No CrossRef data available.
Article contents
Post hoc rationalism in science
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 25 August 2011
Abstract
In advocating Bayesian Enlightenment as a solution to Bayesian Fundamentalism, Jones & Love (J&L) rule out a broader critique of rationalist approaches to cognition. However, Bayesian Fundamentalism is merely one example of the more general phenomenon of Rationalist Fundamentalism: the tendency to characterize human judgments as rational and optimal in a post hoc manner, after the empirical data are already known.
- Type
- Open Peer Commentary
- Information
- Copyright
- Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2011
References
Armor, D. A. (1999) The illusion of objectivity: Bias in the belief in freedom from bias. Doctoral dissertation, University of California, Los Angeles.Google Scholar
Bastardi, A., Uhlmann, E. L. & Ross, L. (2011) Wishful thinking: Belief, desire, and the motivated evaluation of scientific evidence. Psychological Science
22:731–32.Google Scholar
Cohen, G. L., Aronson, J. & Steele, C. M. (2000) When beliefs yield to evidence: Reducing biased evaluation by affirming the self. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin
26:1151–64.Google Scholar
Cosmides, L. & Tooby, J. (1994) Origins of domain specificity: The evolution of functional organization. In: Mapping the mind: Domain specificity in cognition and culture, ed. Hirschfeld, L. A. & Gelman, S., pp. 85–116. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Dunning, D. & Cohen, G. L. (1992) Egocentric definitions of traits and abilities in social judgment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
63:341–55.Google Scholar
Dunning, D., Leuenberger, A. & Sherman, D. A. (1995) A new look at motivated inference: Are self-serving theories of success a product of motivational forces?
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
69:58–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Epstein, S., Lipson, A., Holstein, C. & Huh, E. (1992) Irrational reactions to negative outcomes: Evidence for two conceptual systems. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
62:328–39.Google Scholar
Hamilton, V. L. (1980) Intuitive psychologist or intuitive lawyer: Alternative models of the attribution process. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
39:767–73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krueger, J. I. & Funder, D. C. (2004) Towards a balanced social psychology: Causes, consequences, and cures for the problem-seeking approach to social behavior and cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences
27:313–27.Google Scholar
Kunda, Z. (1987) Motivated inference: Self-serving generation and evaluation of causal theories. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
53:37–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lerner, J. S. & Tetlock, P. E. (1999) Accounting for the effects of accountability. Psychological Bulletin
125:255–75.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lord, C. G., Ross, L. & Lepper, M. R. (1979) Biased assimilation and attitude polarization: The effects of prior theories on subsequently considered evidence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
37:2098–109.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Norton, M. I., Vandello, J. A. & Darley, J. M. (2004) Casuistry and social category bias. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
87:817–31.Google Scholar
Pronin, E., Gilovich, T. & Ross, L. (2004) Objectivity in the eye of the beholder: Perceptions of bias in self versus others. Psychological Review
111:781–99.Google Scholar
Pronin, E., Lin, D. Y. & Ross, L. (2002) The bias blind spot: Perceptions of bias in self versus others. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin
28:369–81.Google Scholar
Pyszczynski, T. & Greenberg, J. (1987) Toward an integration of cognitive and motivational perspectives on social inference: A biased hypothesis-testing model. In: Advances in experimental social psychology, vol. 20, ed. Berkowitz, L., pp. 297–340. Academic Press.Google Scholar
Ross, L. & Ward, A. (1996) Naive realism in everyday life: Implications for social conflict and misunderstanding. In: Values and knowledge, ed. Reed, E. S. & Turiel, E., pp. 103–35. Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Sherman, D. K. & Cohen, G. L. (2002) Accepting threatening information: Self-affirmation and the reduction of defensive biases. Current Directions in Psychological Science
11:119–23.Google Scholar
Sherman, D. K. & Cohen, G. L. (2006) The psychology of self-defense: Self-affirmation theory. In: Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, vol. 38, ed. Zanna, M. P., pp. 183–242. Academic Press.Google Scholar
Steele, C. M. (1988) The psychology of self-affirmation: Sustaining the integrity of the self. In: Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, vol. 21, ed. Berkowitz, L., pp. 261–302. Academic Press.Google Scholar
Tetlock, P. E. (2002) Social functionalist frameworks for judgment and choice: Intuitive politicians, theologians, and prosecutors. Psychological Review
109:451–71.Google Scholar
Tetlock, P. E., Kristel, O., Elson, B., Green, M. & Lerner, J (2000) The psychology of the unthinkable: Taboo trade-offs, forbidden base rates, and heretical counterfactuals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
78:853–70.Google Scholar
Tetlock, P. E., Visser, P., Singh, R., Polifroni, M., Elson, B., Mazzocco, P. & Rescober, P. (2007) People as intuitive prosecutors: The impact of social control motives on attributions of responsibility. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology
43:195–209.Google Scholar
Uhlmann, E. L. & Cohen, G. L. (2005) Constructed criteria: Redefining merit to justify discrimination. Psychological Science
16:474–80.Google Scholar
Target article
Post hoc rationalism in science
Related commentaries (1)
Bayesian Fundamentalism or Enlightenment? On the explanatory status and theoretical contributions of Bayesian models of cognition