Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-s2hrs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-10T13:05:38.705Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Some experiments to determine the methods used in host-finding by the tsetse fly, Glossina medicorum Austen*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 July 2009

R. F. Chapman
Affiliation:
Birkbeck College, London.

Extract

Field and laboratory experiments were carried out in Ghana on the tsetse fly, Glossina medicorum Aust., to discover the senses used by the fly in finding its host. In laboratory experiments on vision a model locomotive with a black screen attached to it was used as a ‘host’. The flies were stimulated to move by the movement of the screen. When the screen was stopped and started for short periods the increase in activity became less each time the screen was put in motion but the fall off was less rapid than in experiments with continuous movement. The possible significance of this is discussed in the light of the normal habitat of the species in the field.

In the field, G. medicorum did not respond to a moving object more than 25 ft. away but G. morsitans Westw. showed a good response at distances up to 150 ft.

Tethered flies were attracted to the model screen and showed a preference for landing on the lower edge near the centre.

Laboratory experiments using a guineapig and a goat as sources of smell produced no response from the flies but a strong reaction was obtained with the vapour of dilute acetic acid. In field experiments, using cows which were not visible to the fly, a very marked response to the smell of the cows was obtained. Orientation to a source of smell probably involves orientation to air movement and G. medicorum showed a strong upwind orientation in a wind tunnel even when blinded.

It is concluded that G. medicorum, which lives in thicket or forest where visibility is poor, probably responds initially to the smell of a potential host. An upwind orientation would then bring it to the general vicinity of the animal and the final approach might be visual.

Type
Research Paper
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1961

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Barrass, R. (1960). The settling of tsetse flies Glossina morsitans Westwood (Diptera, Muscidae) on cloth screens.—Ent. exp. appl. 3 pp. 5967.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buxton, P. A. (1955). The natural history of tsetse flies.—Mem. Land. Sch. Hyg. trop. Med. no. 10, 816 pp. London, Lewis.Google Scholar
Chapman, R. F. (1960). A note on Glossina medicorum Austen (Diptera) in Ghana.—Bull. ent. Res. 51 pp. 435440.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hughes, J. C. (1957). Olfactory stimulation of tsetse flies and blowflies.—Bull. ent. Res. 48 pp. 561579.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jack, R. W. (1939). Studies in the physiology and behaviour of Glossina morsitans, Westw.—Mem. Dep. Agric. S. Rhod. no. 1, 203 pp.Google Scholar
Napier, Bax S. (1937). The senses of smell and sight in Glossina swynnertoni.—Bull. ent. Res. 28 pp. 539582.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nash, T. A. M. & Davey, J. T. (1950). The resting habits of Glossina medicorum, G. fusca and G. longipalpis.—Bull. ent. Res. 41 pp. 153157.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roeder, K. D.Ed. (1953). Insect physiology.—1100 pp. New York, Wiley; London, Chapman & Hall.Google Scholar
Vanderplank, F. L. (1944). Studies of the behaviour of the tsetse-fly (Glossina pallidipes) in the field: the attractiveness of various baits.—J. Anim. Ecol. 13 pp. 3948.CrossRefGoogle Scholar