Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-4rdpn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-11T10:37:39.378Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Orthographic influences on division of labor in learning to read Chinese and English: Insights from computational modeling*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 September 2012

JIANFENG YANG
Affiliation:
Key Laboratory of Behavioral Science, Institute of Psychology, Chinese Academy of Sciences, Beijing 100101, China
HUA SHU
Affiliation:
State Key Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience and Learning, Beijing Normal University, Beijing 100875, China
BRUCE D. McCANDLISS
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Vanderbilt University Peabody College of Education and Human Development, Nashville, TN 37240, USA
JASON D. ZEVIN*
Affiliation:
Sackler Institute for Developmental Psychobiology, Weill Cornell Medical College, NY 10021, USA
*
Address for correspondence: Jason D. Zevin, Box 140, Sackler Institute for Developmental Psychobiology, Weill Cornell Medical College, New York, NY 10021, USAjdz2001@med.cornell.edu

Abstract

Learning to read in any language requires learning to map among print, sound and meaning. Writing systems differ in a number of factors that influence both the ease and the rate with which reading skill can be acquired, as well as the eventual division of labor between phonological and semantic processes. Further, developmental reading disability manifests differently across writing systems, and may be related to different deficits in constitutive processes. Here we simulate some aspects of reading acquisition in Chinese and English using the same model for both writing systems. The contribution of semantic and phonological processing to literacy acquisition in the two languages is simulated, including specific effects of phonological and semantic deficits. Further, we demonstrate that similar patterns of performance are observed when the same model is trained on both Chinese and English as an “early bilingual”. The results are consistent with the view that reading skill is acquired by the application of statistical learning rules to mappings among print, sound and meaning, and that differences in the typical and disordered acquisition of reading skill between writing systems are driven by differences in the statistical patterns of the writing systems themselves, rather than differences in cognitive architecture of the learner.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2012

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

*

This research was supported by Open Research Fund of the State Key Laboratory of Cognitive Neuroscience and Learning (CNKOPZD1005, J.Y.), NSF of China 31171077 (J.Y.), 30870758 (H.S.) and the Fundamental Research Funds for the Central Universities (H.S.), NIH R01 HD06579, R01-HD067364 (J.D.Z.) and R01 DC007694 (B.D.M.), NSF REC 0337765 (B.D.M.). The authors wish to thank Ping Li and two anonymous reviewers for their helpful comments on this manuscript.

References

Andrews, S., & Scarratt, D. R. (1998). Rule and analogy mechanisms in reading nonwords: Hough dou peapel rede gnew wirds? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24 (4), 10521086.Google Scholar
Bates, E., Burani, C., D'amico, S., & Barca, L. (2001). Word reading and picture naming in Italian. Memory and Cognition, 29 (7), 986999.Google Scholar
Bolger, D. J., Perfetti, C. A., & Schneider, W. (2005). Cross-cultural effect on the brain revisited: Universal structures plus writing system variation. Human Brain Mapping,25 (1), 92104.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carlisle, J. F. (2000). Awareness of the structure and meaning of morphologically complex words: Impact on reading. Reading and Writing, 12 (3–4), 169190.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carlisle, J. F. (2003). Morphology matters in learning to read: A commentary. Reading Psychology, 24, 291322.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Castles, A., & Coltheart, M. (1993). Varieties of developmental dyslexia. Cognition, 47 (2), 149180.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Castles, A., & Coltheart, M. (1996). Cognitive correlates of developmental surface dyslexia: A single case study. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 13 (1), 2550.Google Scholar
Coltheart, M., Curtis, B., Atkins, P., & Haller, M. (1993). Models of reading aloud: Dual-route and parallel-distributed-processing approaches. Psychological Review, 100 (4), 589608.Google Scholar
Coltheart, M., Rastle, K., Perry, C., Langdon, R., & Ziegler, J. (2001). DRC: A dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. Psychological Review, 108 (1), 204256.Google Scholar
DeFrancis, J. (1989). Visible speech: The diverse oneness of writing systems. Honolulu, HI: University of Hawaii Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, N. C., & Hooper, A. M. (2001). It is easier to learn to read in Welsh than in English: Effects of orthographic transparency demonstrated using frequency-matched cross-linguistic reading tests. Learning, Language and Development, 22 (4), 571599.Google Scholar
Frost, R. (2005). Orthographic systems and skilled word recognition processes in reading. In Snowling, M. J. & Hulme, C. (eds.), The science of reading: A handbook, pp. 272295. Oxford: Blackwell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frost, R. (in press). Towards a universal model of reading. Behavioral and Brain Sciences.Google Scholar
Frost, R., Katz, L., & Bentin, S. (1987). Strategies for visual word recognition and orthographical depth: A multilingual comparison. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 13 (1), 104115.Google Scholar
Goswami, U., Gombert, J. E., & Barrera, L. F. de (1998). Children's orthographic representations and linguistic transparency: Nonsense word reading in English, French, and Spanish. Applied Psycholinguistics, 19 (1), 1952.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harm, M. W., & Seidenberg, M. S. (1999). Phonology, reading acquisition, and dyslexia: Insights from connectionist models. Psychological Review, 106 (3), 491528.Google Scholar
Harm, M. W., & Seidenberg, M. S. (2004). Computing the meanings of words in reading: Cooperative division of labor between visual and phonological processes. Psychological Review, 111 (3), 662720.Google Scholar
Hetherington, P., & Seidenberg, M. S. (1989). Is there “catastrophic interference” in connectionist networks?The 11th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, pp. 2633. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Ho, C. S., Chan, D. W., Chung, K. K. H., Lee, S.-H., & Tsang, S.-M. (2007). In search of subtypes of Chinese developmental dyslexia. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 97 (1), 6183.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ku, Y.-M., & Anderson, R. C. (2003). Development of morphological awareness in Chinese and English. Reading and Writing, 16 (5), 399422.Google Scholar
Language and Teaching Institute of Beijing Language College (1986). Modern Chinese frequency dictionary. Beijing: Beijing Language College Publisher. [In Chinese.]Google Scholar
Lee, C.-Y., Tsai, J.-L., Su, E. C.-I., Tzeng, O. J. L., & Hung, D. L. (2005). Consistency, regularity, and frequency effects in naming Chinese characters. Language and Linguistics, 6 (1), 75107.Google Scholar
Leong, C. (1973). Reading in Chinese with reference to reading practices in Hong Long. In Downing, J. A. (ed.), Comparative reading: Cross national studies of behavior and processes in reading and writing, pp. 383402. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Li, Y., & Kang, J. (1993). Analysis of phonetics of the ideophonetic characters in modern Chinese. Information analysis of usage of characters in modern Chinese, pp. 8498. Shanghai: Shanghai Education Publisher. [In Chinese.]Google Scholar
Liu, Y., Shu, H., & Li, P. (2007). Word naming and psycholinguistic norms: Chinese. Behavior Research Methods, 39 (2), 192198.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Malone, K. (1925). Benjamin Franklin on spelling reform. American Speech, 1 (2), 96100.Google Scholar
Manis, F. R., Seidenberg, M. S., Doi, L. M., McBride-Chang, C., & Petersen, A. (1996). On the bases of two subtypes of development dyslexia. Cognition, 58 (2), 157195.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marcus, M., Santorini, B., & Marcinkiewicz, M. A. (1993). Building a large annotated corpus of English: The Penn Treebank. Computational Linguistics, 19, 313330.Google Scholar
Mattingly, I. G. (1992). Linguistic awareness and orthographic form. In Frost, R. & Katz, L. (eds.), Orthography, phonology, morphology, and meaning, pp. 1126. Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
McBride-Chang, C., Cho, J.-R., Liu, H., Wagner, R. K., Shu, H., Zhou, A., Cheuk, C. S., & Muse, A. (2005). Changing models across cultures: Associations of phonological awareness and morphological structure awareness with vocabulary and word recognition in second graders from Beijing, Hong Kong, Korea, and the United States. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 92 (2), 140160.Google Scholar
McBride-Chang, C., Shu, H., Zhou, A., Wat, C. P., & Wagner, R. K. (2003). Morphological awareness uniquely predicts young children's Chinese character recognition. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95 (4), 743751.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McBride-Chang, C., Wagner, R. K., Muse, A., Chow, B. W.-Y., & Shu, H. (2005). The role of morphological awareness in children's vocabulary acquisition in English. Applied Psycholinguistics, 26 (3), 415435.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McBride-Chang, C., & Zhong, Y. (2006). Emergent literacy skills in Chinese. In Li, P., Tan, L. H., Bates, E. & Tzeng, O. J. L. (eds.), The handbook of East Asian psycholinguistics (vol. 1): Chinese, pp. 8189. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Mirkovic, J., MacDonald, M. C., & Seidenberg, M. S. (2005). Where does gender come from? Evidence from a complex inflectional system. Language and Cognitive Processes,20 (1/2), 139167.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nation, K., & Snowling, M. (1998). Semantic processing and the development of word recognition skills: Evidence from children with reading comprehension difficulities. Journal of Memory and Language, 39, 85101.Google Scholar
Nation, K., & Snowling, M. (1999). Developmental differences in sensitivity to semantic relations among good and poor comprehenders: Evidence from semantic priming. Cognition, 70, B113.Google Scholar
Pearlmutter, B. A. (1995). Gradient calculations for dynamic recurrent neural networks: A survey. IEEE Transactions on Neural Networks, 6 (5), 12121228.Google Scholar
Peng, D., & Yang, H. (1997). The phonological processing of Chinese phonograms. Asia and Pacific Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing, 2 (2), 177195.Google Scholar
Perfetti, C. A., Liu, Y., & Tan, L. H. (2005). The lexical constituency model: Some implications of research on Chinese for general theories of reading. Psychological Review, 112 (1), 4359.Google Scholar
Plaut, D. C. (1997). Structure and function in the lexical system: Insights from distributed models of word reading and lexical decision. Language and Cognitive Processes, 12, 767808.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Plaut, D. C., McClelland, J. L., Seidenberg, M. S., & Patterson, K. (1996). Understanding normal and impaired word reading: Computational principles in quasi-regular domains. Psychological Review, 103 (1), 56115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Polk, T. A., & Farah, M. J. (1997). A simple common contexts explanation for the development of abstract letter identities. Neural Computation, 9 (6), 12771289.Google Scholar
Polk, T. A., & Farah, M. J. (1998). The neural development and organization of letter recognition: Evidence from functional neuroimaging, computational modeling, and behavioral studies. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 95 (3), 847852.Google Scholar
Saiegh-Haddad, E., & Geva, E. (2008). Morphological awareness, phonological awareness, and reading in English–Arabic bilingual children. Reading and Writing, 21 (5), 481504.Google Scholar
Seidenberg, M. (1993). A connectionist modeling approach of the word recognition and dyslexia. Psychological Science, 4 (5), 299304.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seidenberg, M. (1995). Visual word recognition. In Miller, J. & Eimas, P. (eds.), Handbook of perception and cognition(vol. 11), pp. 138163. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Seymour, P. H. K., Aro, M., & Erskine, J. M. (2003). Foundation literacy acquisition in European orthographies. British Journal of Psychology, 94, 143174.Google Scholar
Shu, H., McBride-Chang, C., Wu, S., & Liu, H. (2006). Understanding Chinese developmental dyslexia: Morphological awareness as a core cognitive construct. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98 (1), 122133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shu, H., Meng, X., Chen, X., Luan, H., & Cao, F. (2005). The subtypes of developmental dyslexia in Chinese: Evidence from three cases. Dyslexia, 11 (4), 311329.Google Scholar
Shu, H., Peng, H., & McBride-Chang, C. (2008). Phonological awareness in young Chinese children. Developmental Science, 11 (1), 171181.Google Scholar
Snowling, M. J. (2000). Dyslexia (2nd edn.). Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Stanovich, K. E., & Siegel, L. S. (1994). Phenotypic performance profile of reading-disabled children: A gression-based test of the phonological core variable-difference model. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 2453.Google Scholar
Strain, E., Patterson, K., & Seidenberg, M. S. (1995). Semantic effects in single-word naming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21 (5), 11401154.Google Scholar
Strain, E., Patterson, K., & Seidenberg, M. S. (2002). Theories of word naming interact with spelling–sound consistency. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28 (1), 207214.Google ScholarPubMed
Tan, L. H., Laird, A. R., Li, K., & Fox, P. T. (2005). Neuroanatomical correlates of phonological processing of Chinese characters and alphabetic words: A meta-analysis. Human Brain Mapping, 25 (1), 8391.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taraban, R., & McClelland, J. L. (1987). Conspiracy effects in word pronunciation. Journal of Memory and Language, 26 (6), 608631.Google Scholar
Temple, C. M., & Marshall, J. C. (1983). A case study of developmental phonological dyslexia. British Journal of Psychology, 74 (4), 517533.Google Scholar
Toyoda, E., & Scrimgeour, A. (2009). Common and script-specific awareness in relation to word recognition in English and Chinese. Language Awareness, 18 (1), 6173.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vellutino, F. R., & Fletcher, J. M. (2005). Developmental dyslexia. In Margaret, C. H. & Snowling, J. (ed.), The science of reading: A handbook, pp. 362378. Oxford: Blackwell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Venezky, R. L. (1970). The structure of English orthography. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Venezky, R. L. (1999). American way of spelling: The structure and origins of American English orthography. New York: The Guildford Press.Google Scholar
Weekes, B. S., Yin, W., Su, F., & Chen, M. J. (2006). The cognitive neuropsychology of reading and writing in Chinese. Language and Linguistics, 7 (3), 595617.Google Scholar
White, T. G., Graves, M. F., & Slater, W. H. (1990). Growth of reading vocabulary in diverse elementary schools: Decoding and word meaning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82 (2), 281290.Google Scholar
Xing, H., Shu, H., & Li, P. (2004). The acquisition of Chinese characters: Corpus analyses and connectionist simulations. Journal of Cognitive Science, 5 (1), 149.Google Scholar
Yang, J., McCandliss, B. D., Shu, H., & Zevin, J. D. (2009). Simulating language-specific and language-general effects in a statistical learning model of Chinese reading. Journal of Memory and Language, 61 (2), 238257.Google Scholar
Zevin, J. D., & Seidenberg, M. S. (2006). Simulating consistency effects and individual differences in nonword naming: A comparison of current models. Journal of Memory and Language, 54 (2), 145160.Google Scholar
Zhao, X., & Li, P. (2009). An online database of phonological representations for Mandarin Chinese. Behavior Research Methods, 41 (2), 575583.Google Scholar
Zhu, X. (1988). Analysis of cueing function of phonetic components in modern Chinese. In Yuan, X. (ed.), Proceedings of the Symposium on the Chinese Language and Characters, pp. 8599. Bejing: Guang Ming Daily Press.Google Scholar
Ziegler, J. C., & Goswami, U. (2005). Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia, and skilled reading across languages: A psycholinguistic grain size theory. Psychological Bulletin, 131 (1), 329.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed