Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-4rdpn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-11T09:47:28.791Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Error-related negativity (ERN) and ‘hot’ executive function in bilingual and monolingual preschoolers

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  20 January 2020

Srishti Nayak*
Affiliation:
Princeton University, Princeton Writing Program Boston University, Department of Psychological & Brain Sciences
Amanda R. Tarullo
Affiliation:
Boston University, Department of Psychological & Brain Sciences
*
Address for correspondence: Srishti Nayak E-mail: srishtin@princeton.edu

Abstract

Early regular experience with dual-language management is thought to shape executive function (EF) circuitry during development. However, previous investigations of bilingual children's EF have largely focused on behavioral measures, or on cognitive aspects of EF. The first part of this study compared monolingual and bilingual preschoolers’ performance on more purely cognitive and more affective versions of a card-sort task, and the second part investigated Error-related negativity (ERN) event-related potential (ERP) waveforms to understand error-awareness mechanisms underlying task performance. Behavioral results showed bilingual advantages in reaction times but not accuracy, and interaction effects of language background, level of challenge, and affective/motivational salience on reaction times. Electrophysiological results revealed smaller ERN peak amplitudes in bilinguals compared to monolinguals in frontal and frontocentral midline regions. Results highlight that bilingualism may shape motivational mechanisms and neural learning mechanisms such as error-detection, such that bilinguals may be less focused on their errors.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2020

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abutalebi, J, & Green, DW (2008) Control mechanisms in bilingual language production: Neural evidence from language switching studies. Language and Cognitive Processes 23, 557582.10.1080/01690960801920602CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Adesope, OO, Lavin, T, Thompson, T, & Ungerleider, C (2010) A systematic review and meta-analysis of the cognitive correlates of bilingualism. Review of Educational Research 80, 207245.10.3102/0034654310368803CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, P (2002) Assessment and development of executive function (EF) during childhood. Child Neuropsychology 8, 7182.10.1076/chin.8.2.71.8724CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barac, R, & Bialystok, E (2012) Bilingual effects on cognitive and linguistic development: Role of language, cultural background, and education. Child Development 83, 413422.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barac, R, Moreno, S, & Bialystok, E (2016) Behavioral and electrophysiological differences in executive control between monolingual and bilingual children. Child Development 87, 12771290.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bialystok, E (1999) Cognitive complexity and attentional control in the bilingual mind. Child Development 70, 636644.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E, Barac, R, Blaye, A, & Poulin-Dubois, D (2010) Word mapping and executive functioning in young monolingual and bilingual children. Journal of Cognition and Development 11, 485508.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bialystok, E, & Feng, X (2009) Language proficiency and executive control in proactive interference: Evidence from monolingual and bilingual children and adults. Brain and Language 109, 93100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bialystok, E, & Martin, MM (2004) Attention and inhibition in bilingual children: Evidence from the dimensional change card sort task. Developmental Science 7, 325339.10.1111/j.1467-7687.2004.00351.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bialystok, E, & Viswanathan, M (2009) Components of executive control with advantages for bilingual children in two cultures. Cognition 112, 494500.10.1016/j.cognition.2009.06.014CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blair, C, & Razza, RP (2007) Relating effortful control, executive function, and false belief understanding to emerging math and literacy ability in kindergarten. Child Development 78, 647663.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bohlmann, NL, & Fenson, L (2005) The effects of feedback on perseverative errors in preschool aged children. Journal of Cognition and Development 6, 119131.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brock, LL, Rimm-Kaufman, SE, Nathanson, L, & Grimm, KJ (2009) The contributions of ‘hot’ and ‘cool’ executive function to children's academic achievement, learning-related behaviors, and engagement in kindergarten. Early Childhood Research Quarterly 24, 337349.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brooker, RJ, Buss, KA, & Dennis, TA (2011) Error-monitoring brain activity is associated with affective behaviors in young children. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience 1, 141152.10.1016/j.dcn.2010.12.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, ED, Ackerman, BP, & Moore, CA (2013) Family adversity and inhibitory control for economically disadvantaged children: preschool relations and associations with school readiness. Journal of Family Psychology 27, 443452.10.1037/a0032886CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bush, G, Luu, P, & Posner, MI (2000) Cognitive and emotional influences in anterior cingulate cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 4, 215222.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carlson, SM, & Meltzoff, AN (2008) Bilingual experience and executive functioning in young children. Developmental Science 11, 282298.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davies, PL, Segalowitz, SJ, & Gavin, WJ (2004) Development of response-monitoring ERPs in 7- to 25-year-olds. Developmental Neuropsychology 25, 355376.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diamond, A (2013) Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology 64, 135168.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dowsett, SM, & Livesey, DJ (2000) The development of inhibitory control in preschool children: Effects of ‘executive skills’ training. Developmental Psychobiology 36, 161174.3.0.CO;2-0>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duñabeitia, JA, & Carreiras, M (2015) The bilingual advantage: acta est fabula? Cortex 73(July), 910.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunn, LM, Dunn, DM, & Assessments, P (2007) PPVT-4: Peabody picture vocabulary test. Minneapolis, MN.: Pearson Assessments.Google Scholar
Espinet, SD, Anderson, JE, & Zelazo, PD (2012) N2 amplitude as a neural marker of executive function in young children: An ERP study of children who switch versus perseverate on the Dimensional Change Card Sort. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience 2, S49S58.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Esposito, AG, & Baker-Ward, L (2013) Dual language education for low-income children: Preliminary evidence of benefits for executive function. Bilingual Research Journal 36, 295310.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fay-Stammbach, T, Hawes, DJ, & Meredith, P (2014) Parenting influence on executive function in early childhood: A review. Child Development Perspectives 8, 258264.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garon, N, Bryson, SE, & Smith, IM (2008) Executive function in preschoolers: a review using an integrative framework. Psychological Bulletin 134, 3160.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gentsch, A, Ullsperger, P, & Ullsperger, M (2009) Dissociable medial frontal negativities from a common monitoring system for self- and externally caused failure of goal achievement. NeuroImage 47, 20232030.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grammer, JK, Carrasco, M, Gehring, WJ, & Morrison, FJ (2014) Age-related changes in error processing in young children: A school-based investigation. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience 9, 93105.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grundy, JG, Anderson, JAE, & Bialystok, E (2017) Neural correlates of cognitive processing in monolinguals and bilinguals. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1396, 183201.10.1111/nyas.13333CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gutiérrez–Clellen, VF, & Kreiter, J (2003) Understanding child bilingual acquisition using parent and teacher reports. Applied Psycholinguistics 24, 267288.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hajcak, G, Moser, JS, Yeung, N, & Simons, RF (2005) On the ERN and the significance of errors. Psychophysiology 42, 151160.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hanania, R, & Smith, LB (2010) Selective attention and attention switching: Towards a unified developmental approach. Developmental Science 13, 622635.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Happaney, K, Zelazo, PD, & Stuss, DT (2004) Development of orbitofrontal function: Current themes and future directions. Brain and Cognition 55, 110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holroyd, CB, & Coles, MGH (2002) The neural basis of human error processing: Reinforcement learning, dopamine, and the error-related negativity. Psychological Review 109, 679709.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hongwanishkul, D, Happaney, KRK, Lee, WSC, & Zelazo, PD (2005) Assessment of hot and cool executive function in young children: age-related changes and individual differences. Developmental Neuropsychology 28, 617644.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kerr, A, & Zelazo, PD (2004) Development of “hot” executive function: The children's gambling task. Brain and Cognition 55, 148157.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, EY, Iwaki, N, Imashioya, H, Uno, H, & Fujita, T (2007) Error-related negativity in a visual go/no-go task: children vs adults. Developmental Neuropsychology 31, 181191.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ladouceur, CD, Dahl, RE, Birmaher, B, Axelson, DA, & Ryan, ND (2006) Increased error-related negativity (ERN) in childhood anxiety disorders: ERP and source localization. Journal of Child Psychology 47, 10731082.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luu, P, & Ferree, T (2005) Determination of the HydroCel Geodesic Sensor Nets' average electrode positions and their 10-10 international equivalents. Electrical Geodesic Inc., Technical Note.Google Scholar
Luk, G, De Sa, E, & Bialystok, E (2011) Is there a relation between onset age of bilingualism and enhancement of cognitive control? Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 14, 596.10.1017/S1366728911000186CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marchman, VA, Fernald, A, & Hurtado, N (2009) How vocabulary size in two languages relates to efficiency in spoken word recognition by young Spanish–English bilinguals. Journal of Child Language 37, 817840.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martin-Rhee, MM, & Bialystok, E (2008) The development of two types of inhibitory control in monolingual and bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 11, 8193.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McClelland, MM, Cameron, CE, Connor, CM, Farris, CL, Jewkes, AM, & Morrison, FJ (2007) Links between behavioral regulation and preschoolers’ literacy, vocabulary, and math skills. Developmental Psychology 43, 947959.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McDermott, JM, Westerlund, A, Zeanah, CH, Nelson, CA, & Fox, NA (2012) Early adversity and neural correlates of executive function: Implications for academic adjustment. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience 2, S59S66.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meyer, A, Weinberg, A, Klein, DN, & Hajcak, G (2012) The development of the error-related negativity (ERN) and its relationship with anxiety: Evidence from 8 to 13 year-olds. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience 2, 152161.10.1016/j.dcn.2011.09.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miyake, A, Friedman, NP, Emerson, MJ, Witzki, AH, Howerter, A, & Wager, TD (2000) The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology 41, 49100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nesbitt, KT, Baker-Ward, L, & Willoughby, MT (2013) Executive function mediates socio-economic and racial differences in early academic achievement. Early Childhood Research Quarterly 28, 774783.10.1016/j.ecresq.2013.07.005CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paap, KR (2014) The role of componential analysis, categorical hypothesising, replicability and confirmation bias in testing for bilingual advantages in executive functioning. Journal of Cognitive Psychology 26(April), 114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paap, KR, & Greenberg, ZI (2013) There is no coherent evidence for a bilingual advantage in executive processing. Cognitive Psychology 66, 232258.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paap, KR, Johnson, HA, & Sawi, O (2015) Bilingual advantages in executive functioning either do not exist or are restricted to very specific and undetermined circumstances. Cortex 69, 265278.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pearson, BZ, Fernandez, SC, Lewedeg, V, & Oller, DK (1997) The relation of input factors to lexical learning by bilingual infants. Applied Psycholinguistics 18, 4158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Place, S, & Hoff, E (2011) Properties of dual language exposure that influence 2-year-olds’ bilingual proficiency. Child Development 82, 18341849.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Qu, L, Finestone, D, Qin, L, & Reena, L (2013) Focused but fixed: The impact of expectation of external rewards on inhibitory control and flexibility in preschoolers. Emotion 13, 562572.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Qu, L, & Zelazo, PD (2007) The facilitative effect of positive stimuli on 3-year-olds’ flexible rule use. Cognitive Development 22, 456473.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ruberry, EJ, Lengua, LJ, Crocker, LH, Bruce, J, Upshaw, MB, & Sommerville, JA (2017) Income, neural executive processes, and preschool children's executive control. Development and Psychopathology, 22, 143154.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ryan, RM, & Deci, EL (2000) Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation: Classic definitions and new directions. Contemporary Educational Psychology 25, 5467.10.1006/ceps.1999.1020CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shaul, S, & Schwartz, M (2014) The role of the executive functions in school readiness among preschool-age children. Reading and Writing 27, 749768.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tarullo, AR, Nayak, S, St John, AM, & Doan, SN (2018) Performance Effects of Reward-Related Feedback on the Dimensional Change Card Sort Task. The Journal of Genetic Psychology 179, 171175.10.1080/00221325.2018.1466264CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thorell, L, Lindqvist, S, & Nutley, SB (2009) Training and transfer effects of executive functions in preschool children. Developmental Science 12, 106113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Valian, V (2015) Bilingualism and cognition: A focus on mechanisms. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 18, 4750.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Velanova, K, Wheeler, ME, & Luna, B (2008) Maturational changes in anterior cingulate and frontoparietal recruitment support the development of error processing and inhibitory control. Cerebral Cortex 18, 25052522.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weintraub, S, Dikmen, SS, Heaton, RK, Tulsky, DS, Zelazo, PD, Bauer, PJ, … Gershon, RC (2013) Cognition assessment using the NIH Toolbox. Neurology 80, S54S64.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weintraub, S, Dikmen, SS, Heaton, RK, Tulsky, DS, Zelazo, PD, Slotkin, J, … Gershon, R (2014) The cognition battery of the NIH toolbox for assessment of neurological and behavioral function: validation in an adult sample. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society 20, 567–78.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yeung, N, Botvinick, MM, & Cohen, JD (2004) The neural basis of error detection: Conflict monitoring and the error-related negativity. Psychological Review 111, 931959.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zelazo, PD, & Müller, U (2002) Executive function in typical and atypical development. In Goswami, U (Ed), Blackwell Handbook of Childhood Cognitive Development. Wiley Online Library, pp. 445469.10.1002/9780470996652.ch20CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zelazo, PD (2006) The Dimensional Change Card Sort (DCCS): a method of assessing executive function in children. Nature Protocols 1, 297301.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zelazo, PD, & Carlson, SM (2012) Hot and cool executive function in childhood and adolescence: Development and plasticity. Child Development Perspectives 6, 354360.Google Scholar
Zelazo, PD, Craik, F, & Booth, L (2004) Executive function across the life span. Acta Psychologica 115, 167183.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zelazo, PD, & Cunningham, WA (2007) Executive function: Mechanisms underlying emotion regulation. Handbook of Emotion Regulation. New York, NY, US: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Zelazo, P, Müller, U, & Frye, D (2003) The development of executive function in early childhood. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 68, i151.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed