Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-v9fdk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-11T04:06:58.973Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Intersubjectivity evolved to fit the brain, but grammar co-evolved with the brain

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 October 2008

Patricia M. Greenfield
Affiliation:
FPR-UCLA Center for Culture, Brain, and Development, and Department of Psychology, University of California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA 90095greenfield@psych.ucla.eduhttp://www.psych.ucla.edu/Faculty/faculty.php?id=59&area=4
Kristen Gillespie-Lynch
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA 90095proserpinae@ucla.edu

Abstract

We propose that some aspects of language – notably intersubjectivity – evolved to fit the brain, whereas other aspects – notably grammar – co-evolved with the brain. Cladistic analysis indicates that common basic structures of both action and grammar arose in phylogeny six million years ago and in ontogeny before age two, with a shared prefrontal neural substrate. In contrast, mirror neurons, found in both humans and monkeys, suggest that the neural basis for intersubjectivity evolved before language. Natural selection acts upon genes controlling the neural substrates of these phenotypic language functions.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2008

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Buxhoeveden, D. P., Switala, A. E., Litaker, M., Roy, E. & Casanova, M. F. (2001) Lateralization of minicolumns in human planum temporale is absent in nonhuman primate cortex. Brain Behavior and Evolution 57:349–58.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cantalupo, C. & Hopkins, W. D. (2001) Asymmetric Broca's area in great apes. Nature 414:505.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Catani, M. C., Jones, D. K. & Affytche, D. H. (2005) Perisylvian language networks of the human brain. Annals of Neurology 57:816.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenfield, P. M. (1978) Structural parallels between language and action in development. In: Action, gesture and symbol: The emergence of language, ed. Lock, A.. Academic Press.Google Scholar
Greenfield, P. M. (1991) Language, tools, and the brain: The ontogeny and phylogeny of hierarchically organized sequential behavior. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14(4):531–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greenfield, P. M. & Lyn, H. (2006) Symbol combination in Pan: Language, action, culture. In: Primate perspectives on behavior and cognition, ed. Washburn, D., pp. 255–67. American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Greenfield, P. M., Lyn, H. & Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S. (2008) Protolanguage in ontogeny and phylogeny: Combining deixis and representation. Interaction Studies 9:3450.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greenfield, P. M., Nelson, K. & Saltzman, E. (1972) The development of rulebound strategies for manipulating seriated cups: A parallel between action and grammar. Cognitive Psychology 3:291310.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greenfield, P. M. & Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S. (1993) Comparing communicative competence in child and chimp: The pragmatics of repetition. Journal of Child Language 20:126.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenfield, P. M. & Smith, J. H. (1976) The structure of communication in early language development. Academic Press.Google Scholar
Iacoboni, M., Woods, R. P., Brass, M., Bekkering, H., Mazziotta, J. C. & Rizzolatti, G. (1999) Cortical mechanisms of human imitation. Science 286:13995–99.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keenan-Ochs, E. (1977) Making it last: Repetition in children's discourse. In: Child discourse, ed. Ervin-Tripp, S., pp. 125–38. Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Molnar-Szakacs, I., Kaplan, J., Greenfield, P. M. & Iacaboni, M. (2006) Observing complex action sequences: The role of the fronto-parietal mirror neuron system. NeuroImage 33:923–35.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rizzolatti, G. & Arbib, M. A. (1998) Language within our grasp. Trends in Neurosciences 21:188–94.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scollon, R. (1979) A real early stage: An unzippered condensation of a dissertation on child language. In: Developmental pragmatics, ed. Ochs, E. & Schieffelin, B. B., pp. 215–28. Academic Press.Google Scholar
Tannen, D. (1987) Repetition in conversation: Toward a poetics of talk. Language 63(3):574606.CrossRefGoogle Scholar