Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-dh8gc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-15T10:46:20.789Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Sociolinguistics and the Language Faculty

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  27 June 2016

J.K. Chambers*
Affiliation:
University of Toronto

Abstract

The discovery that some processes are primitive rather than learned provides a window into the properties of the language faculty. One of the postulated modules of the language faculty is sociolinguistic competence, the ability to use language appropriately in its socio-cultural nexus. I present three cases where variable phenomena exhibit regularities that appear to be essential rather than accidental. First is the Ethan Experience, which inures children engaged in acquiring the community accent from learning foreign features that must later be unlearned. The experience entails an innate accent-filter, and its influence has been observed in all situations in which the accent being acquired differs from the home accent. Second is Sex-Based Variability, whereby women use fewer non-standard variants and have wider stylistic repertoires than men of the same social class in the same settings. Third is Vernacular Roots, the discovery that a number of sociolinguistic variables recur in English vernaculars wherever they have been studied, as well as in interlanguage, Creoles, and child language. The ubiquity of these features presumably reflects their privileged status in the language faculty.

Résumé

Résumé

La découverte qu’il existe des processus qui sont primitifs plutôt qu’appris nous donne des indices par rapport aux propriétés de la faculté du langage. Un des modules postulés de la faculté du langage est la compétence sociolinguistique, c’est-à-dire la capacité de pouvoir utiliser le langage de façon appropriée dans son contexte socioculturel. Je discute trois cas où des phénomènes variables présentent des régularités qui semblent être de nature essentielle plutôt qu’ accidentelle. Le premier est l’expérience Ethan, qui fait que les enfants qui sont en train d’acquérir l’accent de leur communauté n’apprennent pas des traits étrangers qui devront par après être délaissés. Cette expérience implique un filtre d’accent inné, et son influence a été observé dans toutes les situations où l’accent acquis diffère de l’accent domiciliaire. Deuxièmement est le cas de variabilité basée sur le sexe, où les femmes utilisent moins de variantes non standards et ont des répertoires stylistiques plus répandus que les hommes de la même classe sociale dans les mêmes contextes. Troisièmement est le cas des racines vernaculaires, qui est la découverte qu’un ensemble restreint de variables sociolinguistiques réapparaissent dans les vernaculaires anglais partout où ils ont été étudiés, ainsi que dans l’interlangue, les créoles et le parler des enfants. L’ubiquité de ces traits reflète en toute probabilité leur statut privilégié dans la faculté du langage.

Type
Part II: Language in Time and Space
Copyright
Copyright © Canadian Linguistic Association 2005

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bickerton, Derek. 1984. The language bioprogram hypothesis. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences 7:173187.Google Scholar
Braine, Martin D.S. 1974. On what might constitute learnable phonology. Language 50:270299.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chambers, J.K. 2002. Dynamics of dialect convergence. In Journal of Sociolinguistics 6: Investigating Change and Variation through Dialect Contact, ed. Milroy, Lesley, 117130.Google Scholar
Chambers, J.K. 2003a. Sociolinguistic theory: Linguistic variation and its social significance. 2nd ed. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Chambers, J.K. 2003b. The sociolinguistics of immigration. In Social dialectology, ed. Britain, David and Cheshire, Jenny, 97113. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Chambers, J.K. 2004. Dynamic typology and vernacular universals. In Dialectology meets typology: Dialect grammar from a cross-linguistic perspective, ed. Kortmann, Bernd, 127145. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Chambers, J.K. To appear. Canadian Raising retrospect and prospect. In Canadian Journal of Linguistics 51 : Canadian English in a Global Context, ed. Avery, Peter, D’Arcy, Alexandra, Gold, Elaine, and Rice, Keren.Google Scholar
Chomsky, Noam. 1965. Aspects of the theory of syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Chomsky, Noam. 1968. Language and mind. New York: Harcourt, Brace and World.Google Scholar
Chomsky, Noam. 1980. Rules and representations. New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Hazen, Kirk. 2002. The family. In The handbook of language variation and change, ed. Chambers, J.K., Trudgill, Peter, and Schilling-Estes, Natalie, 500525. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Hymes, Dell. 1974. Foundations in sociolinguistics: An ethnographic approach. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.Google Scholar
Jakobson, Roman. 1963. Implications of language universals for linguistics. In Universals of language, ed. Greenberg, Joseph H., 263278. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Kroch, Anthony S. 1978. Toward a theory of social dialect variation. Language in Society 7:1736.Google Scholar
Labov, William. 1966a. The social stratification of English in New York City. Washington DC: Center for Applied Linguistics.Google Scholar
Labov, William. 1966b. The linguistic variable as a structural unit. Washington Linguistics Review 3:422.Google Scholar
Milroy, James, and Milroy, Lesley. 1978. Belfast: Change and variation in an urban vernacular. In Sociolinguistic patterns in British English, ed. Trudgill, Peter, 1936. London: Edward Arnold.Google Scholar
Pinker, Steven, and Prince, Alan. 1999. The nature of human concepts: Evidence from an unusual source. In Language, logic and concepts: Essays in memory of John Macna-mara, ed. Jackendoff, Ray, Bloom, Paul, and Wynn, Karen, 221261. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sapir, Edward. 1929. The status of linguistics as a science. Language 5:207214.Google Scholar
Schoght, H.G. 1961. La notion de loi dans la phonétique historique. Lingua 10:7292. Quoted in Kroch 1978:18, fn.Google Scholar
Schreier, Daniel. 2002. Past be in Tristan da Cunha: The rise and fall of categoricality in language change. American Speech 77:7099.Google Scholar
Smith, William H. 1851. Canada: Past, present and future, being a historical, geographical, geological and statistical account of Canada West. Vol. 2. Toronto: Thomas Maclean Google Scholar
Stampe, David. 1969. The acquisition of phonetic representation. In Papers from the Fifth Regional Meeting of the Chicago Linguistic Society, ed. Binnick, Robert, Davison, Alice, Green, Georgia, and Morgan, Jerry, 443454. Chicago: Chicago Linguistic Society.Google Scholar
Wolfram, Walter A. 1969. A sociolinguistic description of Detroit Negro speech. Washington, DC: Center for Applied Linguistics.Google Scholar
Wolfram, Walt, and Fasold, Ralph W.. 1974. The study of social dialects in American English. Englewood Cliffs NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar