Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-s2hrs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-10T08:28:03.614Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Are the Effects of Terrorism Short-Lived?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 September 2023

Vincenzo Bove*
Affiliation:
Department of Politics and International Studies, University of Warwick, Coventry CV4 7AL, UK
Georgios Efthyvoulou
Affiliation:
Department of Economics, University of Sheffield, 9 Mappin Street, Sheffield, S1 4DT, UK
Harry Pickard
Affiliation:
Newcastle University Business School, Newcastle University, 5 Barrack Road, Newcastle upon Tyne, NE1 4SE, UK
*
Corresponding author. Vincenzo Bove; Email: v.bove@warwick.ac.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Terrorism elicits strong public reactions immediately after the attack, with important implications for democratic institutions and individual well-being. Are these effects short-lived? We answer this question using a natural experiment design and combining data on terrorist attacks in the United Kingdom with a Continuous Monitoring Survey. We find that heightened risk perceptions and emotional reactions in the wake of deadly attacks do not dissipate in the very short run but are sustained over time and up to 120 days after the attacks. Whereas large-scale attacks cause a long-lasting shift in risk assessments and emotions, the corresponding effect of smaller-scale terrorism incidents appears to subside within one month. Overall, the impact of terrorism does not fade away easily.

Type
Letter
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

Terrorist violence has considerable effects on key attitudes such as trust in government, migration preferences, and commitment to civil liberties (Balcells and Torrats-Espinosa Reference Balcells and Torrats-Espinosa2018; Davis and Silver Reference Davis and Silver2004; Dinesen and Jæger Reference Dinesen and Jæger2013; Falcó-Gimeno, Muñoz, and Pannico Reference Ferrín, Mancosu and Cappiali2022; Godefroidt Reference Hainmueller, Mummolo and Xu2023; Huddy et al. Reference Legewie2005; Legewie Reference Maguen, Papa and Litz2013). Terrorist acts also result in a ‘complex state of negative emotional arousal’ (Godefroidt Reference Hainmueller, Mummolo and Xu2023, 5), making ordinary people feel vulnerable and helpless and eliciting negative emotions such as anxiety, anger, and sadness (Hansen, Dinesen and Østergaard Reference Hansen2017; Hansen et al. Reference Helbling and Meierrieks2016; Nussio, Bove, and Steele Reference Pennebaker and Harber2019; Sønderskov et al. Reference Starr and Goldfarb2021).

In this paper we depart from the magnitude of the terrorism effects and focus instead on their duration. We contribute specifically to recent studies that identify the causal impact of terrorism on public sentiments by exploiting the unexpected occurrence of a terrorist attack during the fieldwork of a public opinion survey, where the timing of the event assigns survey respondents into treatment and control groups as good as randomly (Muñoz, Falcó-Gimeno, and Hernández Reference Nussio, Böhmelt and Bove2020). Given the nature of the research question and because of data limitations, these studies usually track public reactions to terrorism only for a few days after an attack, with the corresponding time frames ranging from three to thirty days (Balcells and Torrats-Espinosa Reference Balcells and Torrats-Espinosa2018; Bove, Efthyvoulou, and Pickard Reference Bove, Efthyvoulou and Pickard2022; Breton and Eady Reference Breton and Eady2022; Ferrín, Mancosu, and Cappiali Reference Germann, Godefroidt and Mendez2020; Germann, Godefroidt, and Mendez Reference Godefroidt2022; Godefroidt Reference Hainmueller, Mummolo and Xu2023; Holman et al. Reference Huddy2022; Nussio, Böhmelt, and Bove Reference Nussio, Bove and Steele2021; Pickard, Efthyvoulou, and Bove Reference Shoshani and Slone2023; Van Hauwaert and Huber Reference Van Hauwaert and Huber2020).

Overall, existing evidence suggests that terrorism does shape citizens' attitudes and emotions in important ways. As of yet, however, much less is known about the duration of such effects.Footnote 1 To address this puzzle, we rely on uninterrupted series of individual-level observations with a large N, which allows us to retain the statistical power of our analysis across all time frames considered. Using survey items that are designed to capture attitudes and feelings elicited by terrorism greatly reduces the risk of bias due to other events. In addition, we move beyond the focus on a single emblematic event, and instead exploit the timing of six terrorism incidents (three deadly and three foiled attacks) to shed light on the heterogeneity of the resulting effects and enhance the generalizability of the findings. Perhaps more crucially, we explore first-order effects: the perceptions of terrorism risk and the emotions that are stimulated by the attack itself. Risk assessments and negative emotions following terrorist episodes shape cognition and policy preferences (Epifanio Reference Falcó-Gimeno, Muñoz and Pannico2016; Helbling and Meierrieks Reference Holman, Merolla and Zechmeister2022; Huddy et al. Reference Legewie2005), with wider implications for individual well-being and mental health (Sønderskov et al. Reference Starr and Goldfarb2021). We thus consider risk perceptions and the negative emotions stimulated by terrorism as highly consequential variables.Footnote 2

What explains the temporal dynamics of attention to terrorism, and in particular the duration of its effects? We can isolate two contrasting ex ante predictions. One might expect risk perceptions and emotional reactions to be short-lived and subject to a swift ‘return to homeostasis’ or baseline values (Brandon and Silke Reference Brandon, Silke, Bongar, Brown, Beutner, Breckenridge and Zimbardo2006; Maguen, Papa, and Litz Reference Muñoz, Falcó-Gimeno and Hernández2008). This is underwritten by the general tendency of perturbations to subside as individuals habituate psychologically and return to baseline arousal levels. Breton and Eady (Reference Breton and Eady2022), for example, find that indices of anxiety towards refugees rise sharply, then diminish quickly (within ten days) after a terrorist incident. Yet, the impacts of terrorism could also have a more lasting duration given its unpredictable nature and the dramatic way in which it forces the public to revise its beliefs about risk (Bux and Coyne Reference Bux and Coyne2009). An initial ‘emergency stage’ of intense emotional reaction tends to last for one month and is then followed by a ‘plateau’ period of another month, wherein mental rumination is maintained at high levels (Pennebaker and Harber Reference Pickard, Efthyvoulou and Bove1993). Given the theoretical ambiguity, we consider the duration of the emotional and cognitive effects of terrorism as an empirical question. Appendix A.1 offers additional theoretical insights on our expectations with regards to the duration of these (first-order) effects.

Data and Empirical Strategy

We use individual-level data on public opinion and emotions from the Continuous Monitoring Survey (CMS) – a set of monthly internet surveys of the British electorate (with around 1.3K respondents participating in each survey) that were conducted as part of the British Election Study over the period 2004–14. The CMS has component questions that are asked every month, and, as such, it offers uninterrupted series of individual-level observations.

To capture individuals' perceptions about the risk of terrorism, we explore their answer to the following statement: ‘Do you think the risk of terrorism to British citizens these days is…’; with possible responses being: a lot better, a little better, the same, a little worse, and a lot worse. To capture emotions, we rely on their answer to the question: ‘Which, if any, of the following words describe your feelings about the risk of terrorism to British citizens (please tick up to four)?’, with possible responses being: angry, happy, disgusted, hopeful, uneasy, confident, afraid, and proud. Among negative feelings, the four stated ones – anger, fear, disgust, and unease (or anxiety) – are the most prevalent in the context of terrorism, with important psychological and cognitive consequences.

Using the CMS data, we create the main outcome variables for our regression analysis. We first construct the variable ‘Risk of terror’, a binary indicator taking value 1 if people report that the risk of terrorism these days is either a little worse or a lot worse (and 0 otherwise). We then construct four binary indicators ‘Anger’, ‘Disgust’, ‘Unease’, and ‘Fear’, each taking value 1 if people choose the corresponding word to describe their feelings about the risk of terrorism (and 0 otherwise), as well as a ‘Negative emotions index’ using the average value of the four aforementioned variables.

Comparing individual responses before and after terrorist attacks enables us to examine the causal effect of terrorism on people's responses. Our identification strategy relies on the assumption that the timing of attacks is exogenous (unexpected) and largely randomly assigned relative to that of the interviews, and thus individuals interviewed after the attack can be defined as the ‘treatment’ group whereas those interviewed before the attack can be defined as the ‘control’ group (Muñoz, Falcó-Gimeno, and Hernández Reference Nussio, Böhmelt and Bove2020). We consider three of the four ‘major’ terrorist attacks that occurred between 2004 and 2013: the London bombings (7 July 2005), the Glasgow airport attack (30 June 2007), and the Lee Rigby murder (22 May 2013).Footnote 3 All three attacks received widespread media coverage and resulted in deaths, which makes them particularly impactful and relevant. Moreover, all three attacks were motivated by Islamic extremism, which ensures that the reactions to terrorism are homogeneous with respect to the characteristics of the perpetrators (Pickard, Efthyvoulou, and Bove Reference Shoshani and Slone2023).

Our empirical model specification takes the following form:

(1)$$y_{irw} = \alpha + \beta T_{irw} + \lambda _{rw} + \varepsilon _{irw}$$

where y irw is one of the outcome variables for individual i, living in region r,Footnote 4 and interviewed around the time of terrorist attack w; T irw is a binary indicator that takes value 1 if the individual was interviewed after the day of the attack, and 0 before the day of the attack;Footnote 5 λ rw represents attack-by-region fixed effects; and, $\varepsilon _{irw}$ is an error term, clustered at the attack-by-region level. We compare answers between the control group, interviewed thirty days before the attack, and three different treatment groups, interviewed at three distinct time frames: (i) one week after the attack (very short run), when the threat is the most salient and emotions are potentially very high; (ii) the first month minus the first week after the attack (short run), which represents the short period following the initial emotional reaction; and (iii) the first four months minus the first month after the attack (medium run), which allows us to assess whether any reaction is short-lived or yields a more permanent shift in attitudes or emotions (see also Epifanio, Giani, and Ivandic, Reference Epifanio, Giani and Ivandic2023).

A concern that may arise when one considers outcomes that are measured a long time after the treatment occurred is that this might lead to bias due to the occurrence of other unrelated events (Muñoz, Falcó-Gimeno, and Hernández Reference Nussio, Böhmelt and Bove2020). As such, many researchers choose to rely on short time intervals around the event date to reduce the probability of other factors driving the estimated effects. An important reason why this concern is much less acute in our context is that we focus on the first-order effects of terrorism and exploit information from survey items that are designed to capture attitudes and feelings elicited by terrorism – as opposed to general attitudes or the emotional state which may depend on a wide range of factors and can be influenced by multiple events. This, together with the fact that no other major terrorist incidents occurred within 120 days after the sampled attacks,Footnote 6 maximizes the probability that the post-attack changes in the outcome variables were caused by these attacks, and allows us to credibly estimate the duration of the resulting effects.

Another possible threat to our identification strategy is that individuals with specific characteristics may respond to the survey at different points in time, and these characteristics may be predictive of the outcome. In Appendix B.1, we show that there is a strong balance in observed characteristics across treatment and control units, and that the reported estimates do not change when we augment Eq. (1) with a wide set of individual-level controls. In the same appendix, we also show that our results persist when we use entropy weighting or coarsened exact matching to correct for possible imbalances.

Empirical Results

Dynamics of Risk Perceptions and Negative Feelings

We start by providing a graphical representation of the conditional relationship between the treatment indicator (in bins) and the mean of the variable ‘Risk of terror’, using a ‘binned scatterplot’. Relative to the standard approach of reporting results – that is, plots of fitted values – the binned scatterplot depicts the non-parametric relationship of interest and allows the quick detection of non-linearities, outliers and distributional differences (Hainmueller, Mummolo, and Xu Reference Hansen, Dinesen and Østergaard2019; Starr and Goldfarb Reference Van Hauwaert and Huber2020).Footnote 7

As shown in Fig. 1, exposure to new terrorist attacks leads to strong post-attack reactions: the public's perceived risk of terrorism increases from around 0.52 (on a 0–1 scale) to more than 0.75 in the first few days following the attacks. We can also see that the mean estimate of ‘Risk of terror’ declines in the medium run but remains significantly higher than that of the control group. In fact, the temporal dynamics reveal a level shift upwards that is sustained over time and up to 120 days after the attacks.Footnote 8

Figure 1. Risk of terror: non-parametric estimates.

Notes: The upper panel shows the binned scatterplot and the corresponding confidence intervals and confidence bands (Cattaneo et al. Reference Cattaneo, Crump, Farrell and Feng2019), implemented using the binsreg package. The lower panel shows the frequency of observations.

We continue by estimating the treatment effect across three pre-determined time frames: the very short run, the short run, and the medium run. The results are displayed in the left panel of Fig. 2. In line with the patterns of Fig. 1, we can observe a large and highly statistically significant change of perceptions in the very short run, which persists in the short run. Substantively, the point estimates imply that the perceived risk of terrorism increases by about 50 per cent relative to the pre-treatment mean. In the medium run, we can observe a decline in risk assessments compared to the short run; yet, the treatment effect retains its statistical significance and is way above the pre-treatment levels, suggesting that people continue to feel that another attack is possible for a quite long time after the attack. To ensure that the reported estimates are unlikely to be observed by chance, we perform permutation tests that randomly shuffle the data 1,000 times and estimate a treatment effect for each random draw and each time frame. The resulting distributions are displayed in the right panel of Fig. 2. As can be seen, there is 0 per cent probability that the observed treatment effects are observed by chance.

Figure 2. Risk of terror: main results.

Notes: The left panel shows the treatment effect on public's perceptions about the risk of terrorism across the three time frames. Standard errors are clustered at the attack-by-region level. Fat (thin) lines signify the 90 per cent (95 per cent) confidence interval. The sample sizes are: N (very short run) = 4,186; N (short run) = 6,397; and N (medium run) = 13,870. The right panel shows the results from permutation tests that randomly shuffle the data 1,000 times, stratified by attack-by-region, and estimate a treatment effect for each random draw and each time frame. The reference lines show the observed effects, with labels reporting the proportion of times that the treatment effects under the permuted data are at least as extreme as under the observed data.

Figure 3 shows the results for emotions of negative valence, based on the same regression set-up as in Fig. 2. The evolution of negative feelings, as captured by the overall index, is consistent with the dynamics of risk assessments: there is a sharp increase in the first seven days after the attacks, which persists for one month and is then followed by a noticeable decline (in the next ninety days). Still, even in the medium run, the treatment effect remains substantively and statistically significant. Turning now to the four components, we can see that anger, and to some extent disgust, prevail over the other negative feelings in the very short run and in the short run, and can largely explain the more intense emotional reaction to terrorism in the immediate aftermath of the attacks.Footnote 9

Figure 3. Negative emotions: main results.

Notes: The figure shows the treatment effect on the outcome listed on the horizontal axis across the three time frames. Standard errors are clustered at the attack-by-region level. Fat (thin) lines signify the 90 per cent (95 per cent) confidence interval. The sample sizes are: N (very short run) = 4,350; N (short run) = 6,615; and N (medium run) = 14,314.

Results for Individual Attacks

One would expect the results to be stronger and longer-lasting for attacks that are considered to be more consequential and threatening to the general population, as captured, for example, by the number of victims or the extent of national media coverage. To test for this, we run the same analysis as in Figs. 2 and 3 for each sampled attack separately, and report the results in Fig. 4. For the 2005 London bombings (attack 1) – a highly shocking and sensational event with a large number of victims and a quite long media cycle – the effects seem to persist over a long period of time: both risk perceptions and negative feelings increase in the very short run, become more pronounced in the short run, and stabilize (at the initial post-attack levels) in the medium run.Footnote 10 On the other hand, for the 2007 Glasgow airport attack and the 2013 Lee Rigby murder (attacks 2 and 3, respectively) – two less severe terrorist incidents with a small number of victims and a short media cycle – the effects appear to be transitory: while there is a large increase in risk assessments and negative emotions in the very short run (similar to that of the 2005 London bombings), both reactions become weaker in the short run and return to baseline levels (or remain marginally above them) in the medium run.

Figure 4. Risk of terror and negative emotions: results for each attack.

Notes: See notes of Figs 2 and 3. Attack 1 = 2005 London bombings; Attack 2 = 2007 Glasgow airport attack; Attack 3 = 2013 Lee Rigby murder. The sample sizes for the left panel are: N 1 (very short run) = 1,211; N 1 (short run) = 2,511; N 1 (medium run) = 5,138; N 2 (very short run) = 1,156; N 2 (short run) = 2,237; N 2 (medium run) = 5,172; N 3 (very short run) = 1,819; N 3 (short run) = 1,649; N 3 (medium run) = 3,560. Similar sample sizes are used in the right panel.

Two conclusions emerge from this analysis. First, large-scale attacks can cause a large and long-lasting shift in public reactions and second, smaller-scale attacks can still trigger sizable changes in attitudes and emotions, which tend to dissipate within one month, in line with the media cycle of these events. Appendix A.4 provides a discussion about the role of media coverage in shaping terrorist effects and provides evidence about the extent and duration of coverage for each of the three attacks.

Comparison with Foiled Attacks

We perform a benchmarking exercise where we compare our results with those for three foiled airplane hijackings, whose timing coincides with the CMS data-collection period (taking place on the 9 August 2006, 25 December 2009, and 9 October 2010). Each event was covered extensively by national media, including articles in the Guardian, the Telegraph, and the BBC News. The left panel of Fig. 5 presents the treatment effect of these foiled attacks on ‘Risk of terror’ across the three time frames. Overall, we can observe an increase in risk assessments, which quickly decays back to baseline levels. Substantively, the effect is about 40 per cent smaller in the very short run, and about four times as small in the short run, compared to the deadly attacks. Turning now to the evolution of negative feelings after these foiled attacks (right panel of Fig. 5), we can detect a very small increase in ‘Anger’ and ‘Disgust’ in the very short run and in the short run – which is 50 per cent to 75 per cent smaller than for deadly attacks – but there are no effects in the medium run (and no effects at all for the other two feelings).

Figure 5. Risk of terror and negative emotions: foiled attacks.

Notes: See notes of Figs 2 and 3. The sample sizes for the left panel are: N (very short run) = 3,139; N (short run) = 5,834; and N (medium run) = 10,971. Similar sample sizes are used in the right panel.

Overall, our results support the argument that foiled terrorist attacks, when they are largely reported in the media, can produce a ‘strong alarming effect on a wide audience’ (Shoshani and Slone Reference Sønderskov2008, 637), which can lead to increased threat perceptions and anger in the first few weeks after the attacks. However, as memories of such ‘near-miss’ terrorist incidents fade and evaluations of how close the events came to being fatal diminish, the resulting effects quickly return to normal levels.

Further Analyses and Robustness Tests

In Appendices B.2 to B.10, we carry out additional analyses and robustness checks. Specifically, we perform validity tests to strengthen our identification assumptions, including tests for pre-existing trends (Section B.2); check sensitivity to using quintile-based time frames (Section B.3); conduct placebo tests on alternative unrelated outcomes (Section B.4); examine the treatment effect on positive emotions about the risk of terrorism (Section B.5); test for heterogeneity in the terrorism effects across individuals (Section B.6); compare the results between attacked and non-attacked regions (Section B.7); check robustness to using a probit model (Section B.8); and consider the effects of a foiled and low-reported attack (Section B.9). Taken together, the results lend credibility to our causal claims and provide strong support to our key findings. Finally, in Section B.10, we test for a ‘second-order echo effect’ of terrorism: its influence on migration attitudes. We find that terrorism can also lead to a long-lasting shift in such attitudes, with people perceiving the number of asylum seekers as a more important problem after the attacks compared to before the attacks.

Conclusions

Are the emotional and cognitive effects of terrorism short-lived? To address this question, we rely on uninterrupted series of individual-level observations and employ a natural experiment design. Specifically, we compare survey responses before and after three deadly and three foiled attacks in the United Kingdom and track public opinion dynamics across three time periods: the first week after the attacks, the first month (minus the first week), and the next three months. We find that the impact of terrorism lasts well beyond the few days after an attack, particularly for deadly attacks with a long media cycle.

The deleterious consequences attached to these heightened risk perceptions and emotional reactions are likely to confront policymakers long after the attacks occur. Increased risk perceptions and fear of terrorism can lead to stronger public support for policies that prioritize security and surveillance at the expense of civil liberties. Similarly, by priming ‘mortality salience’, terrorism can contribute to the prevalence of prejudiced attitudes towards out-groups and the marginalization of vulnerable communities, with downstream effects on individual well-being and mental health. Yet, terrorism can also result in actions spilling over across the border, as high levels of anger towards terrorists can lead to calls for more aggressive military actions which seek to retaliate against an identifiable target. As such, disentangling the quantitative effects of terrorism over the short and long run is a crucial task for policymakers, public institutions, and scholars alike.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at: https://doi.org/10.1017/S0007123423000352.

Data availability statement

Replication Data for this article can be found in Harvard Dataverse at: https://doi.org/10.7910/DVN/YWWXEP.

Aknowledgements

We would like to thank Armine Ghazaryan, Andreas Kanaris Miyashiro and Angel Valdivieso Gonzalez for excellent research assistance, and Diego Zambiasi, Soeren Henn, and three anonymous referees for helpful comments and suggestions. We are also indebted to the participants of the UNU-WIDER workshop ‘Institutional Legacies of Violent Conflict’, the ‘London Workshop on Conflict’ at Royal Holloway University and Queen Mary University, and a research seminar at Newcastle University.

Financial support

This is a revised version of WIDER Working Paper 2026/66, UNU-WIDER, Helsinki, which was funded and prepared within the UNU-WIDER project ‘Institutional legacies of violent conflict’. This research was also supported by the Economic and Social Research Council (ESRC Grant number: ES/V002333/1).

Competing interests

None.

Footnotes

1 Two exceptions are Bozzoli and Müller (Reference Bozzoli and Müller2011) and Epifanio, Giani, and Ivandic (Reference Epifanio, Giani and Ivandic2023) who focus on the effects of the 2005 London bombing over a longer time period, but rely on relatively small samples.

2 In Appendix A.1 we discuss the relation between risk perceptions and emotional reactions and how they shape policy preferences.

3 Appendix A.2 offers background material on the three attacks.

4 England, Scotland, and Wales are divided into eleven regions.

5 To avoid measurement errors, we dropped individuals who were interviewed on the same day of the attack.

6 The first (national or international) major attack that occurred after the three sampled attacks is the Westgate Shopping Mall attack in Kenya (21 September 2013), 123 days after the Lee Rigby murder.

7 Confidence intervals around a single point may be misleading since each point in a binned scatterplot represents the data from an entire interval of data (Starr and Goldfarb Reference Van Hauwaert and Huber2020). As such, we report both confidence intervals (at the mean within the bin) and confidence bands (across the whole bin).

8 Appendix C.1 provides the binned scatterplots for both risk perceptions and negative emotions based on a 120-day bandwidth.

9 Evidence of increased negative feelings in the wake of the 2013 Lee Rigby murder is also provided in Appendix A.3, based on a sentiment analysis of Twitter data.

10 Appendix C.2 provides suggestive evidence that this particular attack caused a more permanent shift in risk perceptions and negative emotions.

References

Balcells, L and Torrats-Espinosa, G (2018) Using a natural experiment to estimate the electoral consequences of terrorist attacks. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 115(42), 10624–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bove, V, Efthyvoulou, G and Pickard, H (2022) Did terrorism affect voting in the Brexit referendum? British Journal of Political Science 52(3), 1133–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bove, V, Efthyvoulou, G and Pickard, H (2023) Replication Data for: Are the effects of terrorism short-lived? https://doi.org/10.7910/DVN/YWWXEP, Harvard Dataverse, V1, UNF:6:BsL3Iuk/LY4ty1BdYIUVqA== [fileUNF]Google Scholar
Bozzoli, C and Müller, C (2011) Perceptions and attitudes following a terrorist shock: Evidence from the UK. European Journal of Political Economy 27, S89S106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brandon, SE and Silke, AP (2006) Near-and long-term psychological effects of exposure to terrorist attacks. In Bongar, B., Brown, LM, Beutner, LE, Breckenridge, JN, and Zimbardo, PG (eds), Psychology of Terrorism, Oxford University Press, 175–93.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Breton, C and Eady, G (2022) Does international terrorism affect public attitudes toward refugees? Evidence from a large-scale natural experiment. The Journal of Politics 84(1), 554559.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bux, S. M. and Coyne, S. M. (2009). The effects of terrorism: The aftermath of the London terror attacks. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 39(12), 29362966.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cattaneo, M.D., Crump, R.K., Farrell, M.H. and Feng, Y. (2019). On binscatter. arXiv preprint arXiv:1902.09608.Google Scholar
Davis, D. W. and Silver, B. D. (2004). Civil liberties vs. security: Public opinion in the context of the terrorist attacks on America. American Journal of Political Science, 48(1), 2846.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dinesen, P.T. and Jæger, M.M. (2013). The effect of terror on institutional trust: New evidence from the 3/11 Madrid terrorist attack. Political Psychology, 34(6), 917926.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Epifanio, M., Giani, M. and Ivandic, R. (2023). Wait and see? Public opinion dynamics after terrorist attacks. The Journal of Politics, 85(3), 843859CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Falcó-Gimeno, A, Muñoz, J and Pannico, R (2022) Double-edged bullets: The conditional effect of terrorism on vote for the incumbent. British Journal of Political Science 53(1), 183203.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ferrín, M, Mancosu, M and Cappiali, TM (2020) Terrorist attacks and Europeans’ attitudes towards immigrants: An experimental approach. European Journal of Political Research 59(3), 491516.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Germann, M, Godefroidt, A and Mendez, F (2022) Islamist Terrorism Continues to Affect Political Attitudes. Mimeo, University of Bath,1-22.Google Scholar
Godefroidt, A (2023) How terrorism does (and does not) affect citizens’ political attitudes: a meta-analysis. American Journal of Political Science 67(1), 2238.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hainmueller, J, Mummolo, J and Xu, Y (2019) How much should we trust estimates from multiplicative interaction models? Political Analysis 27(2), 163–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hansen, BT, Dinesen, PT and Østergaard, SD (2017) Increased incidence rate of trauma-and stressor-related disorders in Denmark after the Breivik attacks in Norway. Epidemiology 28(6), 906–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hansen, BT et al. (2016) Increased incidence rate of trauma-and stressor-related disorders in Denmark after the September 11, 2001, terrorist attacks in the United States. American Journal of Epidemiology 184(7), 494500.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Helbling, M and Meierrieks, D (2022) Terrorism and migration: An overview. British Journal of Political Science 52(2), 977–96.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holman, MR, Merolla, JL, Zechmeister, EJ et al. (2022) The curious case of Theresa May and the public that did not rally: Gendered reactions to terrorist attacks can cause slumps not bumps. American Political Science Review 116(1), 249–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huddy, L et al. (2005) Threat, anxiety, and support of antiterrorism policies. American Journal of Political Science 49(3), 593608.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Legewie, J (2013) Terrorist events and attitudes toward immigrants: A natural experiment. American Journal of Sociology 118(5), 11991245.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maguen, S, Papa, A and Litz, BT (2008) Coping with the threat of terrorism: A review. Anxiety, Stress, and Coping 21(1), 1535.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muñoz, J, Falcó-Gimeno, A and Hernández, E (2020) Unexpected event during survey design: Promise and pitfalls for causal inference. Political Analysis 28(2), 186206.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nussio, E, Bove, V and Steele, B (2019) The consequences of terrorism on migration attitudes across Europe. Political Geography 75, 102047.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nussio, E, Böhmelt, T and Bove, V (2021) Do terrorists get the attention they want? Comparing effects of terrorism across Europe. Public Opinion Quarterly 85(3), 900912.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pennebaker, JW and Harber, KD (1993) A social stage model of collective coping: The Loma Prieta earthquake and the Persian Gulf War. Journal of Social Issues 49(4), 125–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pickard, H, Efthyvoulou, G and Bove, V (2023) What's left after right-wing extremism? The effects on political orientation. European Journal of Political Research 62(1), 338–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shoshani, A and Slone, M (2008) The drama of media coverage of terrorism: Emotional and attitudinal impact on the audience. Studies in Conflict & Terrorism 31(7), 627–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sønderskov, KM et al. (2021) Terrorism in the country of origin is linked to deterioration in the mental health of refugees. Nature Human Behaviour 5(11), 1555–61.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Starr, E and Goldfarb, B (2020) Binned scatterplots: A simple tool to make research easier and better. Strategic Management Journal 41(12), 2261–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Hauwaert, SM and Huber, RA (2020) In-group solidarity or out-group hostility in response to terrorism in France? Evidence from a regression discontinuity design. European Journal of Political Research 59(4), 936–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. Risk of terror: non-parametric estimates.Notes: The upper panel shows the binned scatterplot and the corresponding confidence intervals and confidence bands (Cattaneo et al. 2019), implemented using the binsreg package. The lower panel shows the frequency of observations.

Figure 1

Figure 2. Risk of terror: main results.Notes: The left panel shows the treatment effect on public's perceptions about the risk of terrorism across the three time frames. Standard errors are clustered at the attack-by-region level. Fat (thin) lines signify the 90 per cent (95 per cent) confidence interval. The sample sizes are: N (very short run) = 4,186; N (short run) = 6,397; and N (medium run) = 13,870. The right panel shows the results from permutation tests that randomly shuffle the data 1,000 times, stratified by attack-by-region, and estimate a treatment effect for each random draw and each time frame. The reference lines show the observed effects, with labels reporting the proportion of times that the treatment effects under the permuted data are at least as extreme as under the observed data.

Figure 2

Figure 3. Negative emotions: main results.Notes: The figure shows the treatment effect on the outcome listed on the horizontal axis across the three time frames. Standard errors are clustered at the attack-by-region level. Fat (thin) lines signify the 90 per cent (95 per cent) confidence interval. The sample sizes are: N (very short run) = 4,350; N (short run) = 6,615; and N (medium run) = 14,314.

Figure 3

Figure 4. Risk of terror and negative emotions: results for each attack.Notes: See notes of Figs 2 and 3. Attack 1 = 2005 London bombings; Attack 2 = 2007 Glasgow airport attack; Attack 3 = 2013 Lee Rigby murder. The sample sizes for the left panel are: N1 (very short run) = 1,211; N1 (short run) = 2,511; N1 (medium run) = 5,138; N2 (very short run) = 1,156; N2 (short run) = 2,237; N2 (medium run) = 5,172; N3 (very short run) = 1,819; N3 (short run) = 1,649; N3 (medium run) = 3,560. Similar sample sizes are used in the right panel.

Figure 4

Figure 5. Risk of terror and negative emotions: foiled attacks.Notes: See notes of Figs 2 and 3. The sample sizes for the left panel are: N (very short run) = 3,139; N (short run) = 5,834; and N (medium run) = 10,971. Similar sample sizes are used in the right panel.

Supplementary material: File

Bove et al. supplementary material

Bove et al. supplementary material
Download Bove et al. supplementary material(File)
File 11 MB
Supplementary material: File

Bove_et_al._Dataset

Dataset

Download Bove_et_al._Dataset(File)
File