Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-ndw9j Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-15T07:30:01.170Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The Great Illusion: Ignorance, Informational Cascades, and the Persistence of Unpopular Norms

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  23 January 2015

Extract

Norms of discrimination against women and blacks, norms of revenge still alive in some Mediterranean countries, and norms that everybody dislikes and tries to circumvent, such as the invisible norms of reciprocity that hold among the Iks studied by Turnbull, are all examples of unpopular and inefficient norms that often persist in spite of their being disliked as well as being obviously inefficient from a social or economic viewpoint. The world of business is not immune to this problem. In all those countries in which corruption is endemic, bribing public officials to get lucrative contracts is the norm, but it is often true that such a norm is disliked by many, and that it may lead to highly inefficient social outcomes (Bicchieri and Rovelli 1995).

From a functionalist viewpoint such norms are anomalous, since they do not seem to fulfill any beneficial role for society at large or even for the social groups involved in sustaining the norm. In many cases it would be possible to gain in efficiency by eliminating, say, norms of racial discrimination, in that it would be possible to increase the well-being of a racial minority without harming the rest of society. To social scientists who equate persistence with efficiency, the permanence of inefficient norms thus presents an anomaly. They rest their case on two claims: when a norm is inefficient, sooner or later this fact will become evident. And evidence of inefficiency will induce quick changes in the individual choices that sustain the norm. That is, no opportunity for social improvement remains unexploited for long. Unfortunately, all too often this is not the case, and this is not because people mistakenly believe inefficient norms to be good or efficient.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Society for Business Ethics 1999

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Allport, F. H. 1924. Social Psychology. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
Andersen, H. C. 1994. Fairy Tales. London: Penguin Books. First edition, 1835.Google Scholar
Asch, S. 1952. Social Psychology. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Bicchieri, C. 1990. “Norms of cooperation.” Ethics 100.Google Scholar
Bicchieri, C. and Rovelli, C. 1995. “Evolution and revolution: The dynamics of corruption.” Rationality and Society 7.Google Scholar
Bicchieri, C. and Duffy, J. 1997. “Corruption cycles.” Political Studies 1.Google Scholar
Bikhchandani, S.; Hirshleifer, D.; and Welch, I. 1992. “A theory of fads, fashion, custom, and cultural change as informational cascades.” Journal of Political Economy 100.Google Scholar
de Toque ville, A. 1955. The Old Regime and the French Revolution. New York: Doubleday. First edition, 1856.Google Scholar
Hirschman, A. 1970. Exit, Voice and Loyalty. Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Kauffman, K. 1981. “Prison officer attitudes and perceptions of attitudes.” Journal of Research in Crime Delinquency 18.Google Scholar
Latane, B. and Darley, J. M. 1968. “Group inhibition of bystander intervention.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 10.Google Scholar
Latane, B. and Darley, J. M. 1970. The unresponsive bystander: Why doesn’t he help? New York: Appleton Century Crofts.Google Scholar
McFarland, C. and Miller, D. T 1990. “Judgments of self-other similarity: Just like other people, only more so.” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 16.Google Scholar
Matza, D. 1964. Delinquency and drift. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Miller, D. T. and McFarland, C. 1987. “Pluralistic ignorance: When similarity is interpreted as dissimilarity.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 53.Google Scholar
Miller, D. T. and McFarland, C. 1991. “When social comparison goes awry: The case of pluralistic ignorance.” In Suis, J. and Wills, T., eds., Social Comparison: Contemporary Theory and Research. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Miller, D. T and Prentice, D. A. 1994. “Collective errors and errors about the collective.” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 20.Google Scholar
O’Gorman, H. J. 1975. “Pluralistic ignorance and White estimates of White support for racial segregation.” Public Opinion Quarterly 39.Google Scholar
Packard, J. S. and Willower, D. J. 1972. “Pluralistic ignorance and pupil control ideology.” Journal of Education Administration 10.Google Scholar
Piliavin, I. M. and Rodin, J. 1969. “Good samaritanism: an underground phenomenon?” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 13, no. 4.Google Scholar
Prentice, D. A. and Miller, D. T. 1996. “Pluralistic ignorance and the perpetuation of social norms by unwitting actors.” In Zanna, M., ed., Advances in Experimental Social Psychology. San Diego: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Schanck, R. L. 1932. “A study of community and its group institutions conceived of as behavior of individuals.” Psychological Monographs 43, no. 2.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwartz, S. H. and Clausen, G. T 1970. “Responsibility, norms, and helping in an emergency.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 16, no. 2.Google Scholar
Tajfel, H. 1981. Human Groups and Social Categories. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Turnbull, C. 1972. The Mountain People. New York: Simon and Schuster.Google Scholar
Wheeler, S. 1961. “Role conflict in correctional communities.” In Cressey, D. R., ed., The Prison: Studies in Institutional Organization and Change. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.Google Scholar
Wuthnow, R. 1991. Acts of Compassion: Caring for Others and Helping Ourselves. Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar