Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-7cvxr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-29T14:21:49.540Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Who is bilingual? Snapshots across the lifespan

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  31 August 2018

John A. E. Anderson
Affiliation:
York University
Kornelia Hawrylewicz
Affiliation:
York University
Ellen Bialystok*
Affiliation:
York University
*
Address for correspondence: Ellen Bialystok, Department of Psychology, York University, 4700 Keele Street, Toronto, OntarioM3J 1P3, Canadaellenb@yorku.ca

Abstract

Building on our earlier analysis of the factorial structure of bilingualism for young adults obtained from the Language and Social Background Questionnaire (LSBQ; Anderson, Mak, Keyvani Chahi & Bialystok, 2018), we analyzed responses from 675 children and 125 older adults to a similar questionnaire. Three factors accounting for 74% of the variance emerged in the analysis of children's responses: Adult Language in the Home, Non-English use for Media, Non-English use with Siblings. There were also three factors that explained the responses of older adults that accounted for 79% of the variance: Non-English Use, Non-English Proficiency, and English Proficiency. Therefore, bilingual experience is captured by different factors at different points in the lifespan. These results are discussed in conjunction with the earlier results from young adults and the implications for understanding bilingualism across the lifespan.

Type
Review Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2018

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

*Preparation of this paper was supported by grants R01HD052523 and R21AG048431 from the US National Institutes of Health and grant A2559 from the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada to EB.

References

Anderson, J. A. E., Mak, L., Keyvani Chahi, A., & Bialystok, E. (2018). The language and social background questionnaire: Assessing degree of bilingualism in a diverse population. Behavior Research Methods, 50 (1), 250263. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-017-0867-9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bedore, L. M., Peña, E. D., Summers, C. L., Boerger, K. M., Resendiz, M. D., Greene, K., Bohman, T. M., & Gillam, R. B. (2012). The measure matters: Language dominance profiles across measures in Spanish–English bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15 (3), 616629. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728912000090CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bialystok, E. (2016). The signal and the noise. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 6 (5), 517534. https://doi.org/10.1075/lab.15040.biaCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cooper, R. L. (1969). Two Contextualized Measures of Degree of Bilingualism$\ast$. Modern Language Journal, 53 (3), 172178. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4781.1969.tb04585.xGoogle Scholar
Gollan, T. H., Weissberger, G. H., Runnqvist, E., Montoya, R. I., & Cera, C. M. (2012). Self-ratings of spoken language dominance: A Multilingual Naming Test (MINT) and preliminary norms for young and aging Spanish–English bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15 (3), 594615. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728911000332CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Green, D. W., & Abutalebi, J. (2013). Language control in bilinguals: The adaptive control hypothesis. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25 (5), 515530. https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2013.796377CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grosjean, F. (1998). Studying bilinguals: Methodological and conceptual issues. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 1 (2), 131149. https://doi.org/10.1017/S136672899800025XCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hartanto, A., & Yang, H. (2016). Disparate bilingual experiences modulate task-switching advantages: A diffusion-model analysis of the effects of interactional context on switch costs. Cognition, 150, 1019. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2016.01.016CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hatcher, L. (2005). A step-by-step approach to using the SAS system for factor analysis and structural equation modeling. SAS Institute Cary, NC.Google Scholar
JASP Team. (2018). JASP (Version 0.8.6)[Computer software].Google Scholar
Li, P., Sepanski, S., & Zhao, X. (2006). Language history questionnaire: A Web-based interface for bilingual research. Behavior Research Methods, 38 (2), 202210. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03192770CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Li, P., Zhang, F., Tsai, E., & Puls, B. (2014). Language history questionnaire (LHQ 2.0): A new dynamic web-based research tool. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 17 (3), 673680. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728913000606CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luk, G., & Bialystok, E. (2013). Bilingualism is not a categorical variable: Interaction between language proficiency and usage. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25 (5), 605621. https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2013.795574CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marian, V., Blumenfeld, H. K., & Kaushanskaya, M. (2007). The Language Experience and Proficiency Questionnaire (LEAP-Q): Assessing Language Profiles in Bilinguals and Multilinguals. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50 (4), 940967. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2007/067)CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ooi, S. H., Goh, W. D., Sorace, A., & Bak, T. H. (2018). From Bilingualism to Bilingualisms: Bilingual experience in Edinburgh and Singapore affects attentional control differently. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 113. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728918000020Google Scholar
Paap, K. R., & Greenberg, Z. I. (2013). There is no coherent evidence for a bilingual advantage in executive processing. Cognitive Psychology, 66 (2), 232258. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2012.12.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Statistics Canada. (2017, August). Census in Brief: Linguistic diversity and multilingualism in Canadian homes. Statistics Canada Catalogue no. 98-200-X2016010. Retrieved May 2, 2018, from http://www12.statcan.gc.ca/census-recensement/2016/as-sa/98-200-x/2016010/98-200-x2016010-eng.cfmGoogle Scholar
Surrain, S., & Luk, G. (2017). Describing bilinguals: A systematic review of labels and descriptions used in the literature between 2005–2015. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 115. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728917000682Google Scholar
Wilson, R., & Dewaele, J.-M. (2010). The use of web questionnaires in second language acquisition and bilingualism research. Second Language Research, 26 (1), 103123. https://doi.org/10.1177/0267658309337640CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wu, Y. J., & Thierry, G. (2013). Fast Modulation of Executive Function by Language Context in Bilinguals. Journal of Neuroscience, 33 (33), 1353313537. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4760-12.2013CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed