Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-lnqnp Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-25T17:07:26.259Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The weirdest people in the world?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  15 June 2010

Joseph Henrich
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology and Department of Economics, University of British Columbia, Vancouver V6T 1Z4, Canada. joseph.henrich@gmail.comhttp://www.psych.ubc.ca/~henrich/home.html
Steven J. Heine
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver V6T 1Z4, Canada. heine@psych.ubc.ca
Ara Norenzayan
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver V6T 1Z4, Canada. ara@psych.ubc.ca

Abstract

Behavioral scientists routinely publish broad claims about human psychology and behavior in the world's top journals based on samples drawn entirely from Western, Educated, Industrialized, Rich, and Democratic (WEIRD) societies. Researchers – often implicitly – assume that either there is little variation across human populations, or that these “standard subjects” are as representative of the species as any other population. Are these assumptions justified? Here, our review of the comparative database from across the behavioral sciences suggests both that there is substantial variability in experimental results across populations and that WEIRD subjects are particularly unusual compared with the rest of the species – frequent outliers. The domains reviewed include visual perception, fairness, cooperation, spatial reasoning, categorization and inferential induction, moral reasoning, reasoning styles, self-concepts and related motivations, and the heritability of IQ. The findings suggest that members of WEIRD societies, including young children, are among the least representative populations one could find for generalizing about humans. Many of these findings involve domains that are associated with fundamental aspects of psychology, motivation, and behavior – hence, there are no obvious a priori grounds for claiming that a particular behavioral phenomenon is universal based on sampling from a single subpopulation. Overall, these empirical patterns suggests that we need to be less cavalier in addressing questions of human nature on the basis of data drawn from this particularly thin, and rather unusual, slice of humanity. We close by proposing ways to structurally re-organize the behavioral sciences to best tackle these challenges.

Type
Target Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2010

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adams, G. (2005) The cultural grounding of personal relationships: Enemyship on West African worlds. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 88:948–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Allik, J. & McCrae, R. R. (2004) Toward a geography of personality traits: Patterns of profiles across 36 cultures. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 35:1328.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Al-Shehab, A. J. (2002) A cross-sectional examination of levels of moral reasoning in a sample of Kuwait University faculty members. Social Behavior and Personality 30:813–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, S. L., Adams, G. & Plaut, V. C. (2008) The cultural grounding of personal relationship: The importance of attractiveness in everyday life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 95:352–68.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arnett, J. (2008) The neglected 95%: Why American psychology needs to become less American. American Psychologist 63(7):602–14.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Asch, S. E. (1951) Effects of group pressure upon the modification and distortion of judgments. In: Groups, leadership and men, ed. Guetzkow, H., pp. 177–90. Carnegie.Google Scholar
Asch, S. E. (1952) Group forces in the modification and distortion of judgments. In: Social Psychology, pp. 450501. Prentice Hall.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Astuti, R. (2001) Are we all natural dualists? A cognitive developmental approach. Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute 7(3):429–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Astuti, R., Solomon, G. E. A. & Carey, S. (2004) Constraints on conceptual development. A case study of the acquisition of folkbiological and folksociological knowledge in Madagascar. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 69, Serial No. 277; 69(3):vii135.Google ScholarPubMed
Atran, S. (1993) Ethnobiological classification: Principles of categorization of plants and animals in traditional societies – Berlin, B. Current Anthropology 34(2):195–98.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Atran, S. (2005) Adaptationism for human cognition: Strong, spurious or weak? Mind and Language 20(1):3967.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Atran, S., Medin, D. L., Lynch, E., Vapnarsky, V., Ucan, E. E. & Sousa, P. (2001) Folkbiology doesn't come from folkpsychology: Evidence from Yukatek Maya in cross-cultural perspective. Journal of Cognition and Culture 1(1):342.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Atran, S., Medin, D. L. & Ross, N. (2005) The cultural mind: Environmental decision making and cultural modeling within and across populations. Psychological Review 112(4):744–76.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baek, H. (2002) A comparative study of moral development of Korean and British children. Journal of Moral Education 31:373–91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bahry, D. L. & Wilson, R. K. (2006) Confusion or fairness in the field? Rejection in the Ultimatum Game under the strategy method. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization 60(1):3754.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bandura, A. (1982) Self-efficacy mechanism in human agency. American Psychologist 37:122–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bang, M., Medin, D. L. & Atran, S. (2007) Cultural mosaics and mental models of nature. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 104:13868–74.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barrett, H. C. (2006) Modularity and design reincarnation. In: The innate mind: Culture and cognition, ed. Carruthers, P., Laurence, S. & Stich, S., pp. 199217. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Barrett, H. C. & Behne, T. (2005) Children's understanding of death as the cessation of agency: A test using sleep versus death. Cognition 96(2):93108.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Becker, E. (1973) The denial of death. Free Press.Google Scholar
Beja-Pereira, A., Luikart, G., England, P. R., Bradley, D. G., Jann, O. C., Bertorelle, G., Chamberlain, A. T., Nunes, T. P., Metodiev, S., Ferrand, N. & Erhardt, G. (2003) Gene-culture coevolution between cattle milk protein genes and human lactase genes. Nature Genetics 35(4):311–13.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bellemare, C. & Kröger, S. (2007) On representative social capital. European Economic Review 51:183202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bellemare, C., Kröger, S. & Van Soest, A., (2008) Measuring inequity aversion in a heterogeneous population using experimental decisions and subjective probabilities. Econometrica 76(4):815–39.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benet-Martinez, V. & Waller, N. G. (1995) The Big Seven factor model of personality description: Evidence for its cross-cultural generality in a Spanish sample. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 69:701–18.Google Scholar
Berlin, B. (1992) Ethnobiological classification: Principles of categorization of plants and animals in traditional societies. Princeton University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berry, J. W. (1966) Emne and Eskimo perceptual skills. International Journal of Psychology 1:207–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Billing, J. & Sherman, P. W. (1998) Antimicrobial functions of spices: Why some like it hot. Quarterly Review of Biology 73(1):349.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boesch, C. (2007) What makes us human (Homo sapiens)? The challenge of cognitive cross-species comparison. Journal of Comparative Psychology 121(3):227–40.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bond, R. & Smith, P. B. (1996) Culture and conformity: A meta-analysis of studies using Asch's (1952b, 1956) line judgment task. Psychological Bulletin 119(1):111–37.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bouchard, T. J. (2004) Genetic influence on human psychological traits – A survey. Current Directions in Psychological Science 13(4):148–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boyd, R. & Richerson, P. J. (1985) Culture and the evolutionary process. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Brown, D. E. (1991) Human universals. McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Brown, R. (1986) Social psychology, 2nd edition. Free Press.Google Scholar
Buchtel, E. & Norenzayan, A. (2008) Which should you use, intuition or logic? Cultural differences in injunctive norms about reasoning. Asian Journal of Social Psychology 11:264–73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burke, B., Martens, A. & Faucher, E. (2010) Two decades of terror management theory: A meta-analysis of mortality salience research. Personality and Social Psychological Review 14:155–95.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burton, R. & Whiting, J. (1961) The absent father and cross-sex identity. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly 7( 85–95):85.Google Scholar
Buss, D. M. (1989) Sex-differences in human mate preferences: Evolutionary hypothesis tested in 37 cultures. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12(1):114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Callaghan, T., Rochat, P., Lillard, A., Claux, M. L., Odden, H., Itakura, S., Tapanya, S. & Singh, S. (2005) Synchrony in the onset of mental-state reasoning – Evidence from five cultures. Psychological Science 16(5):378–84.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Camerer, C. (2003) Behavior game theory: Experiments in strategic interaction. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Cardenas, J. C. & Carpenter, J. (2008) Behavioural development economics: Lessons from field labs in the developing world. Journal of Development Studies 44(3):311–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carey, S. (1985) Conceptual change in childhood. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Carey, S. (1995) On the origins of causal understanding. In: Causal cognition, ed. Sperber, D., Premack, D. & Premack, A., pp. 268302. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Carpenter, J., Burks, S. & Verhoogen, E. (2005) Comparing students to workers: The effects of social framing on behavior in distribution games. In: Field experiments in economics, ed. Carpenter, J., Harrison, G. W. & List, J. A., pp. 261–90. JAI Press/Elsevier.Google Scholar
Carpenter, J., Connolly, C. & Myers, C. K. (2008) Altruistic behavior in a representative dictator experiment. Experimental Economics 11(3):282–98.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carter, J. R. & Irons, M. D. (1991) Are economists different, and if so, why? Journal of Economic Perspectives 5(Spring):171–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carver, C. S. (2004) Editorial. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 86:95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cavalli-Sforza, L. L., Menozzi, P. & Piazza, A. (1994) The history and geography of human genes. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Chen, C., Lee, S.-Y. & Stevenson, H. W. (1995) Response style and cross-cultural comparisons of rating scales amongst East Asian and North American students. Psychological Science 6:170–75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cheung, F. M., Leung, K., Fan, R. M., Song, W., Zhang, J. X. & Zhang, J. P. (1996) Development of the Chinese Personality Assessment Inventory. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 27:181–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Choi, I. & Nisbett, R. E. (1998) Situational salience and cultural differences in correspondence bias and the actor-observer bias. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 24:949–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Choi, I., Nisbett, R. E. & Norenzayan, A. (1999) Causal attribution across cultures: Variation and universality. Psychological Bulletin 125(1):4761.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chua, H. F., Boland, J. E. & Nisbett, R. E. (2005) Cultural variation in eye movements during scene perception. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 102:12629–33.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Church, A. T., Katigbak, M. S., Del Prado, A. M., Ortiz, F. A., Mastor, K. A., Harumi, Y., Tanaka-Matsumi, J., De Jesús Vargas-Flores, J., Ibáñez-Reyes, J., White, F. A., Miramontes, L. G., Reyes, J. A. S. & Cabrera, H. F. (2006) Implicit theories and self-perceptions of traitedness across cultures: Toward integration of cultural and trait psychology perspectives. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 37(6):694716.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, D., Hoshino-Browne, E. & Leung, A. (2007) Culture and the structure of personal experience: Insider and outsider phenomenologies of the self and social world. In: Advances in experimental social psychology, vol. 39, ed. Zanna, M. P., pp. 167. Academic Press.Google Scholar
Cross, P. (1977) Not can but will college teaching be improved. New Directions for Higher Education 17:115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Daly, M. & Wilson, M. (1988) Homicide. Aldine de Gruyter.Google ScholarPubMed
D'Andrade, R. G. (1995) The development of cognitive anthropology. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dasen, P. R., Mishra, R. C., Niraula, S. & Wassmann, J. (2006) Développement du langage et de la cognition spatiale géocentrique. Enfance 58:146–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dehaene, S. (1997) The number sense: How the mind creates mathematics. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
de Tocqueville, A., (1835) Democracy in America, trans. Lawrence, G.. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Diener, E., Diener, M. & Diener, C. (1995) Factors predicting the subjective well-being of nations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 69:815–64.Google ScholarPubMed
Eagly, A. H. & Wood, W. (1999) The origins of sex differences in human behavior: Evolved dispositions versus social roles. American Psychologist 54:408423.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Earley, P. C. (1993) East meets West meets Mideast: Further explorations of collectivistic and individualistic work groups. Academy of Management Journal 36:319–48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Egas, M. & Riedl, A. (2008) The economics of altruistic punishment and the maintenance of cooperation. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B – Biological Sciences 275(1637):871–78.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ekman, P. (1999a) Basic emotions. In: The handbook of cognition and emotion, ed. Dalgleish, T. & Power, T., pp. 4560. Wiley.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ekman, P. (1999b) Facial expressions. In: The handbook of cognition and emotion, ed. Dalgleish, T. & Power, T., pp. 301–20. Wiley.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elfenbein, H. A. & Ambady, N. (2002) On the universality and cultural specificity of emotion recognition: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin 128:203–35.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elliot, I. T., Chirkov, V. I., Kim, Y. & Sheldon, K. M. (2001) A cross-cultural analysis of avoidance (relative to approach) personal goals. Psychological Science 12:505–10.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ensminger, J. & Cook, K. (under review) Pro-social norms among rural and urban small communities in America: Evidence from dictator, ultimatum, and public goods games. In: Experimenting with social norms: Fairness and punishment in cross-cultural perspective, ed. Ensminger, J. & Henrich, J.. Russell Sage Foundation Press.Google Scholar
Falk, C. F., Heine, S. J., Yuki, M. & Takemura, K. (2009) Why do Westerners self-enhance more than East Asians? European Journal of Personality 23(3):183203.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Farley, J. U., Lehmann, D. R. & Ryan, M. J. (1981) Generalizing from “imperfect” replication. Journal of Business 54:597610.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fehr, E., Fischbacher, U., von Rosenbladt, B., Schupp, J., & Wagner, G. G. (2002) A nation-wide laboratory: Examining trust and trustworthiness by integrating behavioral experiments into representative surveys. Schmoller Fahrbuch 122:519–42.Google Scholar
Fehr, E. & Gächter, S. (1998) Reciprocity and economic: The economic implications of Homo reciprocans. European Economic Review 42(3–5):845–59.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fehr, E. & Gächter, S. (2002) Altruistic punishment in humans. Nature 415:137–40.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fehr, E. & List, J. A. (2004) The hidden costs and returns of incentives: Trust and trustworthiness among CEOs. Journal of the European Economic Association 2(5):743–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fessler, D. M. T. (1999) Toward an understanding of the universality of second order emotions. In: Beyond nature or nurture: Biocultural approaches to the emotions, ed. Hinton, A., pp. 75116. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Fessler, D. M. T. (2004) Shame in two cultures: Implications for evolutionary approaches. Journal of Cognition and Culture 4(2):207–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fessler, D. M. T., Nettle, D., Afshar, Y., Pinheiro, I. D., Bolyanatz, A., Mulder, M. B., Cravalho, M., Delgado, T., Gruzd, B., Correia, M. O., Khaltourina, D., Korotayev, A., Marrow, J., de Souza, L. S., & Zbarauskaite, A. (2005) A cross-cultural investigation of the role of foot size in physical attractiveness. Archives of Sexual Behavior 34(3):267–76.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fiske, A. P. (1993) Social errors in 4 cultures: Evidence about universal forms of social-relations. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 24(4):463–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fiske, A. P. (1998) Learning a culture the way informants do: Observing, imitating, and participating. Unpublished manuscript, University of California-Los Angeles. Available at: http://www.bec.ucla.edu/papers/learning_culture.htm.Google Scholar
Flynn, J. R. (1987) Massive IQ gains in 14 nations: What IQ tests really measure. Psychological Bulletin 101:171–91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flynn, J. R. (2007) What is intelligence? Beyond the Flynn effect. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fodor, J. A. (1983) The modularity of mind: An essay on faculty psychology. MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fryberg, S. A. & Markus, H. R. (2003) On being American Indian: Current and possible selves. Self and Identity 2:325–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gachter, S., Renner, E. & Sefton, M. (2008) The long-run benefits of punishment. Science 322(5907):1510.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gangestad, S. W., Haselton, M. G. & Buss, D. M. (2006) Evolutionary foundations of cultural variation: Evoked culture and mate preference. Psychological Inquiry 17:7595.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Geertz, C. (1974) “From the native's point of view”: On the nature of anthropological understanding. Bulletin of the American Academy of Arts and Sciences 28:2645.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Geertz, C. (1975) On the nature of anthropological understanding. American Scientist 63:453.Google Scholar
Gelman, S. A. (2003) The essential child: Origins of essentialism in everyday thought. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gergen, K. J. (1973) Social psychology as history. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 26:309–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gilbert, D. T. & Malone, P. S. (1995) The correspondence bias. Psychological Bulletin 117:2138.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gilovich, T., Griffin, D. & Kahneman, D., eds. (2002) Heuristics and biases: The psychology of intuitive judgment. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gil-White, F. (2001) Are ethnic groups biological “species” to the human brain? Essentialism in our cognition of some social categories. Current Anthropology 42(4):515–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Godoy, R., Byron, E., Reyes-Garcia, V., Leonard, W. R., Patel, K., Apaza, L., Perez, E., Vadez, V. & Wilkie, D. (2004) Patience in a foraging-horticultural society: A test of competing hypotheses. Journal of Anthropological Research 60(2):179202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gordon, P. (2004) Numerical cognition without words: Evidence from Amazonia. Science 306(5695):496–99.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenberg, J., Solomon, S. & Pyszczynski, T. (1997) Terror management theory of self-esteem and cultural worldviews: Empirical assessments and conceptual refinements. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology 29:61139.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grossmann, I. (2010) Categorization differences between Americans and Russians. Unpublished manuscript. University of Michigan.Google Scholar
Grossmann, I. & Varnum, M. E. W. (2010) Social class, culture, and cognition. Manuscript under review. University of Michigan.Google Scholar
Gurven, M. (2004) Does market exposure affect economic game behavior? The Ultimatum Game and the public goods game among the Tsimane of Bolivia. In: Foundations of human sociality: Economic experiments and ethnographic evidence from fifteen small-scale societies, ed. Henrich, J., Boyd, R., Bowles, S., Camerer, C., Fehr, E. & Gintis, H., pp. 194231. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gutchess, A. H., Welsh, R. C., Boduroglu, A. & Park, D. C. (2006) Cultural differences in neural function associated with object processing. Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience 6:102109.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guth, W., Schmidt, C. & Sutter, M. (2003) Fairness in the mail and opportunism in the Internet: A newspaper experiment on ultimatum bargaining. Economic Review 4(2):456–75.Google Scholar
Haidt, J. & Graham, J. (2007) When morality opposes justice: Conservatives have moral intuitions that liberals may not recognize. Social Justice Research 20:98116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haidt, J., Koller, S. H. & Dias, M. G. (1993) Affect, culture, and morality, or is it wrong to eat your dog. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 65(4):613–28.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hamamura, T., Heine, S. J. & Paulhus, D. L. (2008) Cultural differences in response styles: The role of dialectical thinking. Personality and Individual Differences 44:932–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harrington, L. & Liu, J. H. (2002) Self-enhancement and attitudes toward high achievers: A bicultural view of the independent and interdependent self. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 33:3755.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harrison, G. W., Lau, M. I. & Williams, M. B. (2002) Estimating individual discount rates in Denmark: A field experiment. American Economic Review 92(5):1606–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haun, D. B. M., Call, J., Janzen, G. & Levinson, S. C. (2006) Evolutionary psychology of spatial representations in the hominidae. Current Biology 16(17):1736–40.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haun, D. B. M., Rapold, C. J., Call, J., Janzen, G. & Levinson, S. C. (2006) Cognitive cladistics and cultural override in hominid spatial cognition. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 103(46):17568–73.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hedden, T., Ketay, S., Aron, A., Markus, H. R. & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2008) Cultural influences on neural substrates of attentional control. Psychological Science 19(1):1217.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heine, S. J. (2008) Cultural psychology. W. W. Norton.Google Scholar
Heine, S. J. & Hamamura, T. (2007) In search of East Asian self-enhancement. Personality and Social Psychology Review 11:124.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heine, S. J., Lehman, D. R., Peng, K. & Greenholtz, J. (2002) What's wrong with cross-cultural comparisons of subjective Likert scales? The reference-group problem. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 82:903–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heine, S. J. & Raineri, A. (2009) Self-improving motivations and culture: The case of Chileans. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 40:158–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hennig-Schmidt, H., Li, Z.-Y. & Yang, C. (2008) Why people reject advantageous offers: Non-monotone strategies in ultimatum bargaining: First results from a video experiment in the People's Republic of China. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization 65:373–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henrich, J. (2008) A cultural species. In: Explaining culture scientifically, ed. Brown, M., pp. 184210. University of Washington Press.Google Scholar
Henrich, J., Boyd, R., Bowles, S., Camerer, C. F., Fehr, E., Gintis, H., McElreath, R., Alvard, M., Barr, A., Ensminger, J., Henrich, N. S., Hill, K., Gil-White, F., Gurven, M., Marlowe, F. W., Patton, J. Q. & Tracer, D. (2005a) “Economic man” in cross-cultural perspective: Behavioral experiments in 15 small-scale societies. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 28(6):795–815; discussion 815–55.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henrich, J., Ensminger, J., McElreath, R., Barr, A., Barrett, C., Bolyanatz, A., Cardenas, J. C., Gurven, M., Gwako, E., Henrich, N., Lesorogol, C., Marlowe, F., Tracer, D. P. & Ziker, J. (2010) Market, religion, community size and the evolution of fairness and punishment. Science 327(5972):1480–84.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henrich, J. & Henrich, N. (under review) Fairness without punishment: Behavioral experiments in the Yasawa Island, Fiji. In: Experimenting with social norms: Fairness and punishment in cross-cultural perspective, ed. Henrich, J. & Ensminger, J.. Russell Sage Foundation Press.Google Scholar
Henrich, J. & McElreath, R. (2002) Are peasants risk-averse decision makers? Current Anthropology 43(1):172–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henrich, J., McElreath, R., Ensminger, J., Barr, A., Barrett, C., Bolyanatz, A., Cardenas, J. C., Gurven, M., Gwako, E., Henrich, N., Lesorogol, C., Marlowe, F., Tracer, D. & Ziker, J. (2006) Costly punishment across human societies. Science 312(5868):1767–70.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henrich, J. & Smith, N. (2004) Comparative experimental evidence from Machiguenga, Mapuche, and American populations. In: Foundations of human sociality: Economic experiments and ethnographic evidence from fifteen small-scale societies, ed. Henrich, J., Boyd, R., Bowles, S., Gintis, H., Fehr, E. & Camerer, C., pp. 123–67. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henrich, N. & Henrich, J. (2007) Why humans cooperate: A cultural and evolutionary explanation. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henry, P. J. (2009) College sophomores in the laboratory redux: Influences of a narrow database on social psychology's view of the nature of prejudice. Psychological Inquiry 19:4971.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Herdt, G. H., eds. (1984/1993) Ritualized homosexuality in Melanesia. University of California Press.Google Scholar
Herrmann, B., Thoni, C. & Gächter, S. (2008) Antisocial punishment across societies. Science 319(5868):1362–67.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoffman, E., McCabe, K. & Smith, V. (1998) Behavioral foundations of reciprocity: Experimental economics and evolutionary psychology. Economic Inquiry 36:335–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hofstede, G. (1980) Culture's consequences: International differences in work-related values. Sage.Google Scholar
Hofstede, G. (2001) Culture's consequences: Comparing values, behaviors, institutions, and organizations across nations. Sage.Google Scholar
Hsu, M., Krajbich, I., Zhao, C. & Camerer, C. F. (2009) Neural response to reward anticipation under risk is nonlinear in probabilities. Journal of Neuroscience 29(7):2231–37.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Humphrey, S. J. & Verschoor, A. (2004a) Decision-making under risk among small farmers in East Uganda. Journal of African Economies 13(1):44101.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Humphrey, S. J. & Verschoor, A. (2004b) The probability weighting function: Experimental evidence from Uganda, India and Ethiopia. Economics Letters 84(3):419–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hyde, J. S. (1981) How large are cognitive gender differences? American Psychologist 36:892901.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Inglehart, R., Basanez, M. & Moreno, A. (1998) Human values and beliefs: A cross-cultural sourcebook. University of Michigan Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Iyengar, S. S. & DeVoe, S. E. (2003) Rethinking the value of choice: Considering cultural mediators of intrinsic motivation. In: Nebraska symposium on motivation: vol. 49. Cross-cultural differences in perspectives on the self, ed. Murphy-Berman, V. & Berman, J. J.. pp. 129–74. University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Iyengar, S. S. & Lepper, M. R. (1999) Rethinking the value of choice: A cultural perspective on intrinsic motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 76:349–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jensen, K., Call, J. & Tomasello, M. (2007) Chimpanzees are rational maximizers in an Ultimatum Game. Science 318(5847):107109.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jensen, L. A. (1997) Culture wars: American moral divisions across the adult lifespan. Journal of Adult Development 4:107–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ji, L., Nisbett, R. E. & Zhang, Z. (2004) Is it culture or is it language? Examination of language effects in cross-cultural research on categorization. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 87:5765.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ji, L., Peng, K. & Nisbett, R. E. (2000) Culture, control and perception of relationships in the environment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 78:943–55.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jones, E. E. & Harris, V. A. (1967) The attribution of attitudes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 3:124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kahneman, D., Slovic, P. & Tversky, A. (1982) Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kahneman, D. & Tversky, A. (2000) Choices, values, and frames. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kanagawa, C., Cross, S. E. & Markus, H. R. (2001) “Who am I?”: The cultural psychology of the conceptual self. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 27:90103.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kanovsky, M. (2007) Essentialism and folksociology: Ethnicity again. Journal of Cognition and Culture 7:241–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kay, P. (2005) Color categories are not arbitrary. Cross-Cultural Research 39(1):3955.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kay, P. & Regier, T. (2006) Language, thought and color: Recent developments. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 10(2):5154.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kelley, R. C. (1980) Etoro social structure: A study in structural contradiction. University of Michigan Press.Google Scholar
Kenrick, D. T. & Keefe, R. C. (1992a) Age preferences in mates reflect sex-differences in reproductive strategies. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15(1):75133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kenrick, D. T. & Keefe, R. C. (1992b) Sex-differences in age preference – Universal reality or ephemeral construction. [Authors' response.] Behavioral and Brain Sciences 15(1):119–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kim, H. & Markus, H. R. (1999) Deviance or uniqueness, harmony or conformity? A cultural analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 77:785800.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kim, H. S., Sherman, D. K., Taylor, S. E., Sasaki, J. Y., Chu, T. Q., Ryu, C. & Xu, J. (in press) Culture, serotonin receptor polymorphism (5-HTR1A), and locus of attention. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience.Google Scholar
Kirby, K. N., Godoy, R., Reyes-Garcia, V., Byron, E., Apaza, L., Leonard, W., Perez, E., Vadez, V. & Wilkie, D. (2002) Correlates of delay-discount rates: Evidence from Tsimane' Amerindians of the Bolivian rain forest. Journal of Economic Psychology 23(3):291316.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kitayama, S., Duffy, S., Kawamura, T. & Larsen, J. T. (2003) Perceiving an object and its context in different cultures: A cultural look at new look. Psychological Science 14:201206.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kitayama, S., Park, H., Sevincer, A. T., Karasawa, M. & Uskul, A. K. (2009) A cultural task analysis of implicit independence: Comparing North America, Western Europe, and East Asia. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 97:236–55.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kleinman, A. (1988) Rethinking psychiatry: From cultural category to personal experience. Free Press.Google Scholar
Knowles, E. D., Morris, M. W., Chiu, C. & Hong, Y. (2001) Culture and the process of person perception: Evidence for automaticity among East Asians in correcting for situational influences on behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 27:1344–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kohlberg, L. (1971) From is to ought: How to commit the naturalistic fallacy and get away with it in the study of moral development. In: Cognitive development and epistemology, ed. Mischel, L., pp. 151284. Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kohlberg, L. (1976) Moral stages and moralization. In: Moral development and behavior: Theory, research and social issues, ed. Lickona, T., pp. 3153. Holt, Rinehart and Winston.Google Scholar
Kohlberg, L. (1981) The philosophy of moral development. Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Kraul, C. (2008) Environmental pollution and encroaching narco have taken their toll on Narino's state's Afro-Columbians. The community's unique culture, and altruism, is in peril. Los Angeles Times, April 14, 2008, p. 1.Google Scholar
Krosnick, J. A. & Alwin, D. F. (1989) Aging and susceptibility to attitude change. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 57:416–25.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuhn, M. H. & McPartland, T. (1954) An empirical investigation of self-attitudes. American Sociological Review 19:6876.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuhnen, U., Hannover, B., Roeder, U., Shah, A. A., Schubert, B., Upmeyer, A. & Zakaria, S. (2001) Cross-cultural variations in identifying embedded figures – Comparisons from the United States, Germany, Russia, and Malaysia. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 32(3):365–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kusserow, A. S. (1999) De-homogenizing American individualism: Socializing hard and soft individualism in Manhattan and Queens. Ethos 27:210–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lamont, M. (2000) The dignity of working men. Russell Sage Foundation.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lancy, D. F. (1996) Playing on mother ground: Cultural routines for children's development. Guilford.Google Scholar
Lancy, D. F. (2008) The anthropology of childhood: Cherubs, chattel and changelings. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Lee, A. Y., Aaker, J. L. & Gardner, W. L. (2000) The pleasures and pains of distinct self-construals: The role of interdependence in regulatory focus. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 78:1122–34.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lesorogol, C. & Ensminger, J. (under review) Double-blind dictator games in Africa and the U.S.: Differential experimenter effects. In: Experimenting with social norms, ed. Henrich, J. & Ensminger, J..Google Scholar
Levine, S. C., Vasilyeva, M., Lourenco, S. F., Newcombe, N. S. & Huttenlocher, J. (2005) Socioeconomic status modifies the sex difference in spatial skill. Psychological Science 16(11):841–45.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levinson, S. C. (2003) Space in language and cognition. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levinson, S. C., Kita, S., Haun, D. & Rasch, B. H. (2002) Returning the tables: Language affects spatial reasoning. Cognition 84:155–88.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lewis, C. C. (1995) Educating hearts and minds. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Li, P. & Gleitman, L. (2002) Turning the tables: Language and spatial reasoning. Cognition 83:265–94.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lieberman, M. D., Jarcho, J. M. & Obayashi, J. (2005) Attributional inference across cultures: Similar automatic attributions and different controlled corrections. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 31:889901.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lipset, S. M. (1996) American exceptionalism: A double-edged sword. W. W. Norton.Google Scholar
List, J. A. (2004) Neoclassical theory versus Prospect theory: Evidence from the marketplace. Econometrica 72:615–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liu, D., Wellman, H. M., Tardif, T. & Sabbagh, M. A. (2008) Theory of mind development in Chinese children: A meta-analysis of false-belief understanding across cultures and languages. Developmental Psychology 44(2):523–31.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Low, B. S. (2000) Why sex matters: A Darwinian look at human behavior. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Luria, A. R. (1976) Cognitive development: Its cultural and social foundations. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Majid, A., Bowerman, M., Kita, S., Haun, D. B. M. & Levinson, S. C. (2004) Can language restructure cognition? The case for space. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 8(3):108–14.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mann, V. A., Sasanuma, S., Sakuma, N. & Masaki, S. (1990) Sex differences in cognitive abilities: A cross-cultural perspective. Neuropsychologia 28:1063–77.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Markus, H. R. & Kitayama, S. (1991) Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review 98:224–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marlowe, F. (2004) Mate preferences among Hadza hunter-gatherers. Human Nature 15(4):365–76.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marlowe, F., Apicella, C. & Reed, D. (2005) Men's preferences for women's profile waist-to-hip ratio in two societies. Evolution and Human Behavior 26(6):458–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marlowe, F. & Wetsman, A. (2001) Preferred waist-to-hip ratio and ecology. Personality and Individual Differences 30(3):481–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Masuda, T. & Nisbett, R. E. (2001) Attending holistically versus analytically: Comparing the context sensitivity of Japanese and Americans. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 81:992–34.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
May, R. M. (1997) The scientific wealth of nations. Science 275:793–96.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCauley, R. & Henrich, J. (2006) Susceptibility to the Müller-Lyer illusion, theory-neutral observation, and the diachronic penetrability of the visual input system. Philosophical Psychology 19(1):123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCrae, R. R., Terraciano, A. & 79 members of the Personality Profiles of Culture Project (2005) Universal features of personality traits from the observer's perspective: Data from 50 cultures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 88:547–61.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Medin, D. L. & Atran, S. (2004) The native mind: Biological categorization and reasoning in development and across cultures. Psychological Review 111(4):960–83.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Medin, D. L. & Bang, M. (2008) Perspective taking, diversity and partnerships. American Psychological Association 22(2). (APA Science Directorate Online publication, February 2008.) Available at: http://www.apa.org/science/about/psa/2008/02/medin.aspx.Google Scholar
Mezulis, A. H., Abramson, L. Y., Hyde, J. S. & Hankin, B. L. (2004) Is there a universal positive bias in attributions? A meta-analytic review of individual, developmental, and cultural differences in the self-serving attributional bias. Psychological Bulletin 130:711–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, J. G. (1984) Culture and the development of everyday social explanation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 46:961–78.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, J. G. & Bersoff, D. M. (1992) Culture and moral judgment: How are conflicts between justice and interpersonal responsibilities resolved? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 62:541–54.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, J. G., Bersoff, D. M. & Harwood, R. L. (1990) Perceptions of social responsibilities in India and the United States: Moral imperatives or personal decisions? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 58:3347.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miyamoto, Y. & Kitayama, S. (2002) Cultural variation in correspondence bias: The critical role of attitude diagnosticity of socially constrained behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 83:1239–48.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mook, D. G. (1983) In defense of external invalidity. American Psychologist 38(4):379–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moore, F. R., Cassidy, C., Law Smith, M. J., & Perrett, D. I. (2006) The effects of female control of resources on sex-differentiated mate preferences. Evolution and human behavior 27(3):193205.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morling, B. & Lamoreaux, M. (2008) Measuring culture outside the head: A meta-analysis of individualism-collectivism in cultural products. Personality and Social Psychology Review 12:199–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morris, M. & Peng, K. (1994) Culture and cause: American and Chinese attributions for social and physical events. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 67:949–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Na, J., Grossmann, I., Varnum, M.E.W., Gonzalez, R., Kitayama, S. & Nisbett, R.E. (in press) When cultural differences are not reducible to individual differences. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science..Google Scholar
Neisser, U. (1963) The multiplicity of thought. British Journal of Psychology 54:114.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nisbett, R. E. (2003) The geography of thought: How Asians and Westerners think differently … and why. Free Press.Google Scholar
Nisbett, R. E. (2009) Intelligence and how to get it: Why schools and cultures count. Norton.Google Scholar
Nisbett, R. E., Peng, K., Choi, I. & Norenzayan, A. (2001) Culture and systems of thought: Holistic versus analytic cognition. Psychological Review 108:291310.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Norenzayan, A. (2006) Evolution and transmitted culture. Psychological Inquiry 17(2):123–28.Google Scholar
Norenzayan, A., Choi, I. & Nisbett, R. E. (2002a) Cultural similarities and differences in social inference: Evidence from behavioral predictions and lay theories of behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 28(1):109–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Norenzayan, A., Choi, I. & Peng, K. (2007) Cognition and perception. In: Handbook of cultural psychology, ed. Cohen, S. K. D., pp. 569–94. Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Norenzayan, A. & Heine, S. J. (2005) Psychological universals: What are they and how can we know? Psychological Bulletin 131:763–84.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Norenzayan, A., Henrich, J. & McElreath, R. (n.d.) More Chinese than the Chinese. University of British Columbia.Google Scholar
Norenzayan, A., Smith, E. E., Kim, B. J. & Nisbett, R. E. (2002b) Cultural preferences for formal versus intuitive reasoning. Cognitive Science 26(5):653–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nowak, M. A., Page, K. M. & Sigmund, K. (2000) Fairness versus reason in the Ultimatum Game. Science 289(5485):1773–75.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oyserman, D., Coon, H. M. & Kemmelmeier, M. (2002) Rethinking individualism and collectivism: Evaluation of theoretical assumptions and meta-analyses. Psychological Bulletin 128:1773–75.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oyserman, D. & Lee, S. (2008) Does culture influence what and how we think? Effects of priming individualism and collectivism. Psychological Bulletin 134(2):311–42.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pasupathi, M. (1999) Age differences in response to conformity pressure for emotional and nonemotional material. Psychology and Aging 14:170–74.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peng, K. & Nisbett, R. E. (1999) Culture, dialectics, and reasoning about contradiction. American Psychologist 54(9):741–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peng, K., Nisbett, R. E. & Wong, N. Y. C. (1997) Validity problems comparing values across cultures and possible solutions. Psychological Methods 2:329–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peterson, R. A. (2001) On the use of college students in social science research: Insights from a second-order meta-analysis. Journal of Consumer Research 28:450–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pica, P., Lerner, C., Izard, V. & Dehaene, S. (2004) Exact and approximate arithmetic in an Amazonian indigenous group. Science 306:499501.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pillsworth, E. G. (2008) Mate preferences among the Shuar of Ecuador: Trait rankings and peer evaluations. Evolution and Human Behavior 29(4):256–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Putnam, R. (2000) Bowling alone: The collapse and revival of American community. Simon and Schuster.Google Scholar
Regier, T., Kay, P. & Cook, R. S. (2005) Focal colors are universal after all. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 102(23):8386–91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reifman, A., Klein, J. G. & Murphy, S. T. (1989) Self-monitoring and age. Psychology and Aging 4:245–46.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rennie, L. J. & Dunne, M. (1994) Gender, ethnicity, and student's perceptions about science and science-related careers in Fiji. Science Education 78:285300.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Richerson, P. J. & Boyd, R. (2005) Not by genes alone: How culture transformed human evolution. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Rivers, W. H. R. (1901a) Color vision. In: Reports of the Cambridge anthropological expedition to the Torres Straits, vol. 2: Physiology and psychology, ed. Haddon, A. C., pp. 4896. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Roberson, D., Davies, I. & Davidoff, J. (2000) Color categories are not universal: Replications and new evidence from a stone-age culture. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 129(3):369–98.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rogers, C. R. (1951) Client-centered therapy. Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
Rogoff, B. (2003) The cultural nature of human development. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Ross, L. D., Amabile, T. M. & Steinmetz, J. L. (1977) Social roles, social control, and biases in social-perception processes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 35:485–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ross, N., Medin, D. L., Coley, J. D. & Atran, S. (2003) Cultural and experiential differences in the development of folkbiological induction. Cognitive Development 18(1):2547.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rozin, P. (2001) Social psychology and science: Some lessons from Solomon Asch. Personality and Social Psychology Review 5:214.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rozin, P. (2003) Five potential principles for understanding cultural differences in relation to individual differences. Journal of Research in Personality 37(4):273–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rozin, P. (2009) What kind of empirical research should we publish, fund, and reward? A different perspective. Perspectives on Psychological Science 4:435–39.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rozin, P., Fischler, C., Shields, C. & Masson, E. (2006) Attitudes towards large numbers of choices in the food domain: A cross-cultural study of five countries in Europe and the USA. Appetite 46:304308.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rozin, P. & Nemeroff, C. J. (1990) The laws of sympathetic magic: A psychological analysis of similarity and contagion. In: Cultural psychology: Essays on comparative human development, ed. Stigler, J., Herdt, G. & Shweder, R. A., pp. 205–32. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sanchez-Burks, J. (2005) Protestant relational ideology: The cognitive underpinnings and organizational implications of an American anomaly. Research in Organizational Behavior: An Annual Series of Analytical Essays and Critical Reviews 26:265305.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saucier, G., Georgiades, S., Tsaousis, I. & Goldberg, L. R. (2005) The factor structure of Greek personality adjectives. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 88:856–75.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Savani, K., Markus, H. R. & Conner, A. L. (2008) Let your preference be your guide? Preferences and choices are more tightly linked for North Americans than for Indians. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 95:861–76.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Savani, K., Markus, H. R., Naidu, N. V. R., Kumar, S. & Berlia, N. (in press) What counts as choice? Americans are more likely than Indians to construe actions as choices. Psychological Science.Google Scholar
Schaller, M. & Murray, D. R. (2008) Pathogens, personality, and culture: Disease prevalence predicts worldwide variability in sociosexuality, extraversion, and openness to experience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 95(1):212–21.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmitt, D. P. (2005) Sociosexuality from Argentina to Zimbabwe: A 48-nation study of sex, culture, and strategies of human mating. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 28(2):247311.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmitt, D. P., Allik, J., McCrae, R. R., Benet-Martinez, V., Alcaly, L., Ault, L., Austers, I., Bennett, K. L., Bianchi, G., Boholst, F., Cunen, M., Braeckman, J., Brainerd, E. G., Gerard, L., Caron, G., Casullo, M. M., Cunningham, M., Daibo, I., de Backer, C., Desouza, E., Diaz-Loving, R., Diniz, G., Durkin, K., Echegaray, M., Eremsoy, E., Euler, H. A., Falzon, R., Fisher, M. L., Foley, D., Fry, D. P., Fry, S., Ghayar, M. A., Giri, V. N., Golden, D. L., Grammer, K., Grimaldi, L., Halberstadt, L., Hague, S., Herrera, D., Hertel, J., Hoffmann, H., Hooper, D., Hradilekova, Z., Jaafar, J., Jankauskaite, M., Kabanagu-Stahel, H., Kardum, I., Khoury, N., Kwon, H., Laidra, K., Laireiter, A. R., Lakerveld, D., Lampert, A., Lauri, M., Lavallee, M., Lee, S. J., Leung, L. C., Locke, K. D., Locke, V., Luksik, I., Magaisa, I., Marcinkeviciene, D., Mata, A., Mata, R., McCarthy, B., Mills, M. E., Mikhize, N. J., Moreira, J., Moreira, S., Moya, M., Munyae, M., Noller, P., Olimar, H., Opre, A., Panayiotou, A., Petrovic, N., Poels, K., Popper, M., Poulimenou, M., P'yatokh, V., Raymond, M., Reips, U. D., Reneau, S. E., Rivera-Aragon, S., Rowatt, W. C., Ruch, W., Rus, V. S., Safir, M. P., Salas, S., Sambataro, F., Sandnabba, K. N., Schulmeyer, M. K., Schutz, A., Scrimali, T., Shackelford, T. K., Sharan, M. B., Shaver, P. R., Sichona, F., Simonetti, F., Sineshaw, T., Sookdew, R., Spelman, T., Spyron, S., Sumer, H. C., Sumer, N., Supekova, M., Szlendak, T., Taylor, R., Timmermans, B., Tooke, W., Tsaousis, I., Tungaranza, F. S. K., Van Overwalle, F., Vandermassen, G., Vanhoomissen, T., Vanwesenbeeck, I., Vasey, P. L., Verissimo, J., Voracek, M., Wan, W. W. N., Wang, T. W., Weiss, P., Wijaya, A., Woertman, L., Youn, G. & Zupaneic, A. (2007) The geographic distribution of big five personality traits: Patterns and profiles of human self-description across 56 nations. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 38:173212.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwartz, B. (2004) The paradox of choice: Why more is less. Harper-Collins.Google Scholar
Schwartz, S. H. & Bilsky, W. (1990) Toward a theory of the universal content and structure of values: Extensions and cross-cultural replications. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 58:878–91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sears, D. (1986) College sophomores in the laboratory: Influences of a narrow data base on social psychology's view of human nature. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 51:515–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Segall, M. H., Campbell, D. T. & Herskovits, M. J. (1966) The influence of culture on visual perception. Bobbs-Merrill.Google Scholar
Shariff, A. F. & Norenzayan, A. (2007) God is watching you – Priming god concepts increases prosocial behavior in an anonymous economic game. Psychological Science 18(9):803809.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sheth, J. N. (1970) Are there differences in dissonance reduction behavior among students and housewives? Journal of Marketing Research 7:243–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shweder, R. A. & Bourne, E. J. (1982) Does the concept of the person vary cross-culturally? In: Cultural conceptions of mental health and therapy, ed. Marsella, A. J. & White, G. M., pp. 97137. Kluwer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shweder, R. A., Much, N. C., Mahapatra, M. & Park, L. (1997) The “big three” of morality (autonomy, community, divinity) and the “big three” explanations of suffering. In: Morality and health, ed. Brandt, A. M. & Rozin, P., pp. 119–69. Routledge.Google Scholar
Singh, D. (2006) Universal allure of the hourglass figure: An evolutionary theory of female physical attractiveness. Clinics in Plastic Surgery 33(3):359–70.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Singh, D. & Luis, S. (1994) Ethnic and gender consensus for the effect of WHR on judgment of women's attractiveness. Human Nature 6:5165.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slingerland, E. G. (2008) What science offers the humanities: Integrating body and culture. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sloman, S. (1996) The empirical case for two systems of reasoning. Psychological Bulletin 119:2230.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snarey, J. R. (1985) The cross-cultural universality of social-moral development: A critical review of Kohlbergian research. Psychological Bulletin 97(2):202–32.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Snibbe, A. C. & Markus, H. R. (2005) You can't always get what you want: Social class, agency, and choice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 88:703–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sousa, P., Atran, S. & Medin, D. L. (2002) Essentialism and folkbiology: Evidence from Brazil. Journal of Cognition and Culture 2:195223.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stephens, N. M., Markus, H. R. & Townsend, S. S. M. (2007) Choice as an act of meaning: The case of social class. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 93:814–30.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Streeter, S. A. & McBurney, D. H. (2003) Waist-hip ratio and attractiveness: New evidence and a critique of “a critical test.” Evolution and Human Behavior 24(2):8898.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sue, S. (1999) Science, ethnicity, and bias. Where have we gone wrong? American Psychologist 54:1070–77.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sugiyama, L. S. (2004) Is beauty in the context-sensitive adaptations of the beholder? Shiwiar use of waist-to-hip ratio in assessments of female mate value. Evolution and Human Behavior 25(1):5162.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sugiyama, L. S., Tooby, J. & Cosmides, L. (2002) Cross-cultural evidence of cognitive adaptations for social exchange among the Shiwiar of Ecuadorian Amazonia. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 99(17):11537–42.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Suh, E. M. (2002) Culture, identity consistency, and subjective well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 83:1378–19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sutter, M. & Kocher, M. (2007a) Age and the development of trust and reciprocity. Games and Economic Behavior 59:364–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swami, V., Neto, F., Tovée, M. J. & Furnham, A. (2007) Preferences for female body weight and shape in three European countries. European Psychologist 12(3):220–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swami, V. & Tovée, M. J. (2007) The relative contribution of profile body shape and weight to judgements of women's physical attractiveness in Britain and Malaysia. Body Image 4(4):391–96.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sweeney, P. D., Anderson, K. & Bailey, S. (1986) Attributional style in depression: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 50:974–91.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tanaka, T., Camerer, C. & Nguyen, Q. (forthcoming) Risk and time preferences: Experimental and household survey data from Vietnam. American Economic Review.Google Scholar
Tesser, A. (1988) Toward a self-evaluation maintenance model of social behavior. In: Advances in experimental social psychology, vol. 21, ed. Berkowitz, L., pp. 181227. Academic Press.Google Scholar
Tooby, J. & Cosmides, L. (1992) The psychological foundations of culture. In: The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture, ed. Barkow, J., Cosmides, L. & Tooby, J., pp. 19136. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tracy, J. L. & Matsumoto, D. (2008) The spontaneous expression of pride and shame: Evidence for biologically innate nonverbal displays. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 105(33):11655–60.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tracy, J. L. & Robins, R. W. (2008) The nonverbal expression of pride: Evidence for cross-cultural recognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 94(3):516–30.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Triandis, H. C. (1989) The self and social-behavior in differing cultural contexts. Psychological Review 96(3):506–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Triandis, H. C. (1994) Culture and social behavior. McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Triandis, H. C., McCusker, C. & Hui, C. H. (1990) Multimethod probes of individualism and collectivism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 59:1006–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tropp, L. R. & Wright, S. C. (2003) Evaluations and perceptions of self, ingroup, and outgroup: Comparisons between Mexican-American and European-American children. Self and Identity 2:203–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tsai, J. L. (2007) Ideal affect: Cultural causes and behavioral consequences. Perspectives on Psychological Science 19:242–59.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tseng, W.-S. (2001) Handbook of cultural psychiatry. Academic Press.Google Scholar
Turkheimer, E., Haley, A., Waldron, M., D'Onofrio, B. & Gottesman, I. I. (2003) Socioeconomic status modifies heritability of IQ in young children. Psychological Science 14(6):623–28.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Twenge, J. M. & Campbell, W. K. (2001) Age and birth cohort differences in self-esteem: A cross-temporal meta-analysis. Personality and Social Psychology Review 5(4):321–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Twenge, J. M. & Im, C. (2007) Changes in the need for social approval, 1958–2001. Journal of Research in Personality 41:171–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Uskul, A. K., Kitayama, S. & Nisbett, R. E. (2008) Ecocultural basis of cognition: Farmers and fishermen are more holistic than herders. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 105(25):8552–56.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Boven, L., Kamada, A., & Gilovich, T. (1999) The perceiver as perceived: Everyday intuitions about the correspondence bias. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 77:1188–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Varnum, M., Grossmann, I., Kitayama, S. & Nisbett, R. E. (2008) The origin of cultural differences in cognition: The social orientation hypothesis. University of Michigan Press.Google Scholar
Vohs, K. D., Baumeister, R. F., Schmeichel, B. J., Twenge, J. M., Nelson, N. M. & Tice, D. M. (2008) Making choices impairs subsequent self-control: A limited-resource account of decision making, self-regulation, and active initiative. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 94:883–98.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Voyer, D., Voyer, S. & Bryden, M. P. (1995) Magnitude of sex differences in spatial abilities: A meta-analysis and consideration of critical variables. Psychological Bulletin 117:250.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wallace, B., Cesarini, D., Lichtenstein, P. & Johannesson, M. (2007) Heritability of Ultimatum Game responder behavior. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 104(40):15631–34.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Waxman, S. & Medin, D. L. (2007) Experience and cultural models matter: Placing firm limits on childhood anthropocentrism. Human Development 50(1):2330.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Waxman, S., Medin, D. L. & Ross, N. (2007) Folk-biological reasoning – from a cross-cultural developmental perspective: Early essentialist notions are shaped by cultural beliefs. Developmental Psychology 43(2):294308.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wicherts, J. M., Borsboom, D., Kats, J. & Molenaar, D. (2006) The poor availability of psychological research for reanalysis. American Psychologist 61:726–28.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wintre, M. G., North, C. & Sugar, L. A. (2001) Psychologists' response to criticisms about research based on undergraduate participants: A developmental perspective. Canadian Psychology 42:216–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Witkin, H. A. & Berry, J. W. (1975) Psychological differentiation in cross-cultural perspective. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 6(1):578.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wu, S. & Keysar, B. (2007) Cultural effects on perspective taking. Psychological Science 18:600606.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yik, M. S. M., Russell, J. A., Ahn, C., Fernandez-Dols, J. M. & Suzuki, N. (2002) Relating the five-factor model of personality to a circumplex model of affect: A five language study. In: The five-factor model of personality across cultures, ed. McCrae, R. R. & Allik, J., pp. 79104. Kluwer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yu, D. W. & Shepard, G. H. (1998) Is beauty in the eye of the beholder? Nature 396(6709):321–22.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zebian, S. & Denny, J. P. (2001) Integrative cognitive style in Middle Eastern and Western groups. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 32:5875.CrossRefGoogle Scholar