Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-lnqnp Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-29T11:19:09.688Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Comparative Theories in Clinical Decision Making and their Application to Practice: a Reflective Case Study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  06 December 2012

Nicholas Gladstone*
Affiliation:
The South West London Elective Orthopaedic Centre, Epsom, Surrey, UK
*
Correspondence to: N. Gladstone, Senior Practice Development Nurse, The South West London Elective Orthopaedic Centre, Epsom, Surrey, KT18 7EG, UK. Tel: 01372 735813, Fax: 01372 735854; E-mail: nicholas.gladstone@eoc.nhs.uk.

Abstract

Within this article the author critically reviews the theories surrounding clinical decision making and judgement while discussing a clinical incident, and his experiences of decision making within his own practice setting. Exploring the works of Elstein and Schwarz, Benner, Hammond and Hamm, the author discusses how aspects from each of their theories relate to his practice and clinical reasoning before concluding on the clinical decision-making process and factors that can influence their successful application.

Type
Reflection
Copyright
Copyright © British Association of Anaesthetic and Recovery Nursing 2012

Introduction

The author's experience of clinical decision making details the diagnosis of an acute myocardial infarction and the decision-making process taken to reach this. To maintain confidentiality, the identity of the patient, hospital and other healthcare professionals have been anonymised in accordance with the Nursing and Midwifery Council's (NMC) Guidelines on confidentiality [NMC, 2008].

Incident

Although walking through the post-operative care ward, the author noticed a patient lying in bed who appeared unwell with shallow ‘disorganised’ breathing. On closer examination the gentleman appeared cold and clammy with a grey pallor and a blue cyanotic tinge to his lips. The author attempted to speak to the gentleman; however, he was confused and anxious and talking incoherently. The author immediately undertook an Airway, Breathing and Circulation (ABC) assessment, checked his airway, placed him on 15 L of oxygen via a Hudson non-rebreathe mask and checked his vital signs. The gentleman's pulse rate was 120 beats per minute and regular; he had unrecordable oxygen saturations and his systolic blood pressure was 89 mmHg. The author immediately tilted the base of the patient's bed and set up an IV infusion of plasma expander (colloid) to raise his blood pressure.

Working in post-operative rehabilitation it is often common to encounter patients who suffer with orthostatic hypotension and syncope secondary to hypovolaemic shock. However, the author was concerned that the patient was not responding to the usual treatment provided and contacted the duty doctor on call. He was informed that the gentleman had been reviewed some 10 min earlier and there was no cause for concern as the patient appeared comfortable and was sleeping. It was explained that the patient was acutely unwell and the doctor responded that he would re-review the patient shortly. As the author was convinced that the gentleman's condition was deteriorating, he undertook an electrocardiogram (ECG), which upon examination indicated a myocardial infarction.

Concerned that he was not getting the appropriate response from the duty doctor the author called the clinical lead down from the Post-Anaesthetic Care Unit to help assess the patient again. On examination of the ECG, a posterior myocardial infarction was identified and the gentleman was immediately transferred to a High Dependency Care bed in her unit. The gentleman was promptly started on the Acute Coronary Syndrome protocol of care and further diagnostic tests revealed an extensive myocardial infarction.

Literature review

Decision making forms the foundation of all nursing care and associated interventions from the administration of medicines to assistance with activities of living and rehabilitation. Over the past 40 years considerable emphasis has been placed on the need for knowledge and evidence to support practice and guide the decisions healthcare professionals make. Policies and directives issued by the Government and Department of Health [Department of Health, 2010; House of Commons, 2011] stipulate a need for high quality care underpinned by research and clear evidence.

The phrase ‘Clinical Decision Making’ is used synonymously with terms such as ‘Clinical Judgement’, ‘Problem Solving’ and ‘Critical Thinking’ implying that it is a cognitive process concerned with problem recognition through the identification of cues and clinical features, data gathering, integration, analysis, evaluation and choice to produce an informed decision [Clack, Reference Clack2009]. Standing [Reference Standing2010] suggests that decision making is a complex process that involves observation, information processing, critical thinking and clinical judgement to select the best course of action in promoting and maintaining a patient's health. While Elstein and Schwarz [Reference Elstein and Schwarz2002] assert that clinical decision making involves a rational process of ‘hypothetico-deductive reasoning’ based on information processing.

Within clinical decision-making theory, ‘hypothetico-deductive reasoning’ is considered the most dominant approach in health care with practice based on rationality and empirical precision [Jefford et al, Reference Jefford, Fahy and Sundin2011]. A hermeneutic examination of figures, text, verbal and non-verbal cues forms the foundation from which a hypothesis is generated and subsequent treatment is based. This diagnostic ‘paradigm’ is formed of four stages that assist the assessor in identifying and interpreting cues and constructing and evaluating a hypothesis (primary diagnosis).

These four stages are:

  1. 1. Cue acquisition – primary data and sensory stimuli that steers the nurse towards a particular thought process through specific cues.

  2. 2. Hypothesis generation – development of a provisional and differential diagnosis based on cues and baseline data.

  3. 3. Interpretation of Cues – re-exploration and interpretation of cues to support or dismiss hypothesis with further data collection to aid interpretation.

  4. 4. Evaluation of Hypothesis – the cues are then evaluated and applied to an overall hypothesis that directs the decision made and subsequent intervention/action taken.

Relating the author's clinical experience to the hypothetico-deductive approach, cue acquisition involved the collation of information from previous baseline observations and past medical history with new data that included visual cues and vital signs. From this information an initial hypothesis was formed that suggested the patient was having a heart attack. Further interpretation of cues included the use of electrocardiography that revealed a posterior myocardial infarction leading the author to evaluate and determine his hypothesis as correct.

As detailed above, the hypothetico-deductive model of clinical problem solving relates to the way a nurse or healthcare professional processes patient-relevant information [Norman, Reference Norman2005]. This scientific method to problem solving and analysis is considered beneficial due to the linear approach and prescriptive manner in which practitioners address a given situation; hypothesis generation is considered rational and structured, and related to the directionality of interpretation [Banning, Reference Banning2007]. This assertion is supported by Botti and Reeve [Reference Botti and Reeve2003] who define the generation of a hypothesis through data acquisition and interpretation as ‘forward reasoning’, and claim that clinical experts use such an approach in clinical decision making and diagnosis.

However, this mode of decision making is not infallible and a number of researchers have questioned whether such a simplistic approach to assessment can facilitate safe, accurate diagnosis and subsequent treatment [Patel and Groen, Reference Patel and Groen1986; Kennedy, Reference Kennedy2002; Priddy, Reference Priddy2004; Banning, Reference Banning2007].

Hypothetico-deductive reasoning is dependent on the hypothesis generated. That is, application of an inaccurate hypothesis can lead to a misdiagnosis and an inaccurate result. An example of this is the hypothesis generated in the author's scenario in which he diagnosed a heart attack based on the symptoms presented and associated cues. Symptoms of extreme anxiety, cold clammy pallor, a positive ‘Portsmouth Sign’, unrecordable oxygen saturations and peripheral shutdown are also associated with late signs of septic shock [Woodrow, Reference Woodrow2000]. Thus, it can be suggested that the author could have erroneously interpreted the cues leading to misdiagnosis and ineffective treatment. This assertion is supported by Rozeboom [Reference Rozeboom1990] who is particularly scathing in his criticism of this approach, claiming that the ‘epistemic fecklessness’ of hypothetico-deductive reasoning wrongly implies a simple rational process to decision making without acknowledging context, affect, emotions and intuition.

While hypothetico-deductivism provides a linear and scientific structure to the decision-making process, it fails to acknowledge intuition and experiential learning, both of which influence the approach taken. This notion is supported by Cioffi [Reference Cioffi1997] who claims that intuition is a legitimate and essential component of clinical judgement and decision making. Yet up until the past 25 years, and until the seminal work of Dreyfus and Dreyfus and Benner, the notion of experience and intuition in decision making was not considered legitimate due to ambiguity and a lack of ‘hard-data’ [Mallick, Reference Mallick1981].

In a study of decision-making behaviour among US Air Force pilots, brothers Stuart and Hubert Dreyfus developed a model of skill acquisition based on situated performance and experiential knowledge [Dreyfus and Dreyfus, Reference Dreyfus and Dreyfus1980]. Developing previous research undertaken by Johnson-Laird and Wason (1977, cited in Dreyfus and Dreyfus, Reference Dreyfus and Dreyfus1980, pp. 4–5), the authors determined a subject's task performance improved significantly if related to previous experience. Further research identified a direct correlation between the levels of skill acquisition in relation to concrete experience. As professionals gain experience in their specific line of work, they move through a developmental continuum progressing from a novice to an expert. Dreyfus and Dreyfus, [Reference Dreyfus and Dreyfus1980] assert that workers move through five stages of career development that they categorized as: novice, advanced beginner, competent, proficient and expert. With each stage of ‘skill’ acquisition comes an increase in knowledge and ability. At the beginning, the ‘novice’ acts according to rules that determine a specific action; in the later stages of this paradigm, the ‘expert’ is able to make intuitive decisions based on previous experience, operating outside guidelines and scientific principles [Blum, Reference Blum2010].

In her book ‘From Novice to Expert’, Benner [Reference Benner2001] applies the principles of Dreyfus and Dreyfus’ model to nursing practice and clinical decision making. Employing the five stages of novice to expert, Benner suggests that novices undertake clinical decision making guided by rules and policies. Novices have little experience in clinical decision making and critical analysis and rely on guidelines and rules for direction. Decision making and associated actions are often based on rudimentary ‘primary’ concept formation influenced by fear, mistakes and the need for acceptance from one's peers [Daley, Reference Daley1999]. As the nurse moves through the stages of skill acquisition and gains experience, ‘secondary’ concept formation occurs and new and pre-existing knowledge is assimilated in a process referred to as ‘subsumption’ [Vacek, Reference Vacek2009]. The expert is able to make decisions based on both abstract and concrete information, which has been obtained through individual experience and skill acquisition. This notion is supported by Benner, who states that ‘at the heart of good clinical judgement and clinical wisdom lies experiential learning from particular cases’, before going on to state that nursing practice requires both techné (craftsmanship) and phronesis (wisdom) [Benner et al, Reference Benner, Tanner and Chesla2009, p. XV (introduction)].

What is clear from the work of both Dreyfus and Dreyfus and Benner is that practice is guided by experiential knowledge and that intuition can only take place in the presence of this. However, one significant criticism of the notion of novice to expert is the lack of any clear definition as to what an ‘expert’ actually is [Peña, Reference Peña2010]. Lyneham et al [Reference Lyneham, Parkinson and Denholm2008] asserted that Benner's theory fails to explicate the final stage of the expert practitioner and it therefore remains open to debate. The authors go on to provide their own definition based on Benner's principles suggesting that the expert practitioner uses ‘embodied intuition; taking what action is appropriate at the time, doing so freely, without conscious thought and practising within both cognitive and embodied intuitive paradigms’ [Lyneham et al, Reference Lyneham, Parkinson and Denholm2008, p. 384].

Referring to the author's scenario it is hard to consider his actions as that of an ‘expert’. What occurred with the identified patient was not part of the author's usual working practice and was a predominantly medical (cardiac) problem. Yet despite this, the author instinctively undertook what he considered was the most appropriate action based on a pre-existing knowledge and experience of cardiac arrest and resuscitation. The use of ‘heuristics’ based on previous experience provided a ‘cognitive shortcut’ in the decision-making process and allowed the author to develop a rapid response to a difficult situation.

Based on Simon's theory of bounded rationality [Reference Simon1991], Tversky and Kahneman [Reference Tversky and Kahneman1974] transformed academic research and theories on judgement with their ideas on ‘heuristics’ and rationality. The authors assert that when faced with complex decisions individuals often make use of experience-based problem solving techniques using a three-tier process:

  1. 1. Availability – assessing probability of actions based on previous phenomena and recalling previous occurrences.

  2. 2. Representativeness – comparing representation of data with that from previous experiences. Estimating the probability that patient A is having the same problems as patient B through the comparison of cues and data.

  3. 3. Anchoring/adjustment – Developing an initial hypothesis (anchor) based on availability and representativeness, and then adjusting this based on additional information.

This heuristic framework was employed when the author undertook his decision making in the incident detailed above, with the recollection of previous incidents involving a cardiac arrest guiding the author's actions. The patient presented with symptoms commensurate with previous experience and the author was able to undertake a representative comparison of the cues presented in both. Having developed a hypothesis based on ‘rough pattern recognition’ the author then ‘anchored’ this with the use of additional diagnostics that confirmed this.

However, despite this cognitive approach being successful it can be suggested that the author's actions were based on ‘trial and error’ rather than structured assessment. Thompson and Dowding [Reference Thompson and Dowding2009] claim that the use of heuristics generates predictable systematic biases and errors due to the subjective nature of the decision-making process. Tversky and Kahneman suggest that when recalling previous experience, individuals will often only recall those incidents where interventions or decisions were positive and had ‘favourable’ outcomes; thus rendering the decision-making process biased and unrealistic. Reflecting on the two approaches detailed above, it is apparent that intuition and analysis are at separate ends of the decision-making spectrum. Yet, it can be asserted that decision making is seldom entirely intuitive or analytical but a combination of both.

Based on Brunswik's Lens Model [see Thompson et al, Reference Thompson, Cullum, McCaughan, Sheldon and Raynor2004], Hammond's Cognitive Continuum Theory (CCT) focuses not only on the decision maker, but also on the environmental factors that influence cognition and the decision-making process, taking into account both analytical and intuitive strategies [Hammond, Reference Hammond1996]. Hammond's theory rejects a dichotomous view of intuition and analysis, instead detailing six modes of cognitive inquiry that places intuition and analysis at opposing ends of the continuum [Dunwoody et al, Reference Dunwoody, Haarbauer, Mahan, Marino and Tang2000]:

  1. 1. Physical science experiment

  2. 2. Control group experiment

  3. 3. Quasi-experiment

  4. 4. Computer modelling

  5. 5. Expert judgement

  6. 6. Unrestricted judgement.

In his theory, Hammond argues that different decision-making tasks require different approaches according to the situation and task complexity [Hammond, Reference Hammond1996]. The cognitive continuum places tasks along a vertical axis in accordance with the decision-making approach taken. Along the horizontal axis, is the cognitive approach taken with the decision-making process beginning with ‘pure intuition’ and moving across to ‘pure analysis’. In addition to the two axis, Hammond subdivides the cognitive continuum into six modes that detail the divergent intuitive and analytical methods associated with different tasks and the structure of the decision-making process [Thompson et al, Reference Thompson, Cullum, McCaughan, Sheldon and Raynor2004]. The scientific/analytical modes of 1–3 enable the decision maker to apply explicit theoretical knowledge supported with evidence-based practice and associated research. The intuitive/experimental modes of 4–6 allows the decision maker to undertake tasks supported by tacit knowledge and trial and error [Standing, Reference Standing2010]. Thus, the CCT focuses on the relationship between the concepts of tasks and modes of cognition with Hammond asserting that the more structured a task is, the more analytically induced the decision-making process will be. In contrast, an ill-structured decision-making task is likely to be intuition-induced with little analysis involved [Cader et al, Reference Cader, Campbell and Watson2004].

Hamm [Reference Hamm1988] later revised Hammond's theory to explore doctors’ understanding of decision making and clinical judgement; amending the terminology used in the six modes of inquiry to allow for better association of tasks:

  1. 1. Scientific experiment

  2. 2. Controlled trial

  3. 3. Quasi-experiment

  4. 4. System-aided judgement

  5. 5. Peer-aided judgement

  6. 6. Intuitive judgement.

Hamm also determined that the quality of the task structure was in direct proportion to the amount of time taken along with the potential for judgement bias and visibility of the task process.

In his study, Hamm identified that decision making among doctors fell between the 5th and 6th modes of the cognitive continuum, whereby mainly intuitive and peer-aided judgement was used [Standing, Reference Standing2010]. Thompson [Reference Thompson1999] asserts that employing the cognitive continuum in nursing practice can help provide the ‘middle ground’ in decision making that allows for the use of both analytical and intuitive cognition. By sitting firmly between Elstein's hypothetico-deductive model [Elstein and Schwarz, Reference Elstein and Schwarz2002] and the notion of intuition and tacit knowledge derived from the work of Dreyfus and Dreyfus [Reference Dreyfus and Dreyfus1980] and Benner [Reference Benner2001], the cognitive continuum allows for the synthesis of both theories allowing for more comprehensive decision making. Reviewing Thompson's work, Harbison [Reference Harbison2001] endorses his notion of the usefulness of the cognitive continuum in nursing asserting that Hammond's paradigm helps to instill quality in the decision-making process and subsequent care provision by supporting the need for evidence-based practice and a sound knowledge base. However, Harbison is keen to point out that there needs to be further investigation to determine what constitutes ‘quality’ in nursing judgement and decision making. It can be suggested that like Benner's theory of expertise, the notion of quality is enigmatic and subjective, which is open to mixed interpretation and a multitude of conflicting descriptions.

Like Hamm [Reference Hamm1988], Standing [Reference Standing2008] further revised Hammond's Cognitive Continuum in an attempt to make it more applicable to the nursing profession. Standing asserts that the definitions used in both Hammond and Hamm's versions of the continuum are ambiguous with the typology used for task structure confusing. Moreover, Standing states that decision making in nursing not only involves peer-aided judgement, but involvement from the patient too. In revising Hammond's theory, Standing changes the terminology used and adds a further three ‘modes of practice’ within the cognitive continuum that she feels underpins decision making in nursing. Standing's ‘revised cognitive continuum’ [Standing, Reference Standing2008] identifies the benefits of reflective practice and replaces the quasi-rational and experimental modes of cognition with more specific categories that include action, experiential, qualitative, survey and experimental research.

In removing quasi-rationality from the cognitive modes, Standing acknowledges that, unlike the medical model of care, decision making in nursing does not take a predominantly ‘scientific’ stance. The nursing profession prefers a ‘holistic’ approach that encompasses reflection on and in action, patient involvement and evidence-based practice. This notion is supported by Harbison [Reference Harbison2001] who claims that nurses are reluctant to adopt medical frameworks and theories due to a need for ‘professional identity’ coupled with the fact that decision making in nursing is fundamentally different from the technical/rational approach taken in medicine.

Furthermore, Standing's revised theory removes the numerical sequencing from 1–6 as she asserts that decision making should not be seen as a ‘linear’ activity, but a ‘flexible continuum oscillating in either direction along the continuum in response to continually changing judgement tasks’ [Standing, Reference Standing2008, p. 130].

Standing's revised continuum provides a rational and structured approach that promotes the ethical and professional aspects of decision making in nursing. Recognising that the modes of practice are not sequential, by removing the didactic numerical scale to each mode signifies the personal nature of decision making and the need for an objective multi-modal approach.

Referring to the author's clinical incident, it can be suggested that his actions and the approach taken were positioned across a number of modes in Standing's revised continuum, encompassing elements of intuitive, reflective and peer-aided judgement. Furthermore, having made a clinical decision and undertaken the associated actions, the author returned to the continuum to review this with the use of reflective judgement and critical reflection to evaluate the patient outcome.

Conclusion

In conclusion, it can be suggested that the theoretical principles of the CCT and associated paradigms developed by Hammond [Reference Hammond1996], Standing [Reference Standing2008], Dreyfus and Dreyfus [Reference Dreyfus and Dreyfus1980] and Benner [Reference Benner2001] all provide the conceptual framework and insight from which clinical decisions are formed. The ideas and methods identified are well supported by research that identifies the many different approaches the nurse can take in clinical judgement and decision making. However, it can be suggested that in reality, as patients’ needs change and care provision constantly evolves, these theoretical models will soon become obsolete, forcing the nursing profession to reassess their approach to clinical decision-making theory and practice. Moreover, as demonstrated by Benner [Reference Benner2001] and Elstein et al [Reference Elstein, Shulma and Sprafka1978], no decision-making model is completely infallible; environmental factors associated with socio-economic instability and changes to the way in which care is provided have the potential to ‘shift the goalposts’ and make the decision-making process more difficult even for the ‘experts’.

It is appreciated that the theories discussed within this article have not been explored as fully as the author would have liked due to the constraints of this assignment. However, it is hoped that the link between theory and practice is evident. Although the author takes a more pragmatic approach to the issues raised in this assignment, it is evident that the theories have applications.

References

Banning, M. A review of clinical decision making: models and current research. Journal of Clinical Nursing 2007; 17(2): 187195.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benner, P. From Novice to Expert [Commemorative edition]. Prentice Hall Health: Upper Saddle River, New Jersey, 2001.Google Scholar
Benner, P, Tanner, C, Chesla, C. Expertise in Nursing Practice: Caring, Clinical Judgement & Ethics. Springer Publishing: New York, 2009.Google Scholar
Blum, CA. Using the Benner intuitive-humanistic decision-making model in action: a case study. Nurse Education in Practice 2010; 10: 303307.Google Scholar
Botti, M, Reeve, R. Role of knowledge and ability in student nurses’ clinical decision-making. Nursing and Health Sciences 2003; 5: 3949.Google Scholar
Cader, R, Campbell, S, Watson, D. Cognitive Continuum Theory in nursing decision-making. Journal of Advanced Nursing 2004; 49(4): 397405.Google Scholar
Cioffi, J. Heuristics, servants to intuition, in clinical decision-making. Journal of Advanced Nursing 1997; 26: 203208.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clack, G. Decision making in nursing practice: a case review. Paediatric Nursing 2009; 21(5): 2427.Google Scholar
Daley, B. Novice to expert: an exploration of how professionals learn. Adult Education Quarterly 1999; 49(4): 133147.Google Scholar
Department of Health. Equity and Excellence: Liberating the NHS. London: HMSO, 2010.Google Scholar
Dreyfus, SE, Dreyfus, HL. A five stage model of the mental activities involved in directed skill acquisition. Operations Research Centre: University of California 1980; 80(2): 118.Google Scholar
Dunwoody, PT, Haarbauer, E, Mahan, RP, Marino, C, Tang, C. Cognitive adaptation and its consequences: a test of Cognitive Continuum Theory. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making 2000; 13(1): 3554.3.0.CO;2-U>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elstein, A, Schwarz, A. Clinical problem solving and diagnostic decision making: selective review of the cognitive literature. British Medical Journal 2002; 324(7339): 729732.Google Scholar
Elstein, AS, Shulma, LS, Sprafka, SA. Medical problem solving: An analysis of clinical reasoning. Cambridge: Harvard University Press, 1978.Google Scholar
Hamm, RM. Clinical intuition and clinical analysis: expertise and the cognitive continuum. In: Dowie J, Elstein A (eds). Professional Judgement: A Reader in Clinical Decision Making. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1988: 78109.Google Scholar
Hammond, KR. Human Judgment and Social Policy: Irreducible Uncertainty, Inevitable Error, Unavoidable Injustice. New York: Oxford University Press, 1996.Google Scholar
Harbison, J. Clinical decision making in nursing: theoretical perspectives and their relevance to practice. Journal of Advanced Nursing 2001; 35(1): 126133.Google Scholar
House of Commons. Health and Social Care Bill 2010–2011. London: Stationery Office, 2011.Google Scholar
Jefford, E, Fahy, K, Sundin, D. Decision-Making theories and their usefulness to the midwifery profession both in terms of midwifery practice and the education of midwives. International Journal of Nursing Practice 2011; 17: 246253.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kennedy, C. The decision making process in a district nursing assessment. British Journal of Community Nursing 2002; 7(10): 505513.Google Scholar
Lyneham, J, Parkinson, C, Denholm, C. Explicating Benner's concept of expert practice: intuition in emergency nursing. Journal of Advanced Nursing 2008; 64(4): 380387.Google Scholar
Mallick, MJ. Patient assessment based on data not intuition. Nursing Outlook 1981; 29(10): 600605.Google Scholar
Norman, G. Research in clinical reasoning: past history and current trends. Medical Education 2005; 39(4): 418427.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nursing and Midwifery Council (NMC). The Code: Standards of Conduct, Performance and Ethics for Nurses and Midwives. London: NMC Publications, 2008.Google Scholar
Patel, VL, Groen, GJ. Knowledge based solution strategies in medical reasoning. Cognitive Science 1986; 10: 91116.Google Scholar
Peña, A. The Dreyfus model of clinical problem-solving skills acquisition: a critical perspective. Medical Education Online 2010; 15: 4846.Google Scholar
Priddy, KD. Is there logic behind fetal monitoring? Journal of Obstetric, Gynecologic and Neonatal Nursing 2004; 33: 550553.Google Scholar
Rozeboom, WW. Hypothetico-deductivism is a fraud. American Psychologist 1990; 45(4): 555556.Google Scholar
Simon, HA. Bounded rationality and organizational learning. Organization Science, Special Issue: Organizational Learning: Papers in Honor of (and by) James G. March 1991; 2(1): 125–134.Google Scholar
Standing, M. Clinical judgement and decision-making in nursing – nine modes of practice in a revised cognitive continuum. Journal of Advanced Nursing 2008; 62(1): 124134.Google Scholar
Standing, M. (ed.). Clinical Judgement and Decision-Making in Nursing and Interprofessional Healthcare. Maidenhead: McGraw Hill, 2010.Google Scholar
Thompson, C. A conceptual treadmill: the need for ‘middle ground’ in clinical decision making theory in nursing. Journal of Advanced Nursing 1999; 30(5): 12221229.Google Scholar
Thompson, C, Cullum, N, McCaughan, D, Sheldon, T, Raynor, P. Nurses, information use, and clinical decision making – the real world potential for evidence-based decisions in nursing. Evidence Based Nursing 2004; 7: 6872.Google Scholar
Thompson, C, Dowding, D. Theoretical approaches. In: Thompson C, Dowding D (eds). Essential Decision Making and Clinical Judgement for Nurses. Churchill Livingstone, London, 2009: 5577.Google Scholar
Tversky, A, Kahneman, D. Judgement under uncertainty: heuristics and biases. Science, New Series 1974; 185(4157): 11241131.Google Scholar
Vacek, JE. Using a conceptual approach with concept mapping to promote critical thinking. Journal of Nursing Education 2009; 8(1): 4548.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Woodrow, P. Intensive Care Nursing: A Framework for Practice. London: Routledge, 2000.Google Scholar