Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-dzt6s Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-28T14:30:26.115Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Undermining the foundations: Questioning the basic notions of associationism and mental representation

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  23 April 2009

Ezequiel Morsella
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, San Francisco State University, San Francisco, CA 94132-4168morsella@sfsu.eduhttp://bss.sfsu.edu/emorsella/ Department of Neurology; University of California, San Francisco, San Francisco, CA 94143triddle@sfsu.edu
Travis A. Riddle
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, San Francisco State University, San Francisco, CA 94132-4168morsella@sfsu.eduhttp://bss.sfsu.edu/emorsella/
John A. Bargh
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Yale University, New Haven, CT 06520-8205. john.bargh@yale.eduhttp://pantheon.yale.edu/~jab257/home.html

Abstract

Perhaps the time has come to re-examine the basic notions of cognitive science. Together with previous challenges against associationism, the target article should be viewed as a call to arms to re-evaluate the empirical basis for contemporary conceptualizations of human learning and the notion of “mental representation,” a concept that has become too imprecise for describing the elements of cognition.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2009

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Buzsáki, G. (2006) Rhythms of the brain. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Waal, F. B. M. (2002) Evolutionary psychology: The wheat and the chaff. Current Directions in Psychological Science 11:187–91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fries, P. (2005) A mechanism for cognitive dynamics: Neuronal communication through neuronal coherence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 9:474–80.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gallistel, C. R. (1990) The organization of learning. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Gallistel, C. R. (2001) Mental representations, psychology of. In: International encyclopedia of the social and behavioural sciences, ed. Smelser, N. J. & Baltes, P. B., pp. 9691–95. Elsevier.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gallistel, C. R. & Gibbon, J. (2001) Computational versus associative models of simple conditioning. Current Directions in Psychological Science 10:146–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gray, J. A. (2004) Consciousness: Creeping up on the hard problem. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hallett, M. (2007) Volitional control of movement: The physiology of free will. Clinical Neurophysiology 117:1179–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G. & Prinz, W. (2001) The theory of event coding (TEC): A framework for perception and action planning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24(5):849937.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hummel, F. & Gerloff, C. (2005) Larger interregional synchrony is associated with greater behavioral success in a complex sensory integration task in humans. Cerebral Cortex 15:670–78.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kandel, E. R. (2000) Learning and memory. In: Principles of neural science, 4th edition, ed. Kandel, E. R., Schwartz, J. H. & Jessell, T. M., pp. 1227–46. McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Lashley, K. S. (1951) The problem of serial order in behavior. In: Cerebral mechanisms in behavior. The Hixon symposium, ed. Jeffress, L. A., pp. 112–46. Wiley.Google Scholar
Le Doux, J. E. (2000) Emotion circuits in the brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience 23:155–84.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
LeDoux, J. E. (2000) Emotion circuits in the brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience 23:155–84.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morsella, E. (2005) The function of phenomenal states: Supramodular interaction theory. Psychological Review 112:1000–21.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pessiglione, M., Petrovic, P., Daunizeau, J., Palminteri, S., Dolan, R. J. & Frith, C. D. (2008) Subliminal instrumental conditioning demonstrated in the human brain. Neuron 59:561–67.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Phelps, E. A. & LeDoux, J. E. (2005) Contributions of the amygdala to emotion processing: From animal models to human behavior. Neuron 48:175–87.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shallice, T. (1972) Dual functions of consciousness. Psychological Review 79:383–93.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Skinner, B. F. (1953) Science and human behavior. Macmillan.Google Scholar
Sperry, R. W. (1964) Neurology and the mind-brain problem. In: Basic readings in neuropsychology, ed. Isaacson, R. L., pp. 403–29. Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Stadler, M. A. & Frensch, P. A., eds. (1998) Handbook of implicit learning. Sage.Google Scholar
Terrace, H. S. (2005) The simultaneous chain: A new approach to serial learning. Trends in Cognitive Science 9:202–10.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thompson, R. F. (2005) In search of memory traces. Annual Review of Psychology 56:123.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tolman, E. C. (1948) Cognitive maps in rats and men. Psychological Review 55:189208.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed