Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-mlc7c Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-14T17:10:29.676Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The Nature of Children's Conflict Resolution Styles

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 February 2016

Phillip T. Slee*
Affiliation:
South Australian College of Advanced Education, Sturt Campus, Sturt Road, Bedford Park, 5042 Adelaide, S.A.

Extract

Conflict is an integral part of human relationships. As defined by The University English Dictionary conflict denotes a struggle, a clashing of views or statements, a meeting in opposition or antagonism. The position taken in the present study is that a conflict consists of an opposition between two individuals “when one person does something to which a second person objects” (Hay, 1984, p.2). This particular outlook is consistent with that adopted by Kagan, Knight & Martinez-Romero (1982) in their study of children's conflict resolution styles.

Paradoxically, while conflict may be an important aspect of human relations, conflict as evidenced in children's relations has received scant attention. Research has identified possible sex differences in children's conflict with boys engaging in more direct conflict than girls (Miller, Danaher & Forbes, 1986; Shantz, 1986). Boys do appear to use different conflict resolution strategies (namely more threats and physical force as opposed to negotiation) than girls (Miller et al, 1981). Some evidence also exists that there are developmental changes in children's conflict resolution strategies such that from 5 to 9 years there is an increasing use of a reconciliation conflict resolution style (Aboud, 1981).

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1990

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Aboud, F. (1981). Egocentrism, conformity and agreeing to disagree. Developmental Psychology, 17, 791799.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Camras, L. (1980). Children's understanding of facial expressions used during conflict encounters. Child Development, 51, 879885. Gilligan, C. (1982). In a different voice. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hay, D. (1984). Social conflict in early childhood. In Whitehurst, G. (Ed.) Annals of child development Vol.1, pp.144. Greenwich, CT: JAI.Google Scholar
Kagan, S. & Madsen, M. (1972). Rivalry in Anglo-American and Mexican children of two ages. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 24, 478487.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kagan, S., Knight, g., & Martinez-Romero, S. (1982). Culture and the development of conflict resolution style. Journal of Cross Cultural Psychology, 13, 4358.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laursen, B., & Hartup, W. (1989). The dynamics of preschool children's conflicts. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 35, 281297.Google Scholar
Miller, P., Danaher, D., & Forbes, D. (1986). Sex-related strategies for coping with interpersonal conflict in children aged five to seven. Developmental Psychology, 22, 543548.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shantz, C. (1987). Conflicts between children. Child Development, 58, 283305.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shantz, D. (1986). Conflict, aggression, and peer status: An observational study. Child Development, 57, 13221332.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whiting, B. & Edwards, C. (1973). A cross-cultural analysis of sex differences in the behaviour of children aged three through eleven. Journal of Social Psychology, 91, 171188.CrossRefGoogle Scholar