in central-place foragers: Implications for the emergence of modern human behavior. In Mesoudi, A. & Aoki, K. (Eds.), Learning strategies and cultural evolution during the Palaeolithic, (pp. 4565). Springer Japan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Putol, R. (2024). Humans started “cumulative culture” around 600,000 years ago. Earth.com. Retrieved from https://www.earth.com/news/humans-started-cumulative-culture-around-600000-years-ago/Google Scholar
Reuning, H. (1972). Psychological studies of Kalahari bushmen. In Cronbach, L. J. & Drenth, P. J. D. (Eds.), Mental tests and cultural adaptation (pp. 171182). De Gruyter.Google Scholar
Rhys, B. (2024). Ancient man did most of the hunting after all, claims scientist. The Times.Google Scholar
Riede, F., Lew-Levy, S., Johannsen, N. N., Lavi, N., & Andersen, M. M. (2023). Toys as teachers: A cross-cultural analysis of object use and enskillment in hunter–gatherer societies. Journal of Archaeological Method and Theory, 30(1), 3263.Google Scholar
Russ, J. (2024). Origins of cumulative culture in human evolution. Science Daily. Retrieved from: https://www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2024/06/240617173730.htmGoogle Scholar
Sahlins, M. D. (1972). The original affluent society. In Stone age economics (chapter 1, pp. 139). Aldine Atherton, Inc.Google Scholar
Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S., Shanker, S., & Taylor, T. J. (1998). Apes, language, and the human mind. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sayers, K., & Lovejoy, C. O. (2008). The chimpanzee has no clothes: A critical examination of Pan troglodytes in models of human evolution. Current Anthropology, 49(1), 8799.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sayers, K., Raghanti, M. A., & Lovejoy, C. O. (2012). Human evolution and the chimpanzee referential doctrine. Annual Review of Anthropology, 41(1), 119138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scerri, E. M. L., Thomas, M. G., Manica, A., Gunz, P., Stock, J. T., Stringer, C., … Chikhi, L. (2018). Did our species evolve in subdivided populations across Africa, and why does it matter? Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 33(8), 582594.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scerri, E. M., & Will, M. (2023). The revolution that still isn't: The origins of behavioral complexity in Homo sapiens. Journal of Human Evolution, 179, 103358.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schacter, D. L., & Addis, D. R. (2007). The cognitive neuroscience of constructive memory: Remembering the past and imagining the future. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 362(1481), 773.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schrire, C. (1980). An inquiry into the evolutionary status and apparent identity of San huntergatherers. Human Ecology, 8(1), 932.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sear, R. (2022). “National IQ” datasets do not provide accurate, unbiased or comparable measures of cognitive ability worldwide. OSF. Retrieved from https://osf.io/preprints/psyarxiv/26vfbCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shea, J. J. (2011). Homo sapiens is as homo sapiens was: Behavioral variability versus “behavioral modernity” in paleolithic archaeology. Current Anthropology, 52(1), 135.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shott, M. (1986). Technological organization and settlement mobility: An ethnographic examination. Journal of Anthropological Research, 42(1), 1551.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Singh, M. (2022). Primitive communism: Marx's idea that societies were naturally egalitarian and communal before farming is widely influential and quite wrong. AEON. Retrieved from https://aeon.co/essays/the-idea-of-primitive-communism-is-as-seductive-as-it-is-wrongGoogle Scholar
Singh, M., & Glowacki, L. (2022). Human social organization during the Late Pleistocene: Beyond the nomadic-egalitarian model. Evolution and Human Behavior, 43(5), 418431.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skaanes, T. (2017). Cosmology matters: Power objects, rituals, and meat-sharing among the Hadza of Tanzania. PhD thesis, Aarhaus University.Google Scholar
Skov, L., Peyrégne, S., Popli, D., Iasi, L. N. M., Devièse, T., Slon, V., … Peter, B. M. (2022). Genetic insights into the social organization of Neanderthals. Nature, 610(7932), 519525.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Slimak, L. (2024). The naked Neanderthal: A new understanding of the human creature. Pegasus Books.Google Scholar
Smith, E. A., Bettinger, R. L., Bishop, C. A., Blundell, V., Cashdan, E., Casimir, M. J., … Stini, W. A. (1983). Anthropological applications of optimal foraging theory: A critical review [and comments and reply]. Current Anthropology, 24(5), 625651.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, K. M., & Apicella, C. L. (2022). Hadza hunter-gatherers are not deontologists and do not prefer deontologists as social partners. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 101, 104314.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, L. (1977). Hadza equipment list (unpublished).Google Scholar
Solway, J. S., Lee, R. B., & Barnard, A. (1990). Foragers, genuine or spurious?: Situating the Kalahari San in history. Current Anthropology, 31(2), 109146.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Speth, J. D. (2004). News flash: Negative evidence convicts Neanderthals of gross mental incompetence. World Archaeology, 36(4), 519526.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stagnaro, M. N., Stibbard-Hawkes, D. N., & Apicella, C. L. (2022). Do religious and marketbased institutions promote cooperation in Hadza hunter-gatherers? Religion, Brain and Behavior, 12(1–2), 171189.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stander, P. E. (1992). Cooperative hunting in lions: The role of the individual. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 29(6), 445454.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sterelny, K. (2011). From hominins to humans: How sapiens became behaviourally modern. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 366(1566), 809822.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sterelny, K. (2014). A Paleolithic reciprocation crisis: Symbols, signals, and norms. Biological Theory, 9(1), 6577.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sterelny, K. (2016). Cumulative cultural evolution and the origins of language. Biological Theory, 11(3), 173186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sterelny, K. (2019). The archaeology of the extended mind (pp. 143158). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Sterelny, K. (2021a). Demography and cultural complexity. Synthese, 198(9), 85578580.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sterelny, K. (2021b). Foragers and their tools: Risk, technology and complexity. Topics in Cognitive Science, 13(4), 728749.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sterelny, K. (2021c). Veiled agency? Children, innovation and the archaeological record. Evolutionary Human Sciences, 3, e12.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stibbard-Hawkes, D. N. E. (2020). Egalitarianism and democratized access to lethal weaponry: A neglected approach. In Moreau, L. (Ed.), Social inequality before farming? Multidisciplinary approaches to the study of social organization in prehistoric and ethnograpic hunter-gatherer-fisher societies. McDonald institute conversations (pp. 83102). McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research.Google Scholar
Stibbard-Hawkes, D. N. E. (2023). Did we all go together when we went? Considering the adaptive importance of risky joint action. Religion, Brain & Behavior, 15.Google Scholar
Stibbard-Hawkes, D. N. E., & Apicella, C. L. (2022). Myopia rates among Hadza hunter-gatherers are low but not exceptional. American Journal of Biological Anthropology, 179(4), 655667.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stibbard-Hawkes, D. N. E., Attenborough, R. D., Mabulla, I. A., & Marlowe, F. W. (2020). To the hunter go the spoils? No evidence of nutritional benefit to being or marrying a well-reputed Hadza hunter. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 173(1), 6179.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stibbard-Hawkes, D. N. E., Attenborough, R. D., & Marlowe, F. W. (2018). A noisy signal: To what extent are Hadza hunting reputations predictive of actual hunting skills? Evolution and Human Behavior, 39(6), 639651.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stibbard-Hawkes, D. N. E., Smith, K. M., & Apicella, C. L. (2022). Why hunt? Why gather? Why share? Hadza assessments of foraging and food-sharing motive. Evolution and Human Behavior, 43(3), 257272.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stibbard-Hawkes, D. N. E., Amir, D., & Apicella, C. L. (2023). A cost for signaling: Do Hadza hunter-gatherers forgo calories to show-off in an experimental context? Evolution and Human Behavior, 44(5), 398410. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2022.10.004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stibbard-Hawkes, D. N. E., Abarbanell, L., Mabulla, I. A., Endeko, E. S., Legare, C. H., & Apicella, C. L. (2024). Foreign-language effects in cross-cultural behavioral research: Evidence from the Tanzanian Hadza. PNAS Nexus, 3(6), pgae218.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stout, D., Chaminade, T., Apel, J., Shafti, A., & Faisal, A. A. (2021). The measurement, evolution, and neural representation of action grammars of human behavior. Scientific Reports, 11(1), 13720.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stout, D., & Hecht, E. (2023). Evolutionary neuroarchaeology. In Overmann, K. A. & Coolidge, F. L. (Eds.), Oxford handbook of cognitive archaeology (pp. 197222). Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stringer, C. (2002). Modern human origins: Progress and prospects. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 357(1420), 563579.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Suddendorf, T., Kirkland, K., Bulley, A., Redshaw, J., & Langley, M. C. (2020). It's in the bag: Mobile containers in human evolution and child development. Evolutionary Human Sciences, 2, e48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Suresh, H., Crow, M., Jorstad, N., Hodge, R., Lein, E., Dobin, A., … Gillis, J. (2023). Comparative single-cell transcriptomic analysis of primate brains highlights human-specific regulatory evolution. Nature Ecology & Evolution, 7(11), 19301943.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sutton-Smith, B., & Roberts, J. M. (1971). The cross-cultural and psychological study of games. International Review of Sport Sociology, 6(1), 7987.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tanno, T. (1981). Plant utilization of the Mbuti pygmies – with special reference to their material culture and use of wild vegetable foods. African Study Monographs, 1, 153.Google Scholar
Tennie, C., Bandini, E., van Schaik, C. P., & Hopper, L. M. (2020). The zone of latent solutions and its relevance to understanding ape cultures. Biology & Philosophy, 35(5), 55.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tennie, C., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Ratcheting up the ratchet: On the evolution of cumulative culture. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 364(1528), 24052415.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tennie, C., Premo, L. S., Braun, D. R., & McPherron, S. P. (2017). Early stone tools and cultural transmission: Resetting the null hypothesis. Current Anthropology, 58(5), 652672.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Timmer, J. (2024). When did humans start social knowledge accumulation? Ars technica. Retrieved from https://arstechnica.com/science/2024/06/stone-tool-archeological-record-shows-recent-boost-in-sophistication/Google Scholar
Tribble, E. B., & Keene, N. (2011). Introduction: Cognitive ecologies, distributed cognition, extended mind and memory studies. In Tribble, E. B. & Keene, N. (Eds.), Cognitive ecologies and the history of remembering: Religion, education and memory in early modern England, Palgrave Macmillan memory studies (pp. 118). Palgrave Macmillan UK.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turnbull, C. (1982). The ritualization of potential conflict between the sexes among the Mbuti. In Leacock, E. & Lee, R. B. (Eds.), Politics and history in band societies (pp. 133156). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Venkataraman, V. V., Hoffman, J., Farquharson, K., Davis, H. E., Hagen, E. H., Hames, R. B., … Stibbard-Hawkes, D. N. E. (2024). Female foragers sometimes hunt, yet gendered divisions of labor are real: A comment on Anderson et al. (2023) The Myth of Man the Hunter. Evolution and Human Behavior, 45(4), 106586.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Villa, P., & Roebroeks, W. (2014). Neandertal demise: An archaeological analysis of the modern human superiority complex. PLoS ONE, 9(4), e96424.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wadley, L. (2021). What stimulated rapid, cumulative innovation after 100,000 years ago? Journal of Archaeological Method and Theory, 28(1), 120141.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wallach, E. (2019). Inference from absence: The case of archaeology. Palgrave Communications, 5(1), 110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wall-Scheffler, C., Geiger, K., & Steudel-Numbers, K. (2007). Infant carrying: The role of increased locomotory costs in early tool development. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 133(2), 841846.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wiessner, P. (1983). Style and social information in Kalahari San projectile points. American Antiquity, 48(2), 253276.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wiessner, P. (1984). Reconsidering the behavioral basis for style: A case study among the Kalahari San. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology, 3(3), 190234.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wiessner, P. (2022). Hunter-gatherers: Perspectives from the starting point. Cliodynamics: The Journal of Quantitative History and Cultural Evolution, Review 3, 16.Google Scholar
Wilkie, D. S., & Curran, B. (1991). Research report why do Mbuti hunters use efficiency of archers and net-hunters in the Ituri rain. American Anthropologist, 93(3), 680689.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilkins, J., Schoville, B. J., Brown, K. S., & Chazan, M. (2012). Evidence for early hafted hunting technology. Science, 338(6109), 942946.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilmsen, E. N., & Denbow, J. R. (1990). Paradigmatic history of san-speaking peoples and current attempts at revision. Current Anthropology, 31, 489.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Winterhalder, B. (1986). Diet choice, risk, and food sharing in a stochastic environment. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology, 5(4), 369392.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wobst, H. M. (1978). The archaeo-ethnology of hunter-gatherers or the tyranny of the ethnographic record in archaeology. American Antiquity, 43(2), 303309.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wood, B. M., Harris, J. A., Raichlen, D. A., Pontzer, H., Sayre, K., Sancilio, A., … Jones, J. H. (2021). Gendered movement ecology and landscape use in Hadza hunter-gatherers. Nature Human Behaviour, 5(4), 436446.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Woodburn, J. (1970). Hunters and gatherers: The material culture of the nomadic Hadza. British Museum.Google Scholar
Woodburn, J. (1982). Egalitarian societies. Man, 17(3), 431451.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Woodburn, J. (1997). Indigenous discrimination: The ideological basis for local discrimination against hunter-gatherer minorities in sub-Saharan Africa. Ethnic and Racial Studies, 20(2), 345361.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Woodburn, J. (1998). Sharing is not a form of exchange: An analysis of property-sharing in immediate-return hunter-gatherer societies. In Hann, C. M. (Ed.), Property relations: Renewing the anthropological tradition (pp. 4863). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Wynn, T., Overmann, K. A., & Malafouris, L. (2021). 4E cognition in the Lower Palaeolithic. Adaptive Behavior, 29(2), 99106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yellen, J. E. (1977). Archaeological approaches to the present: Models for reconstructing the past. Studies in Archaeology. Academic Press.Google Scholar
Yellen, J. E. (1991). Small mammals: Post-discard patterning of !Kung San faunal remains. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology, 10(2), 152192.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zheng, Z., Wang, X., Li, M., Li, Y., Yang, Z., Wang, X., … Jiang, Y. (2020). The origin of domestication genes in goats. Science Advances, 6(21), eaaz5216.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zilhão, J. (2007). The emergence of ornaments and art: An archaeological perspective on the origins of “behavioral modernity”. Journal of Archaeological Research, 15(1), 154.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zilhão, J. (2014). Neanderthal-modern human contact in western Eurasia: Issues of dating. Taxonomy, and cultural associations. In Akazawa, T., Nishiaki, Y. & Aoki, K. (Eds.), Dynamics of learning in Neanderthals and modern humans volume 1 (pp. 2157). Springer Japan.Google Scholar
Zilhão, J., Angelucci, D. E., Badal-García, E., D'Errico, F., Daniel, F., Dayet, L., … Zapata, J. (2010). Symbolic use of marine shells and mineral pigments by Iberian Neanderthals. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 107(3), 10231028.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zonker, J., Padilla-Iglesias, C., & Djurdjevac Conrad, N. (2023). Insights into drivers of mobility and cultural dynamics of African hunter-gatherers over the past 120 000 years. Royal Society Open Science, 10(11), 230495.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed