No CrossRef data available.
Article contents
Is social interaction based on guile or honesty?
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 03 February 2011
Abstract
Von Hippel & Trivers suggest that people enhance their own self-views as a means of persuading others to adopt similarly inflated perceptions of them. We question the existence of a pervasive desire for self-enhancement, noting that the evidence the authors cite could reflect self-verification strivings or no motive whatsoever. An identity negotiation framework provides a more tenable approach to social interaction.
- Type
- Open Peer Commentary
- Information
- Copyright
- Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2011
References
Bandura, A. (1982) Self-efficacy mechanism in human agency. American Psychologist
37:122–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cassidy, J. (1988) Child–mother attachment and the self in six-year olds. Child Development
59:121–34.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chambers, J. R. & Windschitl, P. D. (2004) Biases in social comparative judgments: The role of nonmotivated factors in above-average and comparative-optimism effects. Psychological Bulletin
130:813–38.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diener, E. & Diener, M. (1995) Cross-cultural correlates of life satisfaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
68:653–63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Epley, N. & Whitchurch, E. (2008) Mirror, mirror on the wall: Enhancement in self recognition. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin
34:1159–70.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fein, S. & Spencer, S. J. (1997) Prejudice as self-image maintenance: Affirming the self through derogating others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
73:31–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klar, Y. & Giladi, E. E. (1997) No one in my group can be below the group's average: A robust positivity bias in favor of anonymous peers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
73(5):885–901.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kwang, T. & Swann, W. B. Jr., (2010) Do people embrace praise even when they don't feel worthy? A review of critical tests of self-enhancement versus self-verification. Personality and Social Psychology Review
14:263–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sroufe, L. A. (1989) Relationships, self, and individual adaptation. In: Relationship disturbances in early childhood: A developmental approach, ed. Sameroff, A. J. & Emde, R. N., pp. 70–94. Basic.Google Scholar
Swann, W. B. Jr., (1983) Self-verification: Bringing social reality into harmony with the self. In: Social psychological perspectives on the self, vol. 2, ed. Suls, J. & Greenwald, A. G., pp. 33–66. Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Swann, W. B. Jr., (1987) Identity negotiation: Where two roads meet. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
53:1038–51.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swann, W. B. Jr., (in press) Self-verification theory. In: Handbook of theories of social psychology, ed. Van Lang, P., Kruglanski, A. & Higgins, E. T.. Sage.Google Scholar
Swann, W. B. Jr., & Bosson, J. (2008) Identity negotiation: A theory of self and social interaction. In: Handbook of personality psychology: Theory and research, ed. John, O., Robins, R. & Pervin, L., pp. 448–71. Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Swann, W. B. Jr., Chang-Schneider, C. & McClarty, K. (2007) Do our self-views matter? Self-concept and self-esteem in everyday life. American Psychologist
62:84–94.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Target article
Is social interaction based on guile or honesty?
Related commentaries (1)
The evolution and psychology of self-deception