Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-g7gxr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-10T21:59:40.222Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Role of affective associations in the planning and habit systems of decision-making related to addiction

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 July 2008

Marc T. Kiviniemi
Affiliation:
Department of Health Behavior, State University of New York at Buffalo, Buffalo, NY 14214; mtk8@buffalo.eduhttp://sphhp.buffalo.edu/hb/faculty/kiviniemi_marc.php
Rick A. Bevins
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Nebraska–Lincoln, Lincoln, NE 68588-0308. rbevins1@unl.eduhttp://www.unl.edu/psychoneuropharm/

Abstract

The model proposed by Redish et al. considers vulnerabilities within decision systems based on expectancy-value assumptions. Further understanding of processes leading to addiction can be gained by considering other inputs to decision-making, particularly affective associations with behaviors. This consideration suggests additional decision-making vulnerabilities that might explain addictive behaviors.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2008

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bevins, R. A. (in press) Altering the motivational function of nicotine through conditioning processes. In: The motivational impact of nicotine and its role in tobacco use: The 55th Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, ed. Bevins, R. A. & Caggiula, A. R.. Springer.Google Scholar
Bevins, R. A. & Palmatier, M. I. (2004) Extending the role of associative learning processes in nicotine addiction. Behavioral and Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews 3:143–58.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Damasio, A. R. (1994) Descartes' error: Emotion, reason and the human brain. Putnam.Google Scholar
Darke, P. R., Chattopadhyay, A. & Ashworth, L. (2006) The importance and functional significance of affective cues in consumer choice. Journal of Consumer Research 33:322–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gilbert, D. G., Sharpe, J. P., Ramanaiah, N. V., Detwiler, F. R. & Anderson, A. E. (2000) Development of a situation x trait response (STAR) model-based smoking motivation questionnaire. Personality and Individual Differences 29:6584.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greene, J. D., Sommerville, R. B., Nystrom, L. E., Darley, J. M. & Cohen, J. D. (2001) An fMRI investigation of emotional engagement in moral judgment. Science 293:21052108.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoffman, W. F., Moore, M., Templin, R., McFarland, B., Hitzemann, R. J. & Mitchell, S. H. (2006) Neuropsychological function and delay discounting in methamphetamine-dependent individuals. Psychopharmacology 188:162–70.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, S. J., Lyoo, I. K., Hwang, J., Sung, Y. H., Lee, H. Y., Lee, D. S., Jeong, D. U. & Renshaw, P. F. (2005) Frontal glucose hypometabolism in abstinent methamphetamine users. Neuropsychopharmacology 30:1383–91.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kiviniemi, M. T. & Bevins, R. A. (2007) Affect-behavior associations in motivated behavioral choice: Potential transdisciplinary links. In: Issues in the psychology of motivation, ed. Zelick, P. R.. Nova.Google Scholar
Kiviniemi, M. T., Voss-Humke, A. M. & Seifert, A. L. (2007) How do I feel about the behavior? The interplay of affective associations with behaviors and cognitive beliefs as influences on physical activity behavior. Health Psychology 26:152–58.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koob, G. F. & Le Moal, M. (2006) Neurobiology of addiction. Elsevier Academic.Google ScholarPubMed
Molina, J. C., Bannoura, M. D., Chotro, M. G., McKinzie, D. L., Arnold, H. M. & Spear, N. E. (1996) Alcohol-mediated tactile conditioned aversions in infant rats: Devaluation of conditioning through alcohol-sucrose associations. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 66:121–32.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Revusky, S., Taukulis, H. K. & Coombes, S. (1980) Dependence of the Avfail effect on the sequence of training operations. Behavioral and Neural Biology 29:430–45.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Richard, R., van der Pligt, J. & de Vries, N. (1996) Anticipated affect and behavioral choice. Basic and Applied Social Psychology 18:111–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Samson, H. H., Cunningham, C. L., Czachowski, C. L., Chappell, A., Legg, B. & Shannon, E. (2004) Devaluation of ethanol reinforcement. Alcohol 32:203–12.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shiffman, S., Paty, J. A., Gnys, M., Kassel, J. A. & Hickcox, M. (1996) First lapses to smoking: Within-subjects analysis of real-time reports. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 64:66379.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simons, J. & Carey, K. B. (1998) A structural analysis of attitudes toward alcohol and marijuana use. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 24:727–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simons, R. L., Simons, L. G., Burt, C. H., Drummund, H., Stewart, E., Brody, G. H., Gibbons, F. X. & Cutrona, C. (2006) Supportive parenting moderates the effect of discrimination upon anger, hostile view of relationships, and violence among African American boys. Journal of Health and Social Behavior 47:373–89.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Terrell, F., Miller, A. R., Foster, K. & Watkins, C. E. Jr. (2006) Racial discrimination induced anger and alcohol use among black adolescents. Adolescence 40:485–92.Google Scholar
Trafimow, D. & Sheeran, P. (1998) Some tests of the distinction between cognitive and affective beliefs. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 34:378–97.CrossRefGoogle Scholar