Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gvvz8 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-27T06:41:26.185Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

G but not g: In search of the evolutionary continuity of intelligence

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  15 August 2017

Moran Bar-Hen-Schweiger
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Ben-Gurion University of the Negev, Beer-Sheva, Israel 84105moranbar@post.bgu.ac.il
Avraham Schweiger
Affiliation:
Department of Behavioral Sciences, Academic College of Tel Aviv, Israel 61083schweige@mta.ac.il
Avishai Henik*
Affiliation:
New York City Medical & Neurological Offices, New York, NY 11373henik@bgu.ac.ilhttp://www.bgu.ac.il/~henik
*
Corresponding author: Avishai Henik

Abstract

Conceptualizing intelligence in its biological context, as the expression of manifold adaptations, compels a rethinking of measuring this characteristic in humans, relying also on animal studies of analogous skills. Mental manipulation, as an extension of object manipulation, provides a continuous, biologically based concept for studying G as it pertains to individual differences in humans and other species.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2017 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Anderson, M. L. (2010) Neural reuse: A fundamental organizational principle of the brain. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 33:245313.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Byrne, R. W. & Corp, N. (2004) Neocortex size predicts deception rate in primates. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences 271:1693–99.Google Scholar
Deary, I. J., Penke, L. & Johnson, W. (2010) The neuroscience of human intelligence differences. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 11(3):201–11.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gottfredson, L. S. (1997) Mainstream science on intelligence: An editorial with 52 signatories, history, and bibliography. Intelligence 24:1323.Google Scholar
Greenfield, P. M. (1991) Language, tools and brain: The ontogeny and phylogeny of hierarchically organized sequential behavior. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14:531–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Owen, K. (1992) The suitability of Raven's Standard Progressive Matrices for various groups in South Africa. Personality and Individual Differences 13(2):149–59.Google Scholar
Pahl, M., Si, A. & Zhang, S. (2013) Numerical cognition in bees and other insects. Frontiers in Psychology 4:162. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00162.Google Scholar
Parker, S. T. & Gibson, K. R. (1977) Object manipulation, tool use and sensorimotor intelligence as feeding adaptations in Cebus monkeys and great apes. Journal of Human Evolution 6(7):623–41.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1952) The origins of intelligence in children. International Universities Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piaget, J. (1971) Biology and knowledge. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Rozin, P. (1976) The evolution of intelligence and access to the cognitive unconscious. In: Progress in psychobiology and physiological psychology, ed. Sprague, J. M. & Epstein, A. N., pp. 245–80. Academic Press.Google Scholar