Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gxg78 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-25T16:31:12.026Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Everyday Social Support for Health Behaviours in Older Adults during Times of Challenge: Evidence from Daily Life Assessments

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  06 December 2024

Elizabeth Zambrano Garza
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, The University of British Columbia, Vancouver, Canada
Theresa Pauly
Affiliation:
Department of Gerontology, Simon Fraser University, Vancouver, Canada
Yoonseok Choi
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, The University of British Columbia, Vancouver, Canada
Rachel A. Murphy
Affiliation:
School of Population and Public Health, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, Canada Cancer Control Research, BC Cancer, Vancouver, Canada
Wolfgang Linden
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, The University of British Columbia, Vancouver, Canada
Maureen C. Ashe
Affiliation:
Department of Family Practice, The University of British Columbia, Vancouver, Canada
Kenneth M. Madden
Affiliation:
Department of Medicine, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, Canada
Jennifer M. Jakobi
Affiliation:
School of Health and Exercise Sciences, University of British Columbia, Okanagan, Canada
Denis Gerstorf
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Humboldt University, Berlin, Germany
Christiane A. Hoppmann*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, The University of British Columbia, Vancouver, Canada
*
Corresponding author: Christiane Hoppmann, Email: choppmann@psych.ubc.ca
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

The COVID-19 pandemic challenged older adults’ health behaviours, making it even more difficult to engage in healthy diets and physical activity than it had been prepandemic. A resource to promote these could be social support. This study uses data from 136 older adults (Mage = 71.39 years, SD = 5.15, range: 63–87) who reported their daily fruit and vegetable consumption, steps, and health-behaviour-specific support from a close other every evening for up to 10 consecutive days. Findings show that on days when participants reported more emotional support than usual, fruit and vegetable consumption and step counts were higher. Daily instrumental support was positively associated with step counts, only. Participants receiving more overall emotional support across the study period consumed more fruit and vegetables; no parallel person-level association was found for overall steps. There were no significant interactions between dyad type and support links for our outcomes.

Résumé

Résumé

La pandémie de COVID-19 a perturbé les habitudes des personnes âgées, et entraîné pour eux plus de difficultés qu’auparavant à adhérer à un régime alimentaire sain et à des activités physiques. Le soutien social peut être une ressource pour promouvoir ces habitudes de vie. Cette étude puise dans les données recueillies auprès de 136 personnes âgées (âge moyen = 71,39 ans, écart-type = 5,15; fourchette = 63–87 ans), qui ont rendu compte chaque soir jusqu’à dix jours consécutifs de leur consommation de fruits et de légumes, de leur nombre de pas et du soutien à leurs habitudes de vie saine qu’ils ont reçu de la part d’un proche. Les résultats montrent que les jours où les participants ont déclaré avoir reçu davantage de soutien affectif que d’habitude, leur consommation de fruits et légumes et leur nombre de pas étaient plus élevés. Le soutien instrumental quotidien était seulement associé à de plus grands nombres de pas. Les participants qui ont reçu davantage de soutien affectif tout au long de la période de l’étude ont consommé davantage de fruits et légumes; aucune association parallèle n’a été établie à l’échelle personnelle pour le nombre de pas total. Nos résultats n’ont révélé aucune interaction significative entre le type de dyade et les liens de soutien.

Type
Article
Copyright
© Canadian Association on Gerontology 2024

Introduction

Older adults have a high chronic disease risk including diabetes and cardiovascular conditions (Fiest et al., Reference Fiest, Currie, Williams and Wang2011; Halter et al., Reference Halter, Musi, McFarland Horne, Crandall, Goldberg, Harkless, Hazzard, Huang, Kirkman, Plutzky, Schmader, Zieman and High2014). Some risk factors, such as age, cannot be changed; others are modifiable. Specifically, engaging in health promoting behaviours like healthy nutrition and physical activity reduces chronic disease risk, helps manage chronic disease, and improves quality of life (Biddle et al., Reference Biddle, Fox and Boutcher2000; Blanchflower et al., Reference Blanchflower, Oswald and Stewart-Brown2013; Govindaraju et al., Reference Govindaraju, Sahle, McCaffrey, McNeil and Owen2018; He et al., Reference He, Nowson and MacGregor2006). Yet, many older adults do not consume sufficient fruit and vegetables, and they engage in too little physical activity (Colley et al., Reference Colley, Garriguet, Janssen, Craig, Clarke and Tremblay2011; Riediger & Moghadasian, Reference Riediger and Moghadasian2008). These two health behaviours became even more challenging for older adults during the pandemic (e.g. limited access to recreational centres and a need to be extra cautious about in person contact as is the case in supermarkets due to an elevated risk of complications from COVID-19; Applegate & Ouslander, Reference Applegate and Ouslander2020; Carr, Reference Carr2021; Flanagan et al., Reference Flanagan, Beyl, Fearnbach, Altazan, Martin and Redman2021; Ruiz et al., Reference Ruiz, Devonport, Chen-Wilson, Nicholls, Cagas, Fernandez-Montalvo, Choi and Robazza2021). Nonetheless, social resources, including social support or assistance from a network member, have been associated with increased health behaviours (Berli et al., Reference Berli, Schwaninger and Scholz2021; Bloom et al., Reference Bloom, Edwards, Jameson, Syddall, Dennison, Gale, Baird, Cooper, Aihie Sayer and Robinson2017a; Rugel & Carpiano, Reference Rugel and Carpiano2015). Thus, it was important to investigate social support for health behaviours in older adults during the pandemic. Doing so using daily diary methodology generates ecologically valid evidence on the health promoting role of social support by capturing the underlying processes as they occur in older adults’ everyday lives.

Everyday social support for health behaviours

There is an increasing recognition that health behaviours occur in a social context and that they are shaped by social others (Conklin et al., Reference Conklin, Forouhi, Surtees, Khaw, Wareham and Monsivais2014; Khan et al., Reference Khan, Stephens, Franks, Rook and Salem2013; Martire et al., Reference Martire, Stephens, Mogle, Schulz, Brach and Keefe2013; Pietromonaco & Collins, Reference Pietromonaco and Collins2017). For example, many people find it more enjoyable to share a meal with a partner or go on a walk with a friend than engaging in these behaviours alone (Chong et al., Reference Chong, Doyle, Cyarto, Cox, Ellis, Ames, Lautenschlager and Group2014; Fulkerson et al., Reference Fulkerson, Larson, Horning and Neumark-Sztainer2014). However, the pandemic created barriers to doing so. Evidence shows that physical activity went down and sedentary time went up relative to prepandemic times (Flanagan et al., Reference Flanagan, Beyl, Fearnbach, Altazan, Martin and Redman2021; Ruiz et al., Reference Ruiz, Devonport, Chen-Wilson, Nicholls, Cagas, Fernandez-Montalvo, Choi and Robazza2021). Although social distancing changed the way people interacted during the pandemic, social relationships continued to be an important source of joy and comfort (Whitehead & Torossian, Reference Whitehead and Torossian2021). Social support may have been a key factor for overcoming barriers to health behaviours. For instance, an older adult with an adult child may have asked them to help with grocery shopping to avoid exposure to the virus. Or, an older adult may have asked a friend to go on a socially distant, joint walk to be physically active without risking exposure to the virus.

Social support has been associated with higher fruit and vegetable consumption, a key aspect of a healthy diet, in older adults prepandemic using cross-sectional, longitudinal, as well as daily diary methodologies (Bloom et al., Reference Bloom, Edwards, Jameson, Syddall, Dennison, Gale, Baird, Cooper, Aihie Sayer and Robinson2017a; Reyes Fernández et al., Reference Reyes Fernández, Warner, Knoll, Montenegro Montenegro and Schwarzer2015; Romero-Ortuno et al., Reference Romero-Ortuno, Casey, Cunningham, Squires, Prendergast, Kenny and Lawlor2011; Rugel & Carpiano, Reference Rugel and Carpiano2015; Shaikh et al., Reference Shaikh, Yaroch, Nebeling, Yeh and Resnicow2008). Social support can take different forms, including instrumental (e.g., hands-on, tangible support) and emotional support (e.g., encouragement; Barrera, Reference Barrera1986; Coyne & DeLongis, Reference Coyne and DeLongis1986). Instrumental social support has been positively associated with elevated consumption of fruit and vegetables; older adults may have more access to fruit and vegetables if receiving tangible support, for instance through support with grocery shopping or cooking (Bloom et al., 2017; Stephens et al., Reference Stephens, Franks, Rook, Iida, Hemphill and Salem2013). Emotional support, which can involve behaviours such as appreciating one’s efforts to maintain a healthy diet, has also been found to be associated with higher engagement in this health behaviour (Vesnaver & Keller, Reference Vesnaver and Keller2011). A review of over 35 studies, the majority cross-sectional, found that general social support, including emotional encouragement, was one of the strongest psychosocial predictors of fruit and vegetable intake (Shaikh et al., Reference Shaikh, Yaroch, Nebeling, Yeh and Resnicow2008). Longitudinally, emotional and instrumental support have also been associated with a low-fat diet over a 12-month period (Scholz et al., Reference Scholz, Ochsner, Hornung and Knoll2013). Finally, a daily diary study with 129 heterosexual older adult couples living with diabetes found that a combination of different types of support (including general emotional and instrumental) was associated with increases in adherence to a healthy diet. Specifically, on days when spouses provided more support than usual, individuals living with diabetes better adhered to a healthy diet that day than the prior day (Stephens et al., Reference Stephens, Franks, Rook, Iida, Hemphill and Salem2013).

More social support has also been associated with more physical activity, defined as bodily movement requiring energy expenditure (Berli et al., Reference Berli, Bolger, Shrout, Stadler and Scholz2018; Khan et al., Reference Khan, Stephens, Franks, Rook and Salem2013; Martire et al., Reference Martire, Stephens, Mogle, Schulz, Brach and Keefe2013; World Health Organization, 2022). Many studies focusing on physical activity target moderate to vigorous physical activity using activity monitors or self-reported questionnaires (Craig et al., Reference Craig, Marshall, Sjöström, Bauman, Booth, Ainsworth, Pratt, Ekelund, Yngve, Sallis and Oja2003; Rodrigues et al., Reference Rodrigues, Encantado, Carraça, Sousa-Sá, Lopes, Cliff, Mendes, Silva, Godinho and Santos2022; Schrack et al., Reference Schrack, Cooper, Koster, Shiroma, Murabito, Rejeski, Ferrucci and Harris2016). Given that older adults engage in relatively less vigorous physical activity (and in more leisure time physical activity; Davis & Fox, Reference Davis and Fox2007), it has been proposed that steps may be better suited to quantify physical activity in this age group (Grimm et al., Reference Grimm, Swartz, Hart, Miller and Strath2012). A review of 27 papers revealed that both more emotional and more instrumental support were associated with more physical activity levels in healthy, older adults, though the support was not assessed in a health behaviour-specific way (Lindsay Smith et al., Reference Lindsay Smith, Banting, Eime, O’Sullivan and van Uffelen2017). Daily diary studies with young adult samples show that on days when more general support (using composites of emotional and tangible support) was reported, more minutes of accelerometry-based physical activity were recorded (Berli et al., Reference Berli, Bolger, Shrout, Stadler and Scholz2018, Reference Berli, Schwaninger and Scholz2021). Finally, specific to older adults, a daily diary study with 70 older adults living with the effects of diabetes and their partners found that when participants received high levels of general support from their partners, energy expenditure measured via activity monitors was higher on the following day (Khan et al., Reference Khan, Stephens, Franks, Rook and Salem2013).

Sources of social support for health behaviours

Most research on social support in adulthood focuses on support from spouses (Berli et al., Reference Berli, Bolger, Shrout, Stadler and Scholz2018; DeLongis et al., Reference DeLongis, Capreol, Holtzman, O’Brien and Campbell2004; Franks et al., Reference Franks, Stephens, Rook, Franklin, Keteyian and Artinian2006; Martire et al., Reference Martire, Stephens, Mogle, Schulz, Brach and Keefe2013; Rook & Ituarte, Reference Rook and Ituarte1999; Stephens et al., Reference Stephens, Franks, Rook, Iida, Hemphill and Salem2013). Certainly, spouses often are the first line of defence; however, it may be important to investigate the extent to which social support findings from spouses generalize to other close relationship types. This is important given that social relationships in old age are diverse for a variety of reasons including that some older adults do not (or no longer) have a spouse to turn to (Brown & Lin, Reference Brown and Lin2012; Rook & Charles, Reference Rook and Charles2017). There is also significant evidence that nonkin ties are important sources of social support for older adults (Blieszner, Reference Blieszner2009; Suanet et al., Reference Suanet, van Tilburg and Broese van Groenou2013).

In line with tenets from the Convoy Model which posits that individuals move through life surrounded by a convoy of social relationships of varying closeness that could provide support, we assume that social support can come from different types of relationships (Antonucci et al., Reference Antonucci, Ajrouch and Birditt2014; Antonucci & Akiyama, Reference Antonucci and Akiyama1987). There is evidence to suggest that support from friends and family can both benefit health behaviours (Conklin et al., Reference Conklin, Forouhi, Surtees, Khaw, Wareham and Monsivais2014; Lindsay Smith et al., Reference Lindsay Smith, Banting, Eime, O’Sullivan and van Uffelen2017). In fact, it may be more important to consider the quality of support than the type of relationship it is coming from. Doing so opens the door to a broader range of possible support partners. The present study builds on this literature and extends it by recruiting older adults with a close other not limiting close others to any particular relationship type (e.g. spouses, friends, siblings, grandchild); we take into account the possible moderating role of type of dyad on support – health behaviour associations.

Present study

The present study shed light on how older adults may benefit from instrumental and emotional support for two health behaviours during times of challenge. Specifically, we asked participants about their instrumental and emotional support for both health behaviours. This level of granularity allows us to apply findings specifically to the two targeted health behaviours. Furthermore, daily questionnaires allowed us to capture processes as they occurred in individuals’ own environments, maximizing ecological validity and speaking to what distinguished a good day from a bad day (Bolger et al., Reference Bolger, Davis and Rafaeli2003; Hoppmann & Riediger, Reference Hoppmann and Riediger2009). We expected that on days when older adults reported receiving more health-behaviour-specific emotional and instrumental support, more fruit and vegetables were consumed, and more daily steps taken. Likewise, we hypothesized that more overall health-behaviour-specific emotional and instrumental support would be positively associated with overall levels of fruit and vegetable intake and step counts across the study period. We also examined the role of dyad type on health behaviours exploring if the expected time-varying associations between social support and health behaviours would differ by type of dyad (e.g., spouses and nonspouses). Finally, we also took into consideration the day of study, both partners’ age and gender (Inglehart, Reference Inglehart2002; Zacher & Rudolph, Reference Zacher and Rudolph2021) as well as changing pandemic circumstances (participation date).

Methods

This study was part of a larger project ‘Allies in Health during COVID-19’ (Zambrano Garza et al., Reference Zambrano Garza, Pauly, Choi, Murphy, Linden, Ashe, Madden, Jakobi, Gerstorf and Hoppmann2024, Zambrano Garza et al., Reference Zambrano Garza, Pauly, Choi, Murphy, Linden, Ashe, Madden, Jakobi, Gerstorf and Hoppmannaccepted). The study examined older adults’ socio-motivational resources for health behaviours and well-being during the pandemic.

Participants

The sample consisted of 136 Canadian older adults (M age = 71.39 years, SD = 5.15 range: 63–87, 87% White, 52% women, 71% some university education) who participated with a close other of their choice (study partner: M = 61.42 years, SD = 16.96, range: 18–84 years, 83% White, 72% women, 71% some university education). Most study partners were spouses but about a third of the sample was not (81 spouses, 18 friends, 7 siblings, 11 parent-child, 4 grandparent-grandchild, 14 other family member, 1 other). Participants were recruited using online platforms and social media (e.g., Craigslist). Senior and community organizations (e.g., community centres, libraries) across Canada helped disseminate study information. Advertisement materials included a link to the study’s website with more information and the laboratory’s phone number. Participants had to be able to communicate verbally, read newspaper-sized print, and have access to a computer or mobile device with internet connection. Close others needed to be 18 years or older, invested in the participant’s health, close to the participant, and a potential source of support. Participation was contingent on the participation of the study partner due to the focus on social relationships. A total of 261 individuals reached out to the laboratory expressing interest in the study. Because the study was dyadic in nature, we needed to schedule information calls and screen not just the individual who had made first contact, but also their potential study partner, as per our ethics protocol. Consequently, the number of individuals screened exceeded the number of individuals reaching out. The most common reasons for nonparticipation (N = 110) were lack of an eligible partner, time constraints of either the older adult or their study partner, or inability to schedule a phone screening. This process resulted in 151 dyads composed of 302 individuals who were both interested in participating and eligible. Of these 151 dyads, seven dyads were scheduled but withdrew before participation due to time constraints. Of the 144 dyads who started the study, one dyad did not finish the baseline questionnaire and another dyad did not start the daily diary period. Two dyads did not report their age and were therefore excluded from analyses (due to the focus on older adults), resulting in 140 dyads. As this article targeted older adults, if both dyad members were over 65 years old, one of them was randomly excluded. We then excluded participants who submitted 1 daily diary or less (n = 1) and who did not have data on measures central to the current study (n = 3), leaving us with 136 individuals. For the physical activity models, 3 participants were excluded due to missing outcome data. If participants were missing data for control variables, they were estimated using the mean (one observation for partner’s age) or median for binary variables (73 instances for missing data on whether activity monitor was used to report step counts and 2 for university education). Each participant received a $50 Amazon gift card as a token of appreciation. The study received approval by the Behavioural Research Ethics Board at the University of British Columbia, Vancouver, Canada (H20-01645), and participants provided informed consent prior to commencing the study.

Procedures

The study was conducted between June 2020 and June 2021. After a phone screening, dyads participated in a Zoom meeting with a research assistant who introduced the study details and trained them on study procedures including how to determine portion sizes (e.g., one serving of fruit being half a cup). After this meeting, participants were asked to provide information about their background, social relationships, individual difference measures, health, and well-being in an online questionnaire. After both study partners completed this questionnaire, they simultaneously started the repeated daily life assessment component of this study. Participants completed two online questionnaires per day, one in the morning and one in the evening, for up to 10 consecutive days. For the purpose of this study, only the evening questionnaires are used which is when the targeted constructs were assessed. The final sample completed an average of 92.6% of their evening questionnaires (SD = 1.46, range: 2–10).

Measures

Emotional support. Every evening, participants answered “To what extent did your study partner show appreciation for your efforts to stay on track with eating fruit and veggies today?” on a scale of 0 (“not at all”) to 100 (“very much”) and “To what extent did your study partner show appreciation for your efforts to stay on track with being physically active today?” on a scale of 0 (“not at all”) to 100 (“very much”).

Instrumental support. Every evening, participants answered “To what extent did your study partner do something to help you stay on track with eating fruit and veggies today?” on a scale of 0 (“not at all”) to 100 (“very much”) and “To what extent did your study partner do something to help you stay on track with being physically active today?” on a scale of 0 (“not at all”) to 100 (“very much”).

Servings of fruit and vegetables. Every evening, participants reported how many servings of fruit and vegetables they consumed that day.

Daily steps. Every evening, participants reported how many steps they took that day. If participants owned a smartphone or fitness tracking device, they were asked to report this number at the end of the day. If they did not own one (65%), they were asked to estimate their steps, with the example of a brisk walk being about 100 steps/minute (Tudor-Locke et al., Reference Tudor-Locke, Han, Aguiar, Barreira, Kang and Rowe2018). Datapoints representing less than 300 steps per day were excluded (Tudor-Locke et al., Reference Tudor-Locke, Schuna, Barreira, Mire, Broyles, Katzmarzyk and Johnson2013).

Dyad type. Participants were asked to indicate their relationship with their study partner. Most dyads were spouses (81 dyads, Figure 1); a dyad variable was created based on whether study partners were spouses (1) or not (0).

Figure 1. Dyad types within the sample by percentages.

Covariates. Gender (0 = man, 1 = woman) and age of both dyad members, day of study, and start date to account for time-varying COVID-19 restrictions. For the physical activity models, we also included whether the participant’s step count came from a smartphone or fitness watch (0 = no, 1 = yes) and participants’ self-rated health.Footnote 1, Footnote 2, Footnote 3

Statistical analyses

Due to the hierarchically nested nature of the data, data were modelled at two levels: day and person. We examined the origins of variability for fruit and vegetable consumption (30% at the day and 70% at the person level) as well as steps (49% at the day level and 51% at the person level). For all analyses, age was grand-mean centred. Gender, dyad type, and step count source were dichotomous variables. Participation start date was centred to the study start date (range 0–366). All day-level variables were separated into within-person effects (person-centred) and between-person effects (grand-mean centred person means). Within-person effects (e.g., participant reported daily emotional support) capture intraindividual differences whereas between-person effects (e.g., overall emotional support) examine interindividual differences. Due to differences in scales in predictors and outcome for the fruit and vegetable models, support variables were rescaled for the models (divided by 10). Multilevel models using restricted maximum likelihood estimation were used. Models included a random intercept and random slopes for day-level support variables (emotional support within-person and instrumental support within-person). A simulation-based power analysis (Arend & Schäfer, Reference Arend and Schäfer2019) indicated our sample size allowed us to detect small main effects at Level 1 (minimum detectable effect size (MDES) = .13; see Table 5 in Arend & Schäfer, Reference Arend and Schäfer2019), Level 2 (MDES = .36; see Table 6 in Arend & Schäfer, Reference Arend and Schäfer2019), and cross-level interactions based on medium random slope variance (MDES = .91; see Table 7 in Arend & Schäfer, Reference Arend and Schäfer2019) with 80% power. We used R package lme4 (Bates et al., Reference Bates, Mächler, Bolker and Walker2015; see supplemental materials for code).

Results

Descriptive statistics and correlations

Table 1 shows descriptive statistics and Tables 2 and 3 show correlations between the main study variables and daily fruit and vegetable consumption as well as daily steps. Emotional support for fruit and vegetable consumption had an average of 26.78 (SD = 35.95), for physical activity the average was 26.90 (SD = 35.02). Instrumental support for fruit and vegetable consumption had an average of 24.33 (SD = 34.58) whereas for physical activity the average was 22.56 (SD = 33.44). When differentiating between dyad types (Table 1S), emotional support for fruit and vegetable consumption in spouses had an average endorsement of 33.31 (SD = 37.82); for nonspouses, it was 16.71 (SD = 30.23), and instrumental support for fruit and vegetables in spouses had an average 32.42 (SD = 37.59); for nonspouses, it was 11.60 (SD = 24.37). For physical activity, emotional support in spouses had an average score of 31.92 (SD = 36.10) and in nonspouses it was 18.93 (SD = 31.68).Footnote 4 Further descriptives are reported in Table 1S in the Supplemental Document, including dyad types and gender differences. On average, participants reported eating 5.46 servings of fruits and vegetables per day (SD = 2.61, range: 0–18). On average, participants reported taking 6,446.81 steps per day (SD = 4,635.91, range: 300–27,884Footnote 5). Participants had an average self-rated health of 3.66 (range: lowest 1–5 highest).

Table 1. Means and standard deviations.

Table 2. Correlations with fruit and vegetable consumption (N = 136)

Note: M and SD are used to represent mean and standard deviation, respectively. *indicates p <.05. **indicates p <.01.

Table 3. Correlations with self-reported physical activity (N = 133)

Note: M and SD are used to represent mean and standard deviation, respectively. *indicates p <.05. **indicates p <.01

Table 2 and 3 show intercorrelations. Older participants reported eating fewer fruit and vegetables (r = −.08, p < .01). Individuals with older study partners consumed more fruits and vegetables (r = .16, p < .01) and received more emotional support (r = .09, p < .01) and instrumental support (r = .12, p < .01) for their fruit and vegetable consumption. Individuals who received more emotional (r = .21, p < .01) and instrumental (r = .15, p < .01) support for fruit and vegetable consumption reported more fruit and vegetable servings. Women reported receiving less emotional support (for fruit and vegetables: r = −.08, p < .01, for physical activity: r = −.10, p < .01) and instrumental support (for fruit and vegetables: r = −.19, p < .01, for physical activity: r = −.09, p < .01). For physical activity, older participants took fewer daily steps (r = −.18, p < .01) and had lower self-rated health (r = −.12, p < .01); individuals with an older partner received more emotional (r = .09, p < .01), and instrumental (r = .09, p < .01) support. More daily steps were reported when more support was reported (emotional; r = .21, p < .01; instrumental; r = .19, p < .01). Dyad type (0 = nonspouse, 1= spouse) was significantly correlated with the main study variables in such a way that study partners who were spouses were perceived as providing more emotional and instrumental support (fruit and vegetable consumption: emotional; r = .23, p < .01; instrumental; r = .29, p < .01, daily steps: emotional; r = .18, p < .01; instrumental; r = .20, p < .01) than study partners who were not spouses. Also, participating with a spouse was positively associated with both health behaviours (fruit and vegetable consumption: r = .08, p < .01, steps: r = .11, p < .01).

Social support for health behaviours

To examine the predicted associations between social support and fruit and vegetable consumption, we modelled our outcome using participant age and gender, study partner age and gender, participant reported daily emotional support, overall emotional support, participant reported daily instrumental support, overall instrumental support, dyad type, day of study, and start day as predictors (Table 4, Model A). At the within-person level and in line with expectations, participants consumed more fruit and vegetables on days when their emotional support was higher than usual (b = .07, p = .013). At the between-person level, those participants who overall reported consuming more fruit and vegetables also reported receiving more emotional support (b = .02, p = .048). No similar effects were found for instrumental support. Participant (b = -.08, p = .035) and study partner age (b = .04, p = .010) were also significantly associated with fruit and vegetable consumption in opposite directions.

Table 4. Results from multilevel models: support for daily fruit and vegetable consumption (N = 136, n = 1202)

Note. *p<.05; **p<.01.

Parallel models were conducted for step counts. Specifically, to analyse associations with daily steps, we modelled our outcome using participant age and gender, partner age and gender, participant reported daily emotional support, overall emotional support, participant reported daily instrumental support, overall instrumental support, dyad type, day of study, and start day, step count source (whether it came from a smartphone/fitness watch or was calculated by the participant), and self-rated health (Table 5, Model C). Participants reported taking more daily steps on days when emotional support for physical activity (b = 145.99, p = .007) and instrumental support was up (b = 117.29, p = .023). Self-reported health (b = 1420.57, p < .001), participant age (b = −170.23, p = .002), partner age (b = 57.46, p = .006), participant gender (b = −2080.75, p = .007), dyad type (b = 1937.53, p = .032), day of study (b = −104.24, p = .003),Footnote 6 and start day (b = −7.91, p = .004) were also significant predictors of daily step counts.

Table 5. Results from multilevel models: support for daily steps (N = 133, n = 1101)

Note. *p<.05; **p<.0.

Sources of social support for health behaviours

We also explored whether dyad type moderated the association between social support and health behaviours by modelling interaction effects (Table 4, Model B and Table 5, Model D). There was no moderating effect of type of dyad on time-varying or interindividual differences in support – fruit-and-vegetable consumption links. Similar null findings emerged when examining additional ways of distinguishing dyads (living together versus not, kin versus not, or same-gender partners versus not).

For physical activity, there also was no moderating effect of type of dyad either. Similar to the fruit and vegetable consumption models, when examining different types of dyads (living together versus not, kin versus not, or same-gender partners versus not), no significant interactions emerged.

Discussion

The present study examined the role of everyday emotional and instrumental support as well as its source (i.e., the type of relationship individuals received support from) for two key health behaviours using up to 10 days of daily diary reports from a sample of 136 older adults. Consistent with previous studies, both types of support were positively correlated with fruit and vegetable consumption as well as step counts (Berli et al., Reference Berli, Bolger, Shrout, Stadler and Scholz2018; Bloom et al., 2017). Our multilevel models paint a differentiated picture. Specifically, daily and overall emotional support were significantly associated with daily fruit and vegetable consumption whereas only daily, but not overall levels of emotional and instrumental support were positively associated with daily step counts. There were no significant moderating effects for dyad type and support on the targeted health behaviours.

Social support for health behaviours

Consistent with our hypotheses, daily and overall emotional support were positively associated with fruit and vegetable consumption, but the predicted associations for instrumental support were not significant. In other words, findings suggest that on days when individuals reported more emotional support than usual, they also consumed more fruit and vegetables. Similarly, individuals who reported receiving more overall emotional support were found to have consumed more fruit and vegetables during time in study. A possible reason for this might be that instrumental support may be so engrained in older adults’ daily life that it is no longer noticed. For example, in traditional marriages it might be the case that one partner is primarily responsible for buying groceries and cooking meals and that this form of instrumental support goes by unnoticed (Bloom et al., 2017; Hughes et al., Reference Hughes, Bennett and Hetherington2004).

Daily emotional and instrumental support were significantly associated with step counts, but interindividual differences in support were not. This suggests that for daily step counts, what seems to matter most is how much support that person perceives on a given day in relation to their usual levels of support. This is consistent with other research which found that the lion’s share of variability in physical activity is at the day level and that daily support is positively associated with daily physical activity (Berli et al., Reference Berli, Bolger, Shrout, Stadler and Scholz2018; Broen et al., Reference Broen, Choi, Zambrano Garza, Pauly, Gerstorf and Hoppmann2023; Khan et al., Reference Khan, Stephens, Franks, Rook and Salem2013). It is possible that the pandemic affected most people’s physical activity in a similar way reducing variability between participants. Finally, it must be noted that although some participants reported their steps using physical activity tracking devices, the majority of the sample self-reported their physical activity.

It is important to consider why results differ across the two health behaviours; intra- and interindividual differences in emotional support were positively associated with daily fruit and vegetable intake whereas only time-varying associations emerged for social support-step count associations. This difference might arise from the COVID-19 pandemic, as public health mandates might have restricted physical activity more than healthy eating due to closures of recreation facilities. Essential businesses like food outlets remained open, thus highlighting the daily support received for this health behaviour (Larson et al., Reference Larson, Bader-Larsen and Magkos2021).Footnote 7 Furthermore, another possible reason may be individuals’ levels of health literacy, how they interpret each behaviour, and consequently, the support they provide (Geboers et al., Reference Geboers, de Winter, Luten, Jansen and Reijneveld2014). For example, fruit and vegetable consumption might be seen as a healthy behaviour, whereas daily steps might be seen as routine and therefore elicit no praise.

Sources of social support for health behaviours

Moderating effects of dyad type on health behaviours were not significant in our sample. Although this study built on past research examining social support for health behaviours by expanding the source of support beyond the spouse (Berli et al., Reference Berli, Bolger, Shrout, Stadler and Scholz2018, Reference Berli, Schwaninger and Scholz2021; Franks et al., Reference Franks, Stephens, Rook, Franklin, Keteyian and Artinian2006; Garay-Sevilla et al., Reference Garay-Sevilla, Nava, Malacara, Huerta, de León, Mena and Fajardo1995; Khan et al., Reference Khan, Stephens, Franks, Rook and Salem2013; Martire et al., Reference Martire, Stephens, Mogle, Schulz, Brach and Keefe2013; Rook & Ituarte, Reference Rook and Ituarte1999; Stephens et al., Reference Stephens, Franks, Rook, Iida, Hemphill and Salem2013), it is important to note that we did not have equal numbers of participants across different dyad types and that the levels of support reported significantly differed, which may affect power and thus our ability to detect an effect in those that did not participate with a spouse. Future studies should aim for equal representations of targeted types of relationships to be able to reliably tease apart potential group differences. Additionally, the literature suggests points to gender differences (e.g., for men the spouse is often the primary source of support, whereas for women social support comes from different relationship partners; Fuhrer & Stansfeld, Reference Fuhrer and Stansfeld2002). In our sample, there were interesting gender differences in support depending on the actor and partner, but again, there were differences in the proportions of these groups. If gender is of interest, equal representation of gender of actors and partners should be a priority to be able to meaningfully tease apart these associations.

Strengths, limitations, and future directions

Although our study sheds light on positive associations between social support and health behaviours, we did not assess if social support was appreciated or needed. Future studies could include questions for participants regarding their feelings towards the support received. This could shed light on the usefulness of the support received while also considering if individuals actually appreciate and welcome this support, as it might be perceived as less sensitive by certain partners or could kindle relationship tensions (Rook & Charles, Reference Rook and Charles2017).

We were able to distinguish between emotional support and instrumental support, but each support type was measured with only one item.Footnote 8 Further, we analysed interindividual differences with averages from instances of support reported across study days, it is important to highlight that frequencies and averages yield distinct insights. Future studies could implement experience-sampling methodology to analyse support transactions as they occur and take into account not only the nature of the support exchange but also how often it occurs.

Finally, although findings differ, we were able to conduct parallel analyses of two important health behaviours: fruit and vegetable consumption and physical activity. This was done using daily life assessments that have high ecological validity as they allow us to capture life as it is lived. Of note, measurements were based on self-reports and the majority of participants estimated their steps. This inherent weakness can be tackled in future studies by asking participants to take pictures of their food and to wear activity monitors for more objective measures.

Conclusion

Older adults do not always eat healthily or engage in sufficient physical activity, core health behaviours known to lower chronic disease risk and managing existing conditions. The COVID-19 pandemic exacerbated existing challenges and created new ones. Social support has been shown to be an avenue to promote health behaviours, but most past research focuses on spouses (Berli et al., Reference Berli, Bolger, Shrout, Stadler and Scholz2018, Reference Berli, Schwaninger and Scholz2021; Khan et al., Reference Khan, Stephens, Franks, Rook and Salem2013; Martire et al., Reference Martire, Stephens, Mogle, Schulz, Brach and Keefe2013; Stephens et al., Reference Stephens, Franks, Rook, Iida, Hemphill and Salem2013). It is important to consider different sources of social support as older adults’ networks may be diverse (i.e., due to divorce or death); an improved understanding of support for these important health behaviours can help raise public awareness on the importance of social support and help community-based initiatives create campaigns that might benefit older individuals by incorporating and highlighting the influence of close relationships. This study examined two types of social support, emotional and instrumental support, as well as its links with two health behaviours, daily fruit and vegetable intake and step counts. Our results corroborate existing research by showing that emotional and instrumental support are positively associated with health behaviours. It is our hope that these initial findings on spousal and nonspousal dyads can be extended to shed more light on different sources of support that have the capacity to improve older adults’ well-being and health.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at http://doi.org/10.1017/S0714980824000412.

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by the Canadian Institutes of Health Research (PJT - 169093) as well as a University of British Columbia HIFI award. Elizabeth Zambrano Garza and Yoonseok Choi gratefully acknowledge support from UBC’s Four Year Doctoral Fellowship. Rachel Murphy’s time was supported by the Michael Smith Health Research BC (grant #17644). Christiane Hoppmann, Theresa Pauly, and Maureen Ashe gratefully acknowledge support from the Canada Research Chairs Program.

Footnotes

1 Most participants had high intentions at baseline to engage in health behaviours (engage in physical activity: M = 4.33, eat several fruit and vegetables servings: M = 4.21, scale 1-5) and including morning intentions for engagement in the health behaviours as covariates did not change results reported.

2 Including daily number of minutes spent interacting with study partners as covariates did not change results reported.

3 Including SES as a covariate did not change results reported.

4 Welch’s t-tests revealed significant differences across both types of support for both types of health behaviors for spouses and nonspouses, suggesting these groups differ.

5 We also ran models excluding those 2 standard deviations above the mean (965 observations, 43 were excluded) results remain the same.

6 We also ran models that controlled for temporal autocorrelation and lagged effects and there were no differences from our reported results.

7 Indeed, follow-up analyses from t-tests comparing physical activity levels and levels of fruit and vegetable consumption before the pandemic and during the pandemic were only significant for physical activity; levels decreased during the pandemic.

8 Findings did not differ when running analyses with a combined measure (both types of support averaged).

References

Antonucci, T. C., Ajrouch, K. J., & Birditt, K. S. (2014). The convoy model: Explaining social relations from a multidisciplinary perspective. The Gerontologist, 54(1), 8292. https://doi.org/10.1093/geront/gnt118CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Antonucci, T. C., & Akiyama, H. (1987). Social networks in adult life and a preliminary examination of the convoy model. Journal of Gerontology, 42(5), 519527. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronj/42.5.519CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Applegate, W. B., & Ouslander, J. G. (2020). COVID-19 presents high risk to older persons. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 68(4), 681681. https://doi.org/10.1111/jgs.16426CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arend, M. G., & Schäfer, T. (2019). Statistical power in two-level models: A tutorial based on Monte Carlo simulation. Psychological Methods, 24(1), 119. https://doi.org/10.1037/met0000195CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barrera, M. (1986). Distinctions between social support concepts, measures, and models. American Journal of Community Psychology, 14(4), 413445. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00922627CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67, 148. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v067.i01CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berli, C., Bolger, N., Shrout, P. E., Stadler, G., & Scholz, U. (2018). Interpersonal processes of couples’ daily support for goal pursuit: The example of physical activity. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 44(3), 332344. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167217739264CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berli, C., Schwaninger, P., & Scholz, U. (2021). “We Feel Good”: Daily support provision, health behavior, and well-being in romantic couples. Frontiers in Psychology, 11. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.622492CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Biddle, S. J. H., Fox, K., & Boutcher, S. (Eds.). (2000). Physical activity and psychological well-being. Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203468326Google Scholar
Blanchflower, D. G., Oswald, A. J., & Stewart-Brown, S. (2013). Is psychological well-being linked to the consumption of fruit and vegetables? Social Indicators Research, 114(3), 785801. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11205-012-0173-yCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blieszner, R. (2009). Who are the aging families? In Aging families and caregiving (pp. 118). John Wiley & Sons, Inc.Google Scholar
Bloom, I., Edwards, M., Jameson, K. A., Syddall, H. E., Dennison, E., Gale, C. R., Baird, J., Cooper, C., Aihie Sayer, A., & Robinson, S. (2017a). Influences on diet quality in older age: The importance of social factors. Age and Ageing, 46(2), 277283. https://doi.org/10.1093/ageing/afw180Google ScholarPubMed
Bolger, N., Davis, A., & Rafaeli, E. (2003). Diary methods: Capturing life as it is lived. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 579616. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.54.101601.145030CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Broen, T., Choi, Y., Zambrano Garza, E., Pauly, T., Gerstorf, D., & Hoppmann, C. A., & (2023). Time-varying associations between loneliness and physical activity: Evidence from repeated daily life assessments in an adult lifespan sample. Frontiers in Psychology, 13, 1021863.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, S. L., & Lin, I.-F. (2012). The gray divorce revolution: Rising divorce among middle-aged and older adults, 1990-2010. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 67(6), 731741. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbs089CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carr, D. (2021). COVID-19: Trends, disparities, and consequences for older adults. The Journals of Gerontology: Series B, 76(3), e65e67. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbaa211CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chong, T. W. H., Doyle, C. J., Cyarto, E. V., Cox, K. L., Ellis, K. A., Ames, D., Lautenschlager, N. T., & Group, A. R. (2014). Physical activity program preferences and perspectives of older adults with and without cognitive impairment. Asia-Pacific Psychiatry, 6(2), 179190. https://doi.org/10.1111/appy.12015CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Colley, R. C., Garriguet, D., Janssen, I., Craig, C. L., Clarke, J., & Tremblay, M. S. (2011). Physical activity of Canadian adults: Accelerometer results from the 2007 to 2009 Canadian Health Measures Survey. Health Reports, 22(1), 7.Google ScholarPubMed
Conklin, A. I., Forouhi, N. G., Surtees, P., Khaw, K.-T., Wareham, N. J., & Monsivais, P. (2014). Social relationships and healthful dietary behaviour: Evidence from over-50s in the EPIC cohort, UK. Social Science & Medicine, 100, 167175. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2013.08.018Google ScholarPubMed
Coyne, J. C., & DeLongis, A. (1986). Going beyond social support: The role of social relationships in adaptation. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 54(4), 454460. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.54.4.454Google ScholarPubMed
Craig, C. L., Marshall, A. L., Sjöström, M. M., Bauman, A. E., Booth, M. L., Ainsworth, B. E., Pratt, M., Ekelund, U., Yngve, A., Sallis, J. F., & Oja, P. (2003). International physical activity questionnaire: 12-country reliability and validity: Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise, 35(8), 13811395. https://doi.org/10.1249/01.MSS.0000078924.61453.FBCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davis, M. G., & Fox, K. R. (2007). Physical activity patterns assessed by accelerometry in older people. European Journal of Applied Physiology, 100(5), 581589. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00421-006-0320-8Google ScholarPubMed
DeLongis, A., Capreol, M., Holtzman, S., O’Brien, T., & Campbell, J. (2004). Social support and social strain among husbands and wives: A multilevel analysis. Journal of Family Psychology, 18(3), 470.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fiest, K. M., Currie, S. R., Williams, J. V. A., & Wang, J. (2011). Chronic conditions and major depression in community-dwelling older adults. Journal of Affective Disorders, 131(1), 172178. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2010.11.028CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Flanagan, E. W., Beyl, R. A., Fearnbach, S. N., Altazan, A. D., Martin, C. K., & Redman, L. M. (2021). The impact of COVID-19 stay-at-home orders on health behaviors in adults. Obesity, 29(2), 438445. https://doi.org/10.1002/oby.23066CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Franks, M. M., Stephens, M. A. P., Rook, K. S., Franklin, B. A., Keteyian, S. J., & Artinian, N. T. (2006). Spouses’ provision of health-related support and control to patients participating in cardiac rehabilitation. Journal of Family Psychology, 20(2), 311318. https://doi.org/10.1037/0893-3200.20.2.311CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fuhrer, R., & Stansfeld, S. A. (2002). How gender affects patterns of social relations and their impact on health: A comparison of one or multiple sources of support from “close persons”. Social Science & Medicine, 54(5), 811825. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0277-9536(01)00111-3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fulkerson, J. A., Larson, N., Horning, M., & Neumark-Sztainer, D. (2014). A review of associations between family or shared meal frequency and dietary and weight status outcomes across the lifespan. Journal of Nutrition Education and Behavior, 46(1), 219. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneb.2013.07.012CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garay-Sevilla, Ma. E., Nava, L. E., Malacara, J. M., Huerta, R., de León, J. D., Mena, A., & Fajardo, M. E. (1995). Adherence to treatment and social support in patients with non-insulin dependent diabetes mellitus. Journal of Diabetes and Its Complications, 9(2), 8186. https://doi.org/10.1016/1056-8727(94)00021-FCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Geboers, B., de Winter, A. F., Luten, K. A., Jansen, C. J. M., & Reijneveld, S. A. (2014). The association of health literacy with physical activity and nutritional behavior in older adults, and its social cognitive mediators. Journal of Health Communication, 19(sup2), 6176. https://doi.org/10.1080/10810730.2014.934933CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Govindaraju, T., Sahle, B. W., McCaffrey, T. A., McNeil, J. J., & Owen, A. J. (2018). Dietary patterns and quality of life in older adults: A systematic review. Nutrients, 10(8), Article 8. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu10080971Google ScholarPubMed
Grimm, E. K., Swartz, A. M., Hart, T., Miller, N. E., & Strath, S. J. (2012). Comparison of the IPAQ-Short Form and accelerometry predictions of physical activity in older adults. Journal of Aging and Physical Activity, 20(1), 6479. https://doi.org/10.1123/japa.20.1.64CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Halter, J. B., Musi, N., McFarland Horne, F., Crandall, J. P., Goldberg, A., Harkless, L., Hazzard, W. R., Huang, E. S., Kirkman, M. S., Plutzky, J., Schmader, K. E., Zieman, S., & High, K. P. (2014). Diabetes and cardiovascular disease in older adults: Current status and future directions. Diabetes, 63(8), 25782589. https://doi.org/10.2337/db14-0020Google ScholarPubMed
He, F. J., Nowson, C. A., & MacGregor, G. A. (2006). Fruit and vegetable consumption and stroke: Meta-analysis of cohort studies. The Lancet, 367(9507), 320326. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(06)68069-0Google ScholarPubMed
Hoppmann, C. A., & Riediger, M. (2009). Ambulatory assessment in lifespan psychology: An overview of current status and new trends. European Psychologist, 14, 98108. https://doi.org/10.1027/1016-9040.14.2.98Google Scholar
Hughes, G., Bennett, K. M., & Hetherington, M. M. (2004). Old and alone: Barriers to healthy eating in older men living on their own. Appetite, 43(3), 269276. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2004.06.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Inglehart, R. (2002). Gender, aging, and subjective well-being. International Journal of Comparative Sociology, 43(3–5), 391408. https://doi.org/10.1177/002071520204300309CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Khan, C. M., Stephens, M. A. P., Franks, M. M., Rook, K. S., & Salem, J. K. (2013). Influences of spousal support and control on diabetes management through physical activity. Health Psychology, 32(7), 739747. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028609CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Larson, E. A., Bader-Larsen, K. S., & Magkos, F. (2021). The effect of COVID-19-related lockdowns on diet and physical activity in older adults: A systematic review. Aging and Disease, 12(8), 19351947. https://doi.org/10.14336/AD.2021.0606Google Scholar
Lindsay Smith, G., Banting, Lauren, Eime, R., O’Sullivan, G., & van Uffelen, J. G. Z. (2017). The association between social support and physical activity in older adults: A systematic review. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity, 14(1), 56. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12966-017-0509-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martire, L. M., Stephens, M. A. P., Mogle, J., Schulz, R., Brach, J., & Keefe, F. J. (2013). Daily spousal influence on physical activity in knee osteoarthritis. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 45(2), 213223. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12160-012-9442-xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pietromonaco, P. R., & Collins, N. L. (2017). Interpersonal mechanisms linking close relationships to health. American Psychologist, 72(6), 531. https://doi.org/10.1037/amp0000129CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reyes Fernández, B., Warner, L. M., Knoll, N., Montenegro Montenegro, E., & Schwarzer, R. (2015). Synergistic effects of social support and self-efficacy on dietary motivation predicting fruit and vegetable intake. Appetite, 87, 330335. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2014.12.223CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Riediger, N. D., & Moghadasian, M. H. (2008). Patterns of fruit and vegetable consumption and the influence of sex, age and socio-demographic factors among Canadian elderly. Journal of the American College of Nutrition, 27(2), 306313.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rodrigues, B., Encantado, J., Carraça, E., Sousa-Sá, E., Lopes, L., Cliff, D., Mendes, R., Silva, M. N., Godinho, C., & Santos, R. (2022). Questionnaires measuring movement behaviours in adults and older adults: Content description and measurement properties. A systematic review. PLOS ONE, 17(3), e0265100. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0265100CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Romero-Ortuno, R., Casey, A. M., Cunningham, C. U., Squires, S., Prendergast, D., Kenny, R. A., & Lawlor, B. A. (2011). Psychosocial and functional correlates of nutrition among community-dwelling older adults in Ireland. The Journal of Nutrition, Health & Aging, 15(7), 527531. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12603-010-0278-4Google ScholarPubMed
Rook, K. S., & Charles, S. T. (2017). Close social ties and health in later life: Strengths and vulnerabilities. American Psychologist, 72(6), 567. https://doi.org/10.1037/amp0000104CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rook, K. S., & Ituarte, P. H. G. (1999). Social control, social support, and companionship in older adults’ family relationships and friendships. Personal Relationships, 6(2), 199211. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1475-6811.1999.tb00187.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rugel, E. J., & Carpiano, R. M. (2015). Gender differences in the roles for social support in ensuring adequate fruit and vegetable consumption among older adult Canadians. Appetite, 92, 102109. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2015.05.011CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ruiz, M. C., Devonport, T. J., Chen-Wilson, C.-H. (Josephine), Nicholls, W., Cagas, J. Y., Fernandez-Montalvo, J., Choi, Y., & Robazza, C. (2021). A cross-cultural exploratory study of health behaviors and wellbeing during COVID-19. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 3897. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.608216Google ScholarPubMed
Scholz, U., Ochsner, S., Hornung, R., & Knoll, N. (2013). Does social support really help to eat a low-fat diet? Main effects and gender differences of received social support within the health action process approach. Applied Psychology: Health and Well-Being, 5(2), 270290. https://doi.org/10.1111/aphw.12010Google ScholarPubMed
Schrack, J. A., Cooper, R., Koster, A., Shiroma, E. J., Murabito, J. M., Rejeski, W. J., Ferrucci, L., & Harris, T. B. (2016). Assessing daily physical activity in older adults: Unraveling the complexity of monitors, measures, and methods. The Journals of Gerontology: Series A, 71(8), 10391048. https://doi.org/10.1093/gerona/glw026Google ScholarPubMed
Shaikh, A. R., Yaroch, A. L., Nebeling, L., Yeh, M.-C., & Resnicow, K. (2008). Psychosocial predictors of fruit and vegetable consumption in adults: A review of the literature. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 34(6), 535543.e11. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.amepre.2007.12.028CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stephens, M. A. P., Franks, M. M., Rook, K. S., Iida, M., Hemphill, R. C., & Salem, J. K. (2013). Spouses’ attempts to regulate day-to-day dietary adherence among patients with type 2 diabetes. Health Psychology, 32(10), 10291037. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030018CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Suanet, B., van Tilburg, T. G., & Broese van Groenou, M. I. (2013). Nonkin in older adults’ personal networks: More important among later cohorts? The Journals of Gerontology: Series B, 68(4), 633643. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbt043CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tudor-Locke, C., Han, H., Aguiar, E. J., Barreira, T. V. Jr, , J. M. S., Kang, M., & Rowe, D. A. (2018). How fast is fast enough? Walking cadence (steps/min) as a practical estimate of intensity in adults: A narrative review. British Journal of Sports Medicine, 52(12), 776788. https://doi.org/10.1136/bjsports-2017-097628CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tudor-Locke, C., Schuna, J. M. Jr, Barreira, T. V., Mire, E. F., Broyles, S. T., Katzmarzyk, P. T., & Johnson, W. D. (2013). Normative steps/day values for older adults: NHANES 2005–2006. The Journals of Gerontology: Series A, 68(11), 14261432. https://doi.org/10.1093/gerona/glt116Google ScholarPubMed
Vesnaver, E., & Keller, H. H. (2011). Social influences and eating behavior in later life: A review. Journal of Nutrition in Gerontology and Geriatrics, 30(1), 223. https://doi.org/10.1080/01639366.2011.545038CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whitehead, B. R., & Torossian, E. (2021). Older adults’ experience of the COVID-19 pandemic: A mixed-methods analysis of stresses and joys. The Gerontologist, 61(1), 3647. https://doi.org/10.1093/geront/gnaa126CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
World Health Organization. (2022, October). Physical activity. https://www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/physical-activityGoogle Scholar
Zacher, H., & Rudolph, C. W. (2021). Individual differences and changes in subjective wellbeing during the early stages of the COVID-19 pandemic. American Psychologist, 76(1), 50. https://doi.org/10.1037/amp0000702CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zambrano Garza, E., Pauly, T., Choi, Y., Murphy, R. A., Linden, W., Ashe, M. C., Madden, K. M., Jakobi, J. M., Gerstorf, D., & Hoppmann, C. A. (2024). Daily solitude and well-being associations in older dyads: Evidence from daily life assessments. Applied Psychology: Health and Well-Being, 16(1), 356375. https://doi.org/10.1111/aphw.12494Google ScholarPubMed
Zambrano Garza, E., Pauly, T., Choi, Y., Murphy, R. A., Linden, W., Ashe, M. C., Madden, K. M., Jakobi, J. M., Gerstorf, D., & Hoppmann, C. A., (accepted). Social Resources for Everyday Physical Activity During Times of Chronic Stress. Couple and Family Psychology: Research and PracticeGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. Dyad types within the sample by percentages.

Figure 1

Table 1. Means and standard deviations.

Figure 2

Table 2. Correlations with fruit and vegetable consumption (N = 136)

Figure 3

Table 3. Correlations with self-reported physical activity (N = 133)

Figure 4

Table 4. Results from multilevel models: support for daily fruit and vegetable consumption (N = 136, n = 1202)

Figure 5

Table 5. Results from multilevel models: support for daily steps (N = 133, n = 1101)

Supplementary material: File

Zambrano Garza et al. supplementary material

Zambrano Garza et al. supplementary material
Download Zambrano Garza et al. supplementary material(File)
File 17.9 KB