Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-hc48f Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-28T12:49:34.491Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Normative benchmarks are useful for studying individual differences in reasoning

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 October 2011

Edward J. N. Stupple
Affiliation:
Centre for Psychological Research, University of Derby, Kedleston Road, Derby, DE22 1GB, United Kingdom. e.j.n.stupple@derby.ac.ukhttp://psychology.derby.ac.uk/staff/Ed_Stupple.html
Linden J. Ball
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Lancaster University, Lancaster LA1 4YF, United Kingdom. l.ball@lancaster.ac.ukhttp://www.psych.lancs.ac.uk/people/LindenBall.html

Abstract

We applaud many aspects of Elqayam & Evans' (E&E's) call for a descriptivist research programme in studying reasoning. Nevertheless, we contend that normative benchmarks are vital for understanding individual differences in performance. We argue that the presence of normative responses to particular problems by certain individuals should inspire researchers to look for converging evidence for analytic processing that may have a normative basis.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2011

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ball, L. J., Phillips, P., Wade, C. N. & Quayle, J. D. (2006) Effects of belief and logic on syllogistic reasoning: Eye-movement evidence for selective processing models. Experimental Psychology 53:7786.Google Scholar
De Neys, W. (2006) Dual processing in reasoning: Two systems but one reasoner. Psychological Science 17(5):428–33.Google Scholar
De Neys, W., Vartanian, O. & Goel, V. (2008) Smarter than we think. Psychological Science 19(5):48389.Google Scholar
Evans, J. St. B. T. (2000) Thinking and believing. In: Mental models in reasoning, ed. Garcìa-Madruga, J., Carriedo, N. & González-Labra, M. J., pp. 4156. UNED (Universidad de Nacional de Education a Distancia).Google Scholar
Evans, J. St. B. T., Newstead, S. E. & Byrne, R. M. J. (1993) Human reasoning: The psychology of deduction. Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Goel, V. & Dolan, R. J. (2003) Explaining modulation of reasoning by belief. Cognition 87:B11B22.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lucas, E. J. & Ball, L. J. (2005) Think-aloud protocols and the selection task: Evidence for relevance effects and rationalisation processes. Thinking and Reasoning 11(1):3566.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Prowse Turner, J.A. & Thompson, V. A. (2009) The role of training, alternative models, and logical necessity in determining confidence in syllogistic reasoning. Thinking and Reasoning 15(1):69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmidt, J. & Thompson, V. (2008) “At least one” problem with “some” formal reasoning paradigms. Memory and Cognition 36(1):217–29.Google Scholar
Stanovich, K. E. & West, R. F. (2000b) Individual differences in reasoning: Implications for the rationality debate. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 23:645726.Google Scholar
Stupple, E. J. N. & Ball, L. J. (2008) Belief-logic conflict resolution in syllogistic reasoning: Inspection-time evidence for a parallel-process model, Thinking and Reasoning 14(2):168–81.Google Scholar
Stupple, E. J. N. & Waterhouse, E. F. (2009) Negations in syllogistic reasoning: Evidence for a heuristic-analytic conflict. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 62(8):1533–41.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Todd, P. M. & Gigerenzer, G. (2000) Précis of Simple heuristics that make us smart . Behavioral and Brain Sciences 23:727–41.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Torrens, D., Thompson, V. A. & Cramer, K. M. (1999) Individual differences and the belief bias effect: Mental models, logical necessity, and abstract reasoning. Thinking and Reasoning 5(1):128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar