Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-v9fdk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-15T08:43:57.742Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Syntactic Dependencies as Memorized Sequences in the Brain

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  27 June 2016

Yosef Grodzinsky*
Affiliation:
McGill University/Tel Aviv University

Abstract

The prospects of a cognitive neuroscience of syntax are considered with respect to functional neuroanatomy of two seemingly independent systems: Working Memory and syntactic representation and processing. It is proposed that these two systems are more closely related than previously supposed. In particular, it is claimed that a sentence with anaphoric dependencies involves several Working Memories, each entrusted with a different linguistic function. Components of Working Memory reside in the Left Inferior Frontal Gyrus, which is associated with Broca’s region. When lesioned, this area manifests comprehension disruptions in the ability to analyze intra-sentential dependencies, suggesting that Working Memory spans over syntactic computations. The unification of considerations regarding Working Memory with a purely syntactic approach to Broca’s regions leads to the conclusion that mechanisms that compute transformations—and no other syntactic relations—reside in this area.

Résumé

Résumé

La possibilité d’une neuroscience cognitive de la syntaxe est considérée par rapport à la neuroanatomie fonctionnelle de deux systèmes qui sont apparemment independents : la Mémoire de Travail et la repésentation syntaxique et son traitement. Il est proposé que ces deux systèmes sont plus apparentés qu’il ne l’a été supposé. En particulier, il est suggéré qu’une proposition avec des dépendances anaphoriques entraîne plusieurs Mémoires de Travail, chacune chargée avec une fonction linguistique différente. Les composants de la Mémoire de Travail résident dans la partie inférieure de la circonvolution frontale gauche qui est associée avec la région de Broca. Lorsque lésionnée, cette région manifeste des perturbations de compréhension dans la capacité d’analyser des dépendances à l’intérieur d’une proposition, suggérant que la Mémoire de Travail enjambe les computations syntaxiques. L’unification des considérations concernant la Mémoire de Travail avec une approche purement syntactique aux régions de Broca amène à conclure que les mécanismes qui font la computation de transformations—et nul autre relation syntaxique—résident dans cette région.

Type
Part III: Language and the Theory of Mind
Copyright
Copyright © Canadian Linguistic Association 2005

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Awh, E., Jonides, John, Smith, Edward E., Schumacher, E.H., Koeppe, Robert A., and Katz, S.. 1996. Dissociation of storage and rehearsal in verbal working memory: Evidence from PET. Psychological Science 7:2531.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Amunts, Katrin, Schleicher, Axel, Bürgel, Uli, Mohlberg, Harmut, Uylings, Harry B.M., and Zilles, Karl. 1999. Broca’s region revisited: Cytoarchitecture and intersubject variability. Journal of Comparative Neurology 412:319341.3.0.CO;2-7>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baddeley, Alan. 1986. Working memory. Oxford: Clarendon Press/Oxford University Press.Google ScholarPubMed
Balogh, Jennifer, and Grodzinsky, Yosef. 2000. Levels of linguistic representation in Broca’s aphasia: Implicitness and referentiality of arguments. In Grammatical disorders in aphasia: A neurolinguistic perspective, ed. Bastiaanse, Roelien and Grodzinsky, Yosef, 88104. London: Whurr.Google Scholar
Ben-Shachar, Michal, Hendler, Talma, Kahn, Itamar, Ben-Bashat, Dafna, and Grodzinsky, Yosef. 2003. The neural reality of syntactic transformations: Evidence from fMRI. Psychological Science 14:433440.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ben-Shachar, Michal, Palti, Dafna, and Grodzinsky, Yosef. 2004. Neural correlates of syntactic movement: Converging evidence from two fMRI experiments. Neurolmage 21:13201336.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blumstein, Sheila E., Goodglass, Harold, Statlender, Susan, and Biber, Carol. 1983. Comprehension strategies determining reference in aphasia: A study of reflexivization. Brain & Language 18:115127.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Braver, Todd S., Cohen, Johnathan D., Nystrom, Leigh E., Jonides, John, Smith, Edward E., and Noll, Douglas C.. 1997. A parametric study of prefrontal cortex involvement in human working memory. Neurolmage 5:4962.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carpenter, Patricia A., Just, Marcel Adam, and Reichle, Erik D.. 2000. Working memory and executive function: Evidence from neuroimaging. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 10:195199.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caplan, David. 2001. Functional neuroimaging studies of syntactic processing. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 30:297320.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chomsky, Carol. 1969. Acquisition of syntax in children from 5-10. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Chomsky, Noam. 2000. Derivation by phase. Ms., Massachussetts Institute of Technology.Google Scholar
Cohen, Johnathan D., Forman, Steven D., Braver, Todd S., Casey, B.J., Servan-Schreiber, David, and Noll, Douglas C.. 1994. Activation of the prefrontal cortex in a nonspatial working memory task with functional MRI. Human Brain Mapping 1:293304.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, Johnathan D., Perlstein, William M., Braver, Todd S., Nystrom, Leigh E., Noll, Douglas C., Jonides, John, and Smith, Edward E.. 1997. Temporal dynamics of brain activation during a working memory task. Nature 386:604–608.Google Scholar
Fiebach, Christian J., Schlesewsky, Matthias, and Friedend, Angela. 2002. Separating syntactic memory costs and syntactic integration costs during parsing: The processing of German WH-questions. Journal of Memory and Language 47:250272.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frazier, Lyn, Clifton, C., and Randall, J.. 1983. Filling gaps. Cognition 13:187222.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Friederici, Angela, Rueschemeyer, Shirley-Ann, Hahne, Anja, and Fiebach, Christian J.. 2003. The role of left inferior frontal and superior temporal cortex in sentence comprehension: Localizing syntactic and semantic processes. Cerebral Cortex 13:170177.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friedmann, Na’ama, and Gvion, Aviah. 2003. Sentence comprehension and working memory limitation in aphasia: A dissociation between semantic-syntactic and phonological reactivation. Brain & Language 86:2339.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gibson, Edward. 1998. Linguistic complexity: Locality of syntactic dependencies. Cognition 68:176.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grimshaw, Jan, and Rosen, Sara Thomas. 1990. Knowledge and obedience: The developmental status of the binding theory. Linguistic Inquiry 21:187222.Google Scholar
Grodzinsky, Yosef. 1984. Language deficits and linguistic theory. Doctoral dissertation, Brandeis University.Google Scholar
Grodzinsky, Yosef. 1986. Language deficits and the theory of syntax. Brain & Language 27:135159.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grodzinsky, Yosef. 1989. Agrammatic comprehension of relative clauses. Brain & Language 37:480499.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grodzinsky, Yosef. 1995. A restrictive theory of trace deletion in agrammatism. Brain & Language 51:2651.Google Scholar
Grodzinsky, Yosef. 2000. The neurology of syntax. Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 23:1–71.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grodzinsky, Yosef, and Finkel, Lisa. 1998. The neurology of empty categories. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 10:281292.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grodzinsky, Yosef, and Kave, Gitit. 1994. Do children really know Condition A? Language Acquisition 3:4154.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grodzinsky, Yosef, Wexler, Kenneth, Chien, Yu-Chin, Marakovitz, Susan, and Solomon, Julie. 1993. The breakdown of binding relations. Brain & Language 45:396422.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jonides, John, Schumacher, E.H., Smith, Edward E., Lauber, E.J., Awh, E., Satosy, M., and Koeppe, Robert A.. 1997. Verbal working memory load effect regional brain activation as measured by PET. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 9:462475.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Just, Marcel Adam, Carpenter, Patricia A., Keller, Timothy A., Eddy, William F., and Thulborn, Keith R.. 1996. Brain activation modulated by sentence comprehension. Science 274:114116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Linebarger, Marcia C., Schwartz, Myrna F., and Saffran, Eleanor M.. 1983. Syntactic processing in agrammatism: A reply to Zurif and Grodzinsky. Cognition 15:215225.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lukatela, K., Crain, S., and Shankweiler, D.. 1988. Sensitivity to inflectional morphology in agrammatism: Investigations of a highly inflected language. Brain & Language 33:115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mikelic, S., Boskovic, Z., Crain, S., and Shankweiler, D.. 1995. Comprehension of nonlexical categories in agrammatism. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 24:299311.Google Scholar
Rizzi, Luigi. 1990. Relativized minimality. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Roder, Brigitte, Stock, Oliver, Neville, Helen, Bien, S., and Rosier, Frank. 2001. Brain activation modulated by the comprehension of normal and pseudo-word sentences of different processing demands: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Neurolmage 15:10031014.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Santi, Andrea, and Grodzinsky, Yosef. 2004. Dissociating syntactic movement from reflexive binding with fMRI. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience supplement 16:394395.Google Scholar
Santi, Andrea, and Grodzinsky, Yosef. To appear. Taxing working memory with syntax: Bi-hemispheric Modulations.Google Scholar
Schwartz, Myrna F., Linebarger, Marcia C., Saffran, Eleanor M., and Pate, Debra. 1987. Syntactic transparency and sentence interpretation in aphasia. Language and Cognitive Processes 2:85113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shankweiler, D., Crain, S., Gorrell, P., and Tuller, B.. 1989. Reception of language in Broca’s aphasia. Language & Cognitive Processes 4:133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, Edward E., and Jonides, John. 1999. Storage and executive processes in the frontal lobes. Science 283:16571661.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, Edward E., and Geva, A.. 2000. Working memory and its connection to language processing. In Language and the brain: Representation and processing, ed. Grodzinsky, Yosef, Shapiro, L., and Swinney, D., 123141. San Diego: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar