Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-8bhkd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-13T09:10:44.676Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Probability theory and perception of randomness: Bridging “ought” and “is”

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 October 2011

Yanlong Sun
Affiliation:
School of Biomedical Informatics, University of Texas Health Science Center at Houston, Houston, TX 77030. Yanlong.Sun@uth.tmc.edu
Hongbin Wang
Affiliation:
School of Biomedical Informatics, University of Texas Health Science Center at Houston, Houston, TX 77030. Yanlong.Sun@uth.tmc.edu Department of Psychology, Tsinghua University, Beijing, 100084China. Hongbin.Wang@uth.tmc.edu

Abstract

We argue that approaches adhering to normative systems can be as fruitful as those by descriptive systems. In measuring people's perception of randomness, discrepancies between human behavior and normative models could have resulted from unknown properties of the models, and it does not necessarily lead to the conclusion that people are irrational or that the normative system has to be abandoned.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2011

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ayton, P. & Fischer, I. (2004) The hot hand fallacy and the gambler's fallacy: Two faces of subjective randomness? Memory and Cognition 32(8):1369–78.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bar-Eli, M., Avugos, S. & Raab, M. (2006) Twenty years of “hot hand” research: Review and critique. Psychology of Sport and Exercise 7(6):525–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burns, B. D. (2004) Heuristics as beliefs and as behaviors: The adaptiveness of the “hot hand”. Cognitive Psychology 48:295331.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dehaene, S. & Brannon, E. M. (2010) Space, time, and number: A Kantian research program. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 14(12):517–19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Falk, R., Falk, R. & Ayton, P. (2009) Subjective patterns of randomness and choice: Some consequences of collective responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 35(1):203–24.Google ScholarPubMed
Falk, R. & Konold, C. (1997) Making sense of randomness: Implicit encoding as a basis for judgment. Psychological Review 104(2):301–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gardner, M. (1988) Time travel and other mathematical bewilderments. Freeman.Google Scholar
Gilovich, T., Vallone, R. & Tversky, A. (1985) The hot hand in basketball: On the misperception of random sequences. Cognitive Psycholgy 17:295314.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hahn, U. & Warren, P. A. (2009) Perceptions of randomness: Why three heads are better than four. Psychological Review 116(2):454–61.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hahn, U. & Warren, P. A. (2010) Why three heads are a better bet than four: A reply to Sun, Tweney, and Wang (2010) Psychological Review 117(2):706–11.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kareev, Y. (1992) Not that bad after all: generation of random sequences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 18:1189–94.Google Scholar
Liberman, N. & Trope, Y. (2008) The psychology of transcending the here and now. Science 322(5905):1201–205.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lopes, L. L. & Oden, G. C. (1987) Distinguishing between random and nonrandom events. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition 13(3):392400.Google Scholar
Luhmann, C. C., Chun, M. M., Yi, D.-J., Lee, D. & Wang, X.-J. (2008) Neural dissociation of delay and uncertainty in intertemporal choice. Journal of Neuroscience 28(53):14459–66.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Markowitz, H. M. (1991) Foundations of portfolio theory. Journal of Finance 46(2):469–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McClure, S. M., Ericson, K. M., Laibson, D. I., Loewenstein, G. & Cohen, J. D. (2007) Time discounting for primary rewards. Journal of Neuroscience 27(21):5796–804.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nickerson, R. S. (2002) The production and perception of randomness. Psychological Review 109(2):330–57.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nickerson, R. S. & Butler, S. F. (2009) On producing random binary sequences. American Journal of Psychology 122(2):141–51.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oppenheimer, D. M. & Monin, B. (2009) The retrospective gambler's fallacy: Unlikely events, constructing the past, and multiple universes. Judgment and Decision Making 4(5):326–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oskarsson, A. T., Van Boven, L., McClelland, G. H. & Hastie, R. (2009) What's next? Judging sequences of binary events. Psychological Bulletin 135(2):262–85.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rapoport, A. & Budescu, D. V. (1997) Randomization in individual choice behavior. Psychological Review 104(3):603–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sun, Y. & Wang, H. (2010a) Gambler's fallacy, hot hand belief, and time of patterns. Judgment and Decision Making 5(2):124–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sun, Y. & Wang, H. (2010b) Perception of randomness: On the time of streaks. Cognitive Psychology 61(4):333–42.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sun, Y., Tweney, R. D. & Wang, H. (2010a) Occurrence and nonoccurrence of random sequences: Comment on Hahn and Warren (2009) Psychological Review 117(2):697703.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sun, Y., Tweney, R. D. & Wang, H. (2010b) Postscript: Untangling the gambler's fallacy. Psychological Review 117(2):704705.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trope, Y. & Liberman, N. (2010) Construal-level theory of psychological distance. Psychological Review 117(2):440–63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tversky, A. & Kahneman, D. (1974) Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Science 185:1124–31.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weber, E. U., Shafir, S. & Blais, A.-R. (2004) Predicting risk sensitivity in humans and lower animals: Risk as variance or coefficient of variation. Psychological Review 111(2):430–45.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed