Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-d8cs5 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-01-15T06:04:35.596Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Cultural innovation is not only a product of cognition but also of cultural context

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 January 2025

Yotam Ben-Oren*
Affiliation:
Department of Ecology, Evolution, and Behavior, Silberman Institute for Life Science, The Hebrew University of Jerusalem, Jerusalem, Israel Yotam.ben-oren@mail.huji.ac.il Oren.kolodny@mail.huji.ac.il; https://scholar.google.com/citations?user=PxhzC24AAAAJ&hl=en&oi=ao; https://sites.google.com/view/oren-kolodny-homepage
Erella Hovers
Affiliation:
Institute of Archaeology, The Hebrew University of Jerusalem, Jerusalem, Israel Hovers@mail.huji.ac.il; https://archaeology.huji.ac.il/people/erella-hovers
Oren Kolodny
Affiliation:
Department of Ecology, Evolution, and Behavior, Silberman Institute for Life Science, The Hebrew University of Jerusalem, Jerusalem, Israel Yotam.ben-oren@mail.huji.ac.il Oren.kolodny@mail.huji.ac.il; https://scholar.google.com/citations?user=PxhzC24AAAAJ&hl=en&oi=ao; https://sites.google.com/view/oren-kolodny-homepage
Nicole Creanza*
Affiliation:
Department of Biological Sciences, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN, USA nicole.creanza@vanderbilt.edu; http://nicolecreanza.com Evolutionary Studies Initiative, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN, USA
*
*Corresponding authors.
*Corresponding authors.

Abstract

Innovations, such as symbolic artifacts, are a product of cognitive abilities but also of cultural context. Factors that may determine the emergence and retention of an innovation include the population's pre-existing cultural repertoire, exposure to relevant ways of thinking, and the invention's utility. Thus, we suggest that the production of symbolic artifacts is not guaranteed even in cognitively advanced societies.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2025. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Barham, L., Duller, G. A. T., Candy, I., Scott, C., Cartwright, C. R., Peterson, J. R., … Nkombwe, P. (2023). Evidence for the earliest structural use of wood at least 476,000 years ago. Nature, 622(7981), 107111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bar-Yosef, O. (1997). Symbolic expressions in later prehistory of the Levant: Why are they so few. Beyond Art: Pleistocene Image and Symbol, 23, 161187.Google Scholar
Ben-Oren, Y., Jaffe, Y. Y., & Kolodny, O. (2023a). Shifting from correlations to dynamics in the study of human prehistorical resilience to extreme climatic shifts. bioRxiv, 2023-09. https://doi.org/10.1101/2023.09.19.558513Google Scholar
Ben-Oren, Y., Kolodny, O., & Creanza, N. (2023b). Cultural specialization as a double-edged sword: Division into specialized guilds might promote cultural complexity at the cost of higher susceptibility to cultural loss. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 378(1872), 20210418.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ben-Oren, Y., Strassberg, S. S., Hovers, E., Kolodny, O., & Creanza, N. (2023c). Modelling effects of inter-group contact on links between population size and cultural complexity. Biology Letters, 19(4), 20230020.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Creanza, N., Kolodny, O., & Feldman, M. W. (2017a). Cultural evolutionary theory: How culture evolves and why it matters. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 114(30), 77827789.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Creanza, N., Kolodny, O., & Feldman, M. W. (2017b). Greater than the sum of its parts? Modelling population contact and interaction of cultural repertoires. Journal of the Royal Society, Interface/the Royal Society, 14(130), 20170171. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsif.2017.0171CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feldman, M. W., & Creanza, N. (2018). Integrative studies of cultural evolution: Crossing disciplinary boundaries to produce new insights. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 373(1743), 20170048. https://royalsocietypublishing.org/doi/abs/10.1098/rstb.2017.0048Google Scholar
Fogarty, L., & Creanza, N. (2017). The niche construction of cultural complexity: Interactions between innovations, population size and the environment. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 372(1735), 20160428. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2016.0428CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gardner, R. A., & Gardner, B. T. (1969). Teaching sign language to a chimpanzee. Science, 165(3894), 664672.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenbaum, G., Fogarty, L., Colleran, H., Berger-Tal, O., Kolodny, O., & Creanza, N. (2019a). Are both necessity and opportunity the mothers of innovations? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 42, e199. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X19000207CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenbaum, G., Friesem, D. E., Hovers, E., Feldman, M. W., & Kolodny, O. (2019b). Was inter-population connectivity of Neanderthals and modern humans the driver of the Upper Paleolithic transition rather than its product? Quaternary Science Reviews, 217, 316329.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henrich, J. (2004). Demography and cultural evolution: How adaptive cultural processes can produce maladaptive losses – the Tasmanian case. American Antiquity, 69(2), 197214.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henshilwood, C. S., d'Errico, F., & Van Niekerk, K. L. (2011). A 100,000-year-old ochre-processing workshop at Blombos Cave, South Africa. Science, 334, 219222. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1211535CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hovers, E. (1990). Art in the Levantine Epi-Palaeolithic: An engraved pebble from a Kebaran site in the lower Jordan valley. Current Anthropology, 31(3), 317322.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hovers, E., & Belfer-Cohen, A. (2006). “Now you see it, now you don't” – modern human behavior in the Middle Paleolithic. In Hovers, E., & Kuhn, S. L. (Eds.), Transitions before the transition: Evolution and stability in the Middle Paleolithic and Middle Stone Age (pp. 295304). Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hovers, E., & Belfer-Cohen, A. (2022). A Pleistocene record of making symbols. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Jaubert, J., Verheyden, S., Genty, D., Soulier, M., Cheng, H., Blamart, D., … Santos, F. (2016). Early Neanderthal constructions deep in Bruniquel Cave in southwestern France. Nature, 534(7605), 111114.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kolodny, O., Creanza, N., & Feldman, M. W. (2015). Evolution in leaps: The punctuated accumulation and loss of cultural innovations. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 112(49), E6762E6769.Google ScholarPubMed
Kolodny, O., Creanza, N., & Feldman, M. W. (2016). Game-changing innovations: How culture can change the parameters of its own evolution and induce abrupt cultural shifts. PLoS Computational Biology, 12(12), e1005302.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lenneberg, E. H. (1967). The biological foundations of language. Hospital Practice, 2(12), 5967.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mcbrearty, S., & Brooks, A. S. (2000). The revolution that wasn't: A new interpretation of the origin of modern human behavior. Journal of Human Evolution, 39(5), 453563.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paige, J., & Perreault, C. (2024). 3.3 million years of stone tool complexity suggests that cumulative culture began during the Middle Pleistocene. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 121(26), e2319175121.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Powell, A., Shennan, S., & Thomas, M. G. (2009). Late Pleistocene demography and the appearance of modern human behavior. Science, 324(5932), 12981301.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scerri, E. M. L., & Will, M. (2023). The revolution that still isn't: The origins of behavioral complexity in Homo sapiens. Journal of Human Evolution, 179, 103358.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strassberg, S. S., & Creanza, N. (2021). Cultural evolution and prehistoric demography. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 376(1816), 20190713.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vanhaeren, M., d'Errico, F., Stringer, C., & James, S. L. (2006). Middle Paleolithic shell beads in Israel and Algeria. Science, 319, 17851788. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1128139CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zilhão, J., Angelucci, D. E., Badal-García, E., d'Errico, F., Daniel, F., Dayet, L., … Zapata, J. (2010). Symbolic use of marine shells and mineral pigments by Iberian Neandertals. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107(3), 10231028.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed