Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-4rdpn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-11T12:36:24.277Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Executive control in fluent and lapsed bilinguals*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 December 2014

CARI ANNE BOGULSKI
Affiliation:
University of Arkansas, Fayetteville
MICHAEL RAKOCZY
Affiliation:
York University
MICHELLE GOODMAN
Affiliation:
York University
ELLEN BIALYSTOK*
Affiliation:
York University
*
Address for correspondence: Ellen Bialystok Department of PsychologyYork University4700 Keele Street Toronto, Ontario M3J 1P3Canadaellenb@yorku.ca

Abstract

Previous research showing a bilingual advantage on a variety of executive control tasks has typically compared monolinguals and fluent bilinguals. No study to date, however, has examined whether these effects endure for bilingual individuals who revert to monolingualism (‘lapsed bilinguals’). We investigated this question by testing monolinguals, full bilinguals, and lapsed bilinguals on a flanker task and a working memory task. Fully fluent bilinguals exhibited significantly more accurate performance than monolinguals on the working memory task, with lapsed bilinguals performing between the other two groups. Thus, continued bilingual experience appears necessary to maintain these cognitive advantages at a high level.

Type
Research Notes
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2014 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

*

This research was funded by grant A2559 from the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada (NSERC) to EB. We thank Kornelia Hawrylewicz and Deanna Friesen for their assistance in this study.

References

Bialystok, E., & Barac, R. (2012). Emerging bilingualism: Dissociating advantages for metalinguistic awareness and executive control. Cognition, 122 (1), 6773. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2011.08.003 Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., Klein, R., & Viswanathan, M. (2004). Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: Evidence from the Simon task. Psychology and Aging, 19 (2), 290303. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.19.2.290 Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., & Luk, G. (2008). Cognitive control and lexical access in younger and older bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34 (4), 859873. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.34.4.859 Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., & Ryan, J. (2006). Executive control in a modified antisaccade task: Effects of aging and bilingualism. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32 (6), 13411354. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.32.6.1341 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., Poarch, G. J., Luo, L., & Craik, F. I. M. (2014). Effects of bilingualism and aging on executive function and working memory, Psychology and Aging, 29, 696705.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., & Viswanathan, M. (2009). Components of executive control with advantages for bilingual children in two cultures. Cognition, 112 (3), 494500. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2009.06.014 Google Scholar
Blom, E., Kuntay, A. C., Messer, M., Verhagen, J., & Leseman, P. (2014). The benefits of being bilingual: Working memory in bilingual Turkish-Dutch children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 128C, 105119. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2014.06.007 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carlson, S. M., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2008). Bilingual experience and executive functioning in young children. Developmental Science, 11 (2), 282298. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00675.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cattell, R. B., & Cattell, A. K. S. (1960). The individual or group Culture Fair Intelligence Test. Champaign, IL: Institute for Personality and Ability Testing.Google ScholarPubMed
Colzato, L. S., Bajo, M. T., van Den Wildenberg, W., Paolieri, D., Nieuwenhuis, S., La Heij, W., & Hommel, B. (2008). How does bilingualism improve executive control? A comparison of active and reactive inhibition mechanisms. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34 (2), 302312. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.34.2.302 Google Scholar
Costa, A., Hernández, M., Costa-Faidella, J., & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2009). On the bilingual advantage in conflict processing: Now you see it, now you don’t. Cognition, 113 (2), 135149. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2009.08.001 Google Scholar
Costa, A., Hernández, M., & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2008). Bilingualism aids conflict resolution: Evidence from the ANT task. Cognition, 106 (1), 5986.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Delis, D. C., Kaplan, E., & Kramer, J. H. (2001). Verbal fluency subtest of the Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System. San Antonion, TX: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1997). The Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test–Third Edition. Bloomington, MN: Pearson Assessments.Google Scholar
Dunn, L. M., Thériault-Whalen, C. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1993). Échelle de Vocabulaire en Images Peabody. Toronto, Canada: Psycan.Google Scholar
Emmorey, K., Luk, G., Pyers, J. E., & Bialystok, E. (2008). The source of enhanced cognitive control in bilinguals: Evidence from bimodal bilinguals. Psychological Science, 19 (12), 12011206. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02224.x Google Scholar
Engel de Abreu, P. M. J., Gathercole, S. E., & Martin, R. (2011). Disentangling the relationship between working memory and language: The roles of short-term storage and cognitive control. Learning and Individual Differences, 21 (5), 569574.Google Scholar
Eriksen, B. A., & Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Perception and Psychophysics, 16 (1), 143149.Google Scholar
Gold, B. T., Kim, C., Johnson, N. F., Kriscio, R. J., & Smith, C. D. (2013). Lifelong bilingualism maintains neural efficiency for cognitive control in aging. Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 387396.Google Scholar
Green, D. W., & Abutalebi, J. (2013). Language control in bilinguals: The adaptive control hypothesis. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25 (5), 515530. doi:10.1080/20445911.2013.796377 Google Scholar
Jonides, J., & Nee, D. E. (2006). Brain mechanisms of proactive interference in working memory. Neuroscience, 139 (1), 181193. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.06.042 Google Scholar
Kalashnikova, M., & Mattock, K. (2014). Maturation of executive functioning skills in early sequential bilingualism, International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 17, 111123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kapa, L.L., & Colombo, J. (2013). Attentional control in early and later bilingual children. Cognitive Development, 28, 233246.Google Scholar
Khare, V., Verma, A., Kar, B., Srinivasan, N., & Brysbaert, M. (2013). Bilingualism and the increased attentional blink effect: Evidence that the difference between bilinguals and monolinguals generalizes to different levels of second language proficiency. Psychological Research, 77 (6), 728737.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kovács, A. M., & Mehler, J. (2009). Cognitive gains in 7-month-old bilingual infants. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106 (16), 65566560. doi:10.1073/pnas.0811323106 Google Scholar
Kroll, J. F., & Bialystok, E. (2013). Understanding the consequences of bilingualism for language processing and cognition. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25 (5), 497514. doi: 10.1080/20445911.2013.799170 Google Scholar
Kushalnagar, P., Hannay, H. J., & Hernandez, A. E. (2010). Bilingualism and attention: A study of balanced and unbalanced bilingual Deaf users of American Sign Language and English. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 15 (3), 263273. doi:10.1093/deafed/enq011 Google Scholar
Luk, G., Anderson, J. A. E., Craik, F. I. M., Grady, C., & Bialystok, E. (2010). Distinct neural correlates for two types of inhibition in bilinguals: Response inhibition versus interference suppression. Brain and Cognition, 74 (3), 347357. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2010.09.004 Google Scholar
Luk, G., & Bialystok, E. (2013). Bilingualism is not a categorical variable: Interaction between language proficiency and usage. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25 (5), 605621. doi:10.1080/20445911.2013.795574.Google Scholar
Luo, L., Luk, G., & Bialystok, E. (2010). Effect of language proficiency and executive control on verbal fluency performance in bilinguals. Cognition, 114 (1), 2941. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2009.08.014 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martin-Rhee, M. M., & Bialystok, E. (2008). The development of two types of inhibitory control in monolingual and bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 11 (1), 8193. doi:10.1017/S1366728907003227 Google Scholar
Morales, J., Calvo, A., & Bialystok, E. (2013). Working memory development in monolingual and bilingual children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 114 (2), 187202. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2012.09.002 Google Scholar
Paap, K. R., & Greenberg, Z. I. (2013). There is no coherent evidence for a bilingual advantage in executive processing. Cognitive Psychology, 66 (2), 232258. doi:10.1016/j.cogpsych.2012.12.002 Google Scholar
Prior, A., & MacWhinney, B. (2010). A bilingual advantage in task switching. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13 (2), 253262. doi:10.1017/S1366728909990526 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schroeder, S. R., & Marian, V. (2012). A bilingual advantage for episodic memory in older adults. Journal of Cognitive, 24 (5), 591601. doi:10.1080/20445911.2012.669367 Google Scholar
Singh, L., Fu, C. S. L., Rahman, A. A., Hameed, W. B., Sanmugam, S., Agarwal, P., . . . Rifkin-Graboi, A. (2014). Back to basics: A bilingual advantage in infant visual habituation. Child Development. doi:10.1111/cdev.12271 Google Scholar
Singh, N., & Mishra, R. K. (2012). Does language proficiency modulate oculomotor control?: Evidence from Hindi-English bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15 (4), 771781. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1366728912000065 Google Scholar
Singh, N., & Mishra, R. K. (2013). Second language proficiency modulates conflict-monitoring in an oculomotor Stroop task: Evidence from Hindi-English bilinguals. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 322. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00322 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sternberg, S. (1966). High-speed scanning in human memory. Science, 153 (3736), 652654.Google Scholar
Yang, S., Yang, H., & Lust, B. (2011). Early childhood bilingualism leads to advances in executive attention: Dissociating culture and language. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 14 (3), 412422. doi:10.1017/S1366728910000611 Google Scholar