Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-dh8gc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-14T09:37:52.176Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Why we assume it's all good: The role of theory of mind in early inherent feature inferences

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  24 October 2014

Karen Bartsch
Affiliation:
Psychology Department, University of Wyoming, Laramie, WY 82071-3415. bartsch@uwyo.eduhttp://www.uwyo.edu/psychology/faculty/bartsch.htmlestes@uwyo.eduhttp://www.uwyo.edu/psychology/faculty/estes.html
David Estes
Affiliation:
Psychology Department, University of Wyoming, Laramie, WY 82071-3415. bartsch@uwyo.eduhttp://www.uwyo.edu/psychology/faculty/bartsch.htmlestes@uwyo.eduhttp://www.uwyo.edu/psychology/faculty/estes.html

Abstract

Cimpian & Salomon's (C&S's) characterization of a domain-general inherence heuristic, available to young children, underplays the importance of our early interest in and recognition of agency, intentionality, and mental life. A consideration of the centrality of desires, goals, and agency in our earliest reasoning suggests an alternative, perhaps complementary, account of our tendency to be satisfied with the status quo.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2014 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Astington, J. W. (2001) The paradox of intention: Assessing children's metarepresentational understanding. In: Intentions and intentionality: Foundations of social cognition, ed. Malle, B. F., Moses, L. J. & Baldwin, D. A., pp. 85103. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Bartsch, K. & Wellman, H. M. (1995) Children talk about the mind. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Dennett, D. C. (1987) The intentional stance. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Gopnik, A. & Wellman, H. M. (1994) The theory theory. In: Mapping the mind: Domain specificity in culture and cognition, ed. Hirschfeld, L. A. & Gelman, S. A., pp. 257–93. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kahneman, D. (2011) Thinking, fast and slow. Farrar, Straus, and Giroux.Google Scholar
Lillard, A. S. & Flavell, J. H. (1990) Young children's preference for mental state versus behavioral descriptions of human action. Child Development 61:731–41.Google Scholar
Onishi, K. H. & Baillargeon, R. (2005) Do 15-month-old infants understand false beliefs? Science 308:255–58.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1929) The child's conception of the world. Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Repacholi, B. M. & Gopnik, A. (1997) Early reasoning about desires: Evidence from 14- and 18-month-olds. Developmental Psychology 33:1221. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.33.1.12.Google Scholar
Wellman, H. M. (1990) The child's theory of mind. MIT Press.Google Scholar