Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gvvz8 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T15:16:26.523Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Characterizing the role of unpredictability within different dimensions of early life adversity

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 October 2024

Bence Csaba Farkas*
Affiliation:
UVSQ, Inserm, CESP, Université Paris-Saclay, Villejuif, France Conseil Départemental Yvelines et Hauts-de-Seine et Centre Hospitalier des Versailles, Institut du Psychotraumatisme de l’Enfant et de l’Adolescent, Versailles, France Centre de recherche en épidémiologie et en santé des populations, Université Paris-Saclay, Université Versailles Saint-Quentin, Paris, France LNC2, Département d’études Cognitives, École Normale Supérieure, INSERM, PSL Research University, Paris, France
Pierre Olivier Jacquet
Affiliation:
UVSQ, Inserm, CESP, Université Paris-Saclay, Villejuif, France Conseil Départemental Yvelines et Hauts-de-Seine et Centre Hospitalier des Versailles, Institut du Psychotraumatisme de l’Enfant et de l’Adolescent, Versailles, France Centre de recherche en épidémiologie et en santé des populations, Université Paris-Saclay, Université Versailles Saint-Quentin, Paris, France LNC2, Département d’études Cognitives, École Normale Supérieure, INSERM, PSL Research University, Paris, France
*
Corresponding author: Bence Csaba Farkas; Email: bence.farkas@universite-paris-saclay.fr
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Dimensional models of early life adversity highlight the distinct roles of deprivation and threat in shaping neurocognitive development and mental health. However, relatively little is known about the role of unpredictability within each dimension. We estimated both the average levels of, and the temporal unpredictability of deprivation and threat exposure during adolescence in a high-risk, longitudinal sample of 1354 youth (Pathways to Desistance study). We then related these estimates to later life psychological distress, and Antisocial and Borderline personality traits, and tested whether any effects are mediated by future orientation. High average levels of both deprivation and threat exposure were found to be associated with worse mental health on all three outcomes, but only the effects on Antisocial and Borderline personality traits were mediated by decreased future orientation, a pattern consistent with evolutionary models of psychopathology. Unpredictability in deprivation exposure proved to be associated with increased psychological distress and a higher number of Borderline traits, but with increased future orientation. There was some evidence of unpredictability in threat exposure buffering against the detrimental developmental effects of average threat levels. Our results suggest that the effects of unpredictability are distinct within different dimensions of early life adversity.

Type
Regular Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Childhood adversity refers to negative environmental experiences, such as poverty, neglect, or maltreatment, that require significant adaptation by a typical child (Frankenhuis & Amir, Reference Frankenhuis and Amir2022; McLaughlin et al., Reference McLaughlin, Weissman and Bitrán2019). A large body of research using the cumulative adversity approach has demonstrated the pervasive effects of early life adversity on mental and physical health (Evans et al., Reference Evans, Li and Whipple2013; Grummitt et al., Reference Grummitt, Kreski, Kim, Platt, Keyes and McLaughlin2021). Developmental scholars have been increasingly highlighting the value of extending these important results by building more specific and mechanistic models of the effects of adversity factors (Berman et al., Reference Berman, McLaughlin, Tottenham, Godfrey, Seeman, Loucks, Suomi, Danese and Sheridan2022; McLaughlin et al., Reference McLaughlin, Weissman and Bitrán2019, Reference McLaughlin, Sheridan, Humphreys, Belsky and Ellis2021). So-called dimensional models of adversity and psychopathology propose that individual adversity types (such as physical or sexual abuse, emotional neglect, poverty, etc.) impact development through neither fully distinct, nor fully overlapping mechanisms. Instead, their effects on psychological and biological functions are best accounted for by a set of core dimensions (Ellis et al., Reference Ellis, Sheridan, Belsky and McLaughlin2022; McLaughlin et al., Reference McLaughlin, Sheridan, Humphreys, Belsky and Ellis2021).

One line of dimensional models, based on life history theory – a theoretical framework in evolutionary developmental biology (Stearns, Reference Stearns1992) – identifies “harshness” and “unpredictability” as the most important dimensions (Ellis et al., Reference Ellis, Figueredo, Brumbach and Schlomer2009). Harshness refers to extrinsic morbidity–mortality, which encompasses all external factors causing death and disability in a given population and that are beyond the individuals’ control, while unpredictability refers to the rates at which harshness varies stochastically over time and space (Ellis et al., Reference Ellis, Figueredo, Brumbach and Schlomer2009). In harsh and/or unpredictable environments, organisms favor reproductive efforts and short-term goals, at the expense of somatic maintenance efforts and longer-term goals. This “fast” life history strategy – so named in opposition to a “slow” strategy whereby the organism would prioritize health and survival over immediate reproduction – is not only mediated by physiological mechanisms. Although this acceleration is only adaptive in certain contexts (de Vries et al., Reference de Vries, Galipaud and Kokko2023), there is evidence consistent with this pattern from a wide array of species (Promislow & Harvey, Reference Promislow and Harvey1990; Promislow, Reference Promislow1991). In humans, high levels of psychosocial adversity and temporal unpredictability in such adversity have also been shown to be associated with accelerated paces of life (Šaffa et al., Reference Šaffa, Kubicka, Hromada and Kramer2019; Bulley & Pepper, Reference Bulley and Pepper2017; Mell et al., Reference Mell, Safra, Algan, Baumard and Chevallier2018; Nettle, Reference Nettle2010), although the evidence is somewhat weaker in non-Western populations (Sear et al., Reference Sear, Sheppard and Coall2019; Sear, Reference Sear2020). Importantly, this “fast” life history strategy is also dependent on psychological traits such as impulsivity, risk-taking, and present orientation that facilitate access to biological goals (Del Giudice et al., Reference Del Giudice, Gangestad, Kaplan and Buss2015; Ellis et al., Reference Ellis, Del Giudice, Dishion, Figueredo, Gray, Griskevicius, Hawley, Jacobs, James, Volk and Wilson2012). These traits can then predispose individuals to certain mental and physical health conditions, especially those that comprise the externalizing spectrum of psychopathology (e.g., Antisocial and Borderline Personality Disorders, substance abuse, the positive symptoms of Schizophrenia and some eating disorders) (Del Giudice & Haltigan, Reference Del Giudice and Haltigan2023). This might happen for multiple reasons, for example, because such traits are adaptive in the evolutionary sense of increasing fitness, but are associated with behaviors that are considered undesirable in the sociocultural context, or because such traits are developed as a result of early environmental cues, that end up being mismatched with the actual state of adult environments.

A parallel line of models, rooted in experience-driven neuroplasticity, instead focuses on the dimensions of “deprivation” and “threat” (Sheridan & McLaughlin, Reference Sheridan and McLaughlin2014). Deprivation is defined as the lack of expected environmental inputs, and threat is defined as physical harm or threat of harm. Once again, there is a large evidence base linking these dimensions to cognitive, affective and behavioral dysregulation (McLaughlin et al., Reference McLaughlin, Weissman and Bitrán2019; Miller et al., Reference Miller, Sheridan, Hanson, McLaughlin, Bates, Lansford, Pettit and Dodge2018, Reference Miller, Machlin, McLaughlin and Sheridan2021). Recent theoretical and empirical progress has integrated the Deprivation-Threat and Harshness-Unpredictability frameworks into a three-component model based on the assumption that threat and deprivation are best conceptualized as distinct sources of harshness, in the sense that both contribute to increasing disability and death in the population (Ellis et al., Reference Ellis, Sheridan, Belsky and McLaughlin2022; Usacheva et al., Reference Usacheva, Choe, Liu, Timmer and Belsky2022). This model therefore identifies (i) Threat as a source of harshness capturing morbidity–mortality from harm imposed by other agents; (ii) Deprivation as a source of harshness capturing morbidity–mortality from insufficient environmental inputs; and (iii) Unpredictability as stochastic spatiotemporal variability in both Threat and Deprivation.

One major gap in our current understanding of the effects of these dimensions concerns the conceptualization and operationalization of unpredictability (Young et al., Reference Young, Frankenhuis and Ellis2020). There are two sources of unpredictability in the environment. On the one hand, humans might have evolved to preferentially process certain discrete events that have served as reliable cues of environmental variability in the evolutionary past (e.g., disruptive family events). These events now serve as “ancestral cues” to estimate environmental unpredictability and to guide development. Research guided by this approach has tended to operationalize unpredictability by creating sum scores of exposure to disruptive events, hypothesized to be such ancestral cues (e.g., Belsky et al., Reference Belsky, Schlomer and Ellis2012; Brumbach et al., Reference Brumbach, Figueredo and Ellis2009; Doom et al., Reference Doom, Vanzomeren-Dohm and Simpson2016). On the other hand, the brain also might have the capacity to continuously monitor informative features of the current environment (e.g., harshness of physical discipline) and integrate these estimates to infer environmental unpredictability, with “statistical learning” mechanisms. Research guided by this approach has tended to operationalize unpredictability by quantifying the degree of random variability in trajectories of harshness exposure across time (e.g., Li et al., Reference Li, Liu, Hartman and Belsky2018; Li & Belsky, Reference Li and Belsky2022; Zachrisson & Dearing, Reference Zachrisson and Dearing2015).

Results using both approaches have been generally consistent with idea that unpredictability contributes to the development of “fast” life history strategies, and related mental health outcomes. It has been linked to more unrestricted sociosexuality (Belsky et al., Reference Belsky, Schlomer and Ellis2012; Brumbach et al., Reference Brumbach, Figueredo and Ellis2009; Simpson et al., Reference Simpson, Griskevicius, Kuo, Sung and Collins2012; Szepsenwol et al., Reference Szepsenwol, Griskevicius, Simpson, Young, Fleck and Jones2017), greater risk-taking (Belsky et al., Reference Belsky, Schlomer and Ellis2012; Simpson et al., Reference Simpson, Griskevicius, Kuo, Sung and Collins2012; Wu et al., Reference Wu, Guo, Gao and Kou2020), and more externalizing problems and substance use (Doom et al., Reference Doom, Vanzomeren-Dohm and Simpson2016; Martinez et al., Reference Martinez, Hasty, Morabito, Maranges, Schmidt and Maner2022). There is also some evidence linking unpredictability with internalizing problems (Glynn et al., Reference Glynn, Howland, Sandman, Davis, Phelan, Baram and Stern2018; Wu, Reference Wu2024). Results regarding the interaction of unpredictability and harshness have been far less consistent. Li and Belsky (Reference Li and Belsky2022) identify multiple possible patterns, with each of them having some empirical support in the literature: (a) A dual-risk pattern would mean that unpredictability amplifies the negative effects of harshness, with worse outcomes for children exposed to high harshness and high unpredictability. This is what is found by Doom et al. (Reference Doom, Vanzomeren-Dohm and Simpson2016). (b) A dual-benefit pattern would mean that low unpredictability amplifies the positive effects of low harshness, with the best outcomes for children exposed to low harshness and low unpredictability. This is consistent with the results of Simpson et al. (Reference Simpson, Griskevicius, Kuo, Sung and Collins2012). (c) A buffering pattern would mean that the negative consequences of harshness are attenuated in the absence of unpredictability. This is what Cohen and Wills (Reference Cohen and Wills1985) found. (d) A pattern in which the combination of high harshness with low unpredictability leads to the most problematic developmental outcomes. This is observed by Li et al. (Reference Li, Liu, Hartman and Belsky2018). Moreover, while it is theoretically assumed that unpredictability is a feature of deprivation and threat (i.e., each dimension has its own pattern of variability, potentially distinct from each other), studies have tended to operationalize it as a separate exposure on its own. Therefore, we know very little about the way mean levels of deprivation and threat and their unpredictability interact to shape development and contribute to the emergence of psychopathology.

In this work, we start to fill this gap by separately estimating average levels of adolescent deprivation and threat exposure, as well as their temporal unpredictability, using residuals from random effects models on the same set of indicators, in a sample of more than 1300 youth. We first investigate the agreement between our proposed residual based metric to other unpredictability metrics. We then relate these scores to later mental health outcomes of Borderline and Antisocial personality features, and overall psychological distress. We also investigate whether the effects are mediated by future orientation, as more present-oriented decision-making has been repeatedly highlighted as an important psychological component of “fast” life history strategies (Copping et al., Reference Copping, Campbell and Muncer2014; Farkas et al., Reference Farkas, Chambon and Jacquet2021; Pepper & Nettle, Reference Pepper and Nettle2017). Our predictions were guided by the evolutionary psychopathology framework of Del Giudice (Reference Del Giudice2018), proposing that while independent clusters, both fast spectrum conditions (such as Borderline and Antisocial personality disorder) and distress conditions (such as depression and anxiety) are more likely to develop following early life adversity. However, as distress conditions do not reflect “fast” life histories, they should not be strongly linked to more present orientation. With respect to the specific effects of deprivation and threat and their unpredictability, the current evidence base does not allow strong hypotheses to be made. Putting it all together, we expected (i) that average and unpredictable deprivation and threat will be associated with more present orientation and worse outcomes on all three of our mental health measures and (ii) that present orientation will be associated with more Borderline and Antisocial traits, but not with higher overall psychological distress. We were agnostic regarding the effects of average × unpredictability interactions.

Methods

Participants

Data was drawn from the Pathways to Desistance Study, a United States longitudinal study of primarily male juvenile offenders in Phoenix, Arizona and Philadelphia, Pennsylvania (Schubert et al., Reference Schubert, Mulvey, Steinberg, Cauffman, Losoya, Hecker, Chassin and Knight2004). Youth were eligible for enrollment if they were between 14 and 17 years of age and had been convicted of a felony or a similarly serious nonfelony offense (e.g., a misdemeanor weapons offense, misdemeanor sexual assault). Enrollment into the study occurred between November, 2000 and January, 2003. The proportion of male youth found guilty of a drug charge was capped at 15% to avoid an over-representation of drug offenders. All females who met the age and crime criteria were also approached for enrollment, as were youth being considered for trial in the adult system. Eighty percent of approached youth agreed to participate. The total sample consists of 1354 participants.

Participants completed an initial baseline interview and were then reinterviewed a total of 10 times. Every 6 months for the first 6 assessments, and yearly for the remaining 4. Data for the current study was drawn from the baseline interview, and the follow-up interviews at 6, 12, 18, 24, 30, 36, and 72 months after baseline. Demographic information is presented in Table 1. Most of the sample was male, identified as Black, with parental education levels corresponding to High school diploma or less. Mean age at the Baseline interview was 16 years. The ratio of respondents from the two study sites was relatively balanced, with slightly more participants recruited at the Philadelphia site. Around half of all interviews were conducted at the participant’s home. Retention rates were high, with 87% of participants completing the 72 months follow-up interview.

Table 1. Sample descriptive statistics

Calculated from the full sample, comprising all participants of the Pathways study (N = 1354).

Participant assent and parent or guardian consent were obtained for youth under age 18. All participants were consented as an adult when they reached 18. Computer assisted interviews were conducted in either a facility, the juvenile’s home, or a mutually agreed-upon location in the community by trained interviewers. Participants were reminded during each interview, that the investigators were prohibited from disclosing any personally identifiable information to anyone outside the research staff. All recruitment and assessment procedures were approved by the Institutional Review Boards (IRBs) of the participating universities. Participants were paid $50 for their participation in the baseline interview, $65 at 6 months, $75 at 12 months, $100 at 18 months, $115 at 24 months, $130 at 30 months, and $150 at 36 months. All data accessed by us for the current study was fully anonymized.

Measures

Our measurements can be divided into four sets: early life adversity, serving as the primary independent variables of interest; future orientation, serving as a mediator; mental health, serving as the primary outcome variables of interest; and covariates.

Early life adversity

Our aim was to capture youths’ level of exposure to deprivation and threat, as well as the unpredictability of these exposures during the study period. We were guided by previous research in our operationalization of these constructs. Our conception of the dimensions of deprivation, threat and unpredictability followed the recent integrative model of Ellis et al. (Reference Ellis, Sheridan, Belsky and McLaughlin2022). This model defines threat as experiences that confer the risk of physical and psychological harm, deprivation as experiences that reflect insufficient environmental input, and unpredictability as spatiotemporal variability in deprivation and threat. We operationalized average levels of deprivation and threat exposure using sum scores of average values on a set of indicators during the study period, and operationalized the unpredictability of deprivation and threat using sum scores of random temporal variability of the same set of indicators during the study period (Li & Belsky, Reference Li and Belsky2022). Thus, we created four composite scores in total: Deprivation average, Deprivation unpredictability, Threat average, and Threat unpredictability. For all the early life adversity indicators, we used the data from the Baseline, and the 6, 12, 18, 24, 30, and 36 month follow-up assessments. Below, we summarize all chosen measures, and report their psychometric properties provided by the investigators of the Pathways dataset.

We selected our indicators in the following way. Firstly, we inspected and listed all 66 measures listed in the Pathways documentation (https://www.pathwaysstudy.pitt.edu/codebook/measures.html), plus the Money and Living calendars. We then classified these measures, and kept the ones that belonged to one of four pre-specified categories: (a) potential indicators of adversity; (b) potential mental health measures, specifically ones that are clearly linked to F-type, S-type, or D-type conditions in the framework of Del Giudice & Haltigan (Reference Del Giudice and Haltigan2023); (c) potential proxies of life-history related traits, specifically linked to time perspective or impulsivity; (d) potential confounding variables necessary to be included as adjustment variables in our analyses. This resulted in 39 variables. From this, we then further excluded variables that: (i) had large amounts (>50%) of missing values due to the measure being taken only by a subset of participants, e.g., variables relating to institutional climate for participants residing at an institution during interview; (ii) were only available from collateral informants with no self-reported data, e.g., Disruptive Behaviour Disorder variable; (iii) were not available at relevant timepoints, e.g., domestic violence exposure being only assessed at baseline; (iv) did not have available data with enough detail for our analysis, e.g., the CIDI scale only providing binary variables indicating meeting or not meeting diagnostic criteria, instead of continuous symptom scores; (v) were adversity indicators but not clearly definable as deprivation or threat, based on our working definitions of deprivation and threat as written below, e.g., friendship quality. After this, we were left with two candidate measures for life-history related traits (Psychosocial Maturity Inventory, Future Outlook Inventory) and F-type disorders (PAI, Substance use), between which we selected the one that had better psychometric properties based on available data, resulting in the final set of measures detailed below. Finally, we wanted clear temporal separation between what variables correspond to causes and consequences in our conceptual and eventual statistical model, therefore we retained longitudinal data of adversity variables at Baseline and the 6, 12, 18, 24, 30, and 36 month follow-up assessments, and for our outcome variables at the 36 month follow-up assessment only.

Deprivation

Deprivation was measured using five indicators of Caring adults, Maternal warmth, Social capital, Neighbourhood physical disorder, and Unstructured socializing. What ties these indicators together is that they are indicative of family and neighborhood environments characterized by relatively lower levels of social and cognitive input (Berman et al., Reference Berman, McLaughlin, Tottenham, Godfrey, Seeman, Loucks, Suomi, Danese and Sheridan2022; Ellis et al., Reference Ellis, Sheridan, Belsky and McLaughlin2022; McLaughlin et al., Reference McLaughlin, Sheridan, Humphreys, Belsky and Ellis2021).

Caring adults was measured by asking youth to identify the total number of supportive adults in their environment across a range of eight domains: adults you admire and want to be like, adults you could talk to if you needed information or advice about something, adults you could talk to about trouble at home, adults you would tell about an award or if you did something well, adults with whom you can talk about important decisions, adults you can depend on for help, adults you feel comfortable talking about problems with, and special adults who care about your feelings. We used the total number of adults across the eight domains, and reverse scored this indicator by subtracting values from 0, so that higher values indicated a smaller number of caring adults. The measure had excellent psychometric properties during both the baseline and follow-up interviews, reliability and CFA fit indices at baseline: Cronbach alpha = .78; NFI = .98, NNFI = .99, CFI = .99, RMSEA = 04; reliabilities at follow-up: 6 month Cronbach alpha = .84; 12 month Cronbach alpha = .87; 18 month Cronbach alpha = .89; 24 month Cronbach alpha = .90.

Maternal warmth was measured with an adaptation of the Quality of Parental Relationships Inventory (Conger et al., Reference Conger, Ge, Elder, Lorenz and Simons1994). Items from the measure tap parental warmth (e.g., “How often does your mother let you know she really cares about you?”) and parental hostility (e.g., “How often does your mother get angry at you?”). We used the subscale reflecting maternal warmth, and reverse coded the indicator so that higher scores indicate a less supportive and nurturing relationship with the youth’s mother. A four-factor model fit to the overall scale resulted in acceptable fit: NFI = .78, NNFI = .82, CFI = .83, RMSEA = .06. The maternal warmth subscale specifically had the following CFA goodness of fit at baseline: NFI = .95, NNFI = .94, CFI = .95, RMSEA = .08. It also had excellent reliability, baseline Cronbach alpha = .92; 6 month Cronbach alpha = .93; 12 month Cronbach alpha = .92; 18 month Cronbach alpha = .93; 24 month Cronbach alpha = .93.

Social capital was measured with the Social Capital Inventory (Nagin & Paternoster, Reference Nagin and Paternoster1994). This scale captures the connectedness an adolescent feels to their community, along three dimensions of intergenerational closure (e.g., “How many of the parents of your friends know your parents?”), social integration (e.g., “How many of your teachers do your parents know by name?”), and perceived opportunity for work (e.g., “Employers around here often hire young people from this neighbourhood?”). Total scores on the combined Closure and Integration subscale provided by the Pathways investigators were used. This scale had adequate psychometric properties: Cronbach alpha = .74, CFA fit indices RMR = .059, GFI = .954, RMSEA = .084. This indicator was reverse coded, so that higher scores indicate a lower degree of perceived community connectedness.

Neighborhood physical disorder was measured by an adaptation of a neighborhood conditions measure by Sampson and Raudenbush (Reference Sampson and Raudenbush1999). Items from the self-report measure tap physical disorder (e.g., “cigarettes on the street or in the gutters,” “graffiti or tags”), and social disorder (e.g., “adults fighting or arguing loudly,” “people using needles or syringes to take drugs”) of the neighborhood. We used the physical disorder subscale, which is the mean of the 12 physical disorder items. Higher scores indicate a greater degree of physical disorder within the community. This scale had excellent reliabilities: baseline Cronbach alpha = .91; 6 month Cronbach alpha = .94; 12 month Cronbach alpha = .94; 18 month Cronbach alpha = .93; 24 month Cronbach alpha = .94.

Unstructured socializing was measured by items drawn from the Monitoring the Future Questionnaire (Osgood et al., Reference Osgood, Wilson, O’Malley, Bachman and Johnston1996). The four items tap into unstructured activities with peers, that happen in the absence of an authority figure (e.g., “How often did you get together with friends informally?”). There is a crucial difference between the original coding of these items, and ours. Namely, that in Osgood et al. (Reference Osgood, Wilson, O’Malley, Bachman and Johnston1996), the authors investigated the relationship between unsupervised socializing and drug use and delinquency, based on the idea that less supervision in these activities allows for more dangerous or criminal behavior. Whereas, in our framework, we consider the lack of opportunities for unstructured socializing to be indicative of greater social deprivation. As such, a total score of the mean of the four items was utilized, and reverse coded, so that higher scores indicate less unstructured socializing. This scale had low, but acceptable levels of reliability: baseline Cronbach alpha = .62; 6 month Cronbach alpha = .73; 12 month Cronbach alpha = .70; 18 month Cronbach alpha = .70; 24 month Cronbach alpha = .68.

Threat

Threat was measured using five indicators of Exposure to violence, Maternal hostility, Peer delinquent behavior, Peer delinquent influence, and Neighbourhood social disorder. What ties these indicators together is that they are indicative of family and neighborhood environments characterized by a high degree of violence, crime and threatening situations (Berman et al., Reference Berman, McLaughlin, Tottenham, Godfrey, Seeman, Loucks, Suomi, Danese and Sheridan2022; Ellis et al., Reference Ellis, Sheridan, Belsky and McLaughlin2022; McLaughlin et al., Reference McLaughlin, Sheridan, Humphreys, Belsky and Ellis2021).

Exposure to violence was measured by an adaptation of the Exposure to Violence Inventory (Selner-O’Hagan et al., Reference Selner-O’Hagan, Kindlon, Buka, Raudenbush and Earls1998). Items tap into multiple types of violence that the youth either experienced directly (i.e., Victim – 6 items, e.g., “Have you been chased where you thought you might be seriously hurt in the past month?”) or observed (i.e., Witnessed – 7 items, e.g., “Have you seen someone else being raped, an attempt made to rape someone or any other type of sexual attack in the past month?”). In addition to these items about experiences with violent incidents, four questions inquire about the youth’s exposure to death (e.g. has anyone close to you tried to kill him/her self in the past month, has anyone close to you died in the past month, have you found a dead body in the past month, have you tried to kill yourself in the past month). Finally, one open-ended item assesses involvement in other types of situations which could have led to death or serious injury. In total, this scale inquires about 18 types of situations. We used the total sum score, reflecting the total number of violent situations that the youth either experienced or witnessed. Higher scores indicate a greater exposure to violence. This scale had low, but acceptable levels of reliability: baseline Cronbach alpha = .67; 6 month Cronbach alpha = .75; 12 month Cronbach alpha = .74; 18 month Cronbach alpha = .75; 24 month Cronbach alpha = .75. We note that this scale includes items assessing exposure to self-harm and suicide, which might inflate its association with mental health outcomes. However, given the large number and variety of items included in our overall threat composite, it is unlikely that this single item would significantly inflate associations. Indeed, as seen in the results below, the association between threat and Antisocial traits was even higher than with Borderline traits, even though the former show a much weaker (although not absent, see Verona et al., Reference Verona, Patrick and Joiner2001) association with suicide risk. This scale also includes items assessing sexual assault/rape exposure. Sexual assault and rape incur harm to the victims and are often accompanied by additional physical violence. For female victims, sexual assault and rape is significantly linked with a dysregulation of the reproductive physiology (medically explained missing menstrual periods, and medically unexplained dysmenorrhea, menstrual irregularity) and a decreased sexual activity (less sexual desire, more pain during intercourse, lack of sexual pleasure) (Golding, Reference Golding1996). For these reasons, sexual assault and rape can be said to threaten two fundamental biological goals, i.e., survival and reproduction. In addition, sexual assault and rape are generally considered as prototypical examples of threating and violent experiences (Chen et al., Reference Chen, Murad, Paras, Colbenson, Sattler, Goranson, Elamin, Seime, Shinozaki, Prokop and Zirakzadeh2010; Domino et al., Reference Domino, Whiteman, Weathers, Blevins and Davis2020; Dworkin et al., Reference Dworkin, Menon, Bystrynski and Allen2017; Nickerson et al., Reference Nickerson, Steenkamp, Aerka, Salters-Pedneault, Carper, Barnes and Litz2013; Siegel et al., Reference Siegel, Golding, Stein, Burnam and Sorenson1990), which warrant their inclusion in our composite.

Maternal hostility was measured with an adaptation of the Quality of Parental Relationships Inventory (Conger et al., Reference Conger, Ge, Elder, Lorenz and Simons1994). Items from the measure tap parental warmth (e.g., “How often does your mother let you know she really cares about you?”) and parental hostility (e.g., “How often does your mother get angry at you?”). We used the subscale reflecting maternal hostility, and reverse coded the indicator so that higher scores indicate a more hostile relationship with the youth’s mother. A four-factor model fit to the overall scale resulted in acceptable fit: NFI = .78, NNFI = .82, CFI = .83, RMSEA = .06. The maternal warmth subscale specifically had the following CFI goodness of fit at baseline: NFI = .73, NNFI = .69, CFI = .74, RMSEA = .09. It also had good reliability, baseline Cronbach alpha = .85; 6 month Cronbach alpha = .80; 12 month Cronbach alpha = .82; 18 month Cronbach alpha = .79; 24 month Cronbach alpha = .82.

Peer delinquent behavior and Peer delinquent influence were measured by a subset of items from the Rochester Youth Study (Thornberry et al., Reference Thornberry, Lizotte, Krohn, Farnworth and Jang1994). These items assess the degree of antisocial activity among the youth’s peers, along two dimensions: Antisocial behavior (e.g., “During the recall period how many of your friends have sold drugs?”) and Antisocial influence (e.g., “During the recall period how many of your friends have suggested that you should sell drugs?”). The scale contains 19 items in total. We used the Antisocial behavior score, which is the mean rating of the prevalence of friends who engage in the 12 behaviors listed in this section and the Antisocial influence score, which is the mean rating of the prevalence of friends who encourage the youth to engage in the seven items listed in this section. Higher scores indicate a greater degree of peer antisocial behavior and influence. Both scales had acceptable CFA model fits, Peer Delinquency-Antisocial behavior: NFI = .93, NNFI = .92, CFI = .94, RMSEA = .09; Peer Delinquency-Antisocial influence: NFI = .95, NNFI = .93, CFI = .96, RMSEA = .07. Their reliabilities were also good, Peer Delinquency-Antisocial behavior: baseline Cronbach alpha = .92; 6 months Cronbach alpha = .89; 12 months Cronbach alpha = .89; 18 months Cronbach alpha = .89; 24 months Cronbach alpha = .91; 30 months Cronbach alpha = .90; 36 months Cronbach alpha = .88; 48 months Cronbach alpha = .88; 60 months Cronbach alpha = .89; 72 months Cronbach alpha = .88. 84 months Cronbach alpha = .87; Peer Delinquency-Antisocial influence: baseline Cronbach alpha = .89; 6 months Cronbach alpha = .93; 12 months Cronbach alpha = .94; 18 months Cronbach alpha = .94; 24 months Cronbach alpha = .94; 30 months Cronbach alpha = .93; 36 months Cronbach alpha = .93; 48 months Cronbach alpha = .94; 60 months Cronbach alpha = .94; 72 months Cronbach alpha = .94; 84 months Cronbach alpha = .93.

Neighborhood social disorder was measured by an adaptation of a neighborhood conditions measure by Sampson and Raudenbush (Reference Sampson and Raudenbush1999). Items from the self-report measure tap physical disorder (e.g., “cigarettes on the street or in the gutters,” “graffiti or tags”), and social disorder (e.g., “adults fighting or arguing loudly,” “people using needles or syringes to take drugs”) of the neighborhood. We used the social disorder subscale, which is the mean of the 9 social disorder items. Higher scores indicate a greater degree of social disorder within the community. This scale had excellent reliabilities: baseline Cronbach alpha = .87; 6 month Cronbach alpha = .92; 12 month Cronbach alpha = .92; 18 month Cronbach alpha = .92; 24 month Cronbach alpha = .92.

Mediators

Future orientation was measured by the Future Outlook Inventory, developed for the Pathways to Desistance study, using items from the Life Orientation Task (Scheier & Carver, Reference Scheier and Carver1985), the Zimbardo Time Perspective Scale (Zimbardo & Boyd, Reference Zimbardo and Boyd1999), and the Consideration of Future Consequences Scale (Strathman et al., Reference Strathman, Gleicher, Boninger and Edwards1994). The inventory contains 15 items asking participants to rank from 1 to 4 (1 = Never True to 4 = Always True) the degree to which each statement reflects how they usually are (e.g., I will keep working at difficult, boring tasks if I know they will help me get ahead later). Higher scores indicate a greater degree of future consideration and planning. We used the data from the 72 months follow-up assessment. A one factor CFA at baseline resulted in the following fit indices: NFI = .96, NNFI = .96; CFI = .97, RMSEA = .03. The scale also had acceptable reliabilities: baseline Cronbach alpha = .68; 6 month Cronbach alpha = .73; 12 month Cronbach alpha = .70; 18 month Cronbach alpha = .72; 24 month Cronbach alpha = .69

Mental health

Antisocial traits and Borderline traits were both measured by the associated clinical scales of the Personality Assessment Inventory (PAI, Morey, Reference Morey1991). The Borderline Features items focus on attributes indicative of a borderline personality, including unstable and fluctuating interpersonal relations, impulsivity, affective lability and instability, and uncontrolled anger. Subscales are: affective instability, identify problems, negative relationships and self-harm. The Antisocial Features items focus on a history of illegal acts and authority problems, egocentrism, lack of empathy and loyalty, instability, and excitement-seeking. Subscales are: antisocial behaviors, egocentricity, and stimulus-seeking. We used the total raw Antisocial and Borderline subscale scores, reflecting the sum of all associated items. Higher scores are indicative of more Antisocial and Borderline personality features. We used the data from the 72 month follow-up assessment. Reliability information for these scales was not provided by the investigators of the Pathways dataset, but Boyle and Lennon (Reference Boyle and Lennon1994) found good internal consistency for both scales: Borderline Cronbach alpha = .88; Antisocial Cronbach alpha = .85.

Psychological distress was measured by the Brief Symptom Inventory (BSI, Derogatis & Melisaratos, Reference Derogatis and Melisaratos1983). The BSI is a 53-item self-report inventory in which participants rate the extent to which they have been bothered (0 =”not at all” to 4=”extremely”) in the past week by various symptoms. We used the global severity index, which is the mean score of all items. We used the data from the 72 month follow-up assessment as the primary outcome measure. The global severity index had excellent reliability: 6 months Cronbach alpha = .95; 12 months Cronbach alpha = .96; 18 months Cronbach alpha = .96; 24 months Cronbach alpha = .96; 30 months Cronbach alpha = .96; 36 months Cronbach alpha = .96; 48 months Cronbach alpha = .96; 60 months Cronbach alpha = .96; 72 months Cronbach alpha = .96; 84 months Cronbach alpha = .96.

Covariates

Gender was measured as participants self-reported gender at Baseline, and coded as Male (0) or Female (1).

Ethnicity was measured by participants self-reported ethnicity as baseline, and was recoded into 2 categories of White (0), and non-White (1), as members of minority racial and ethnic groups likely experience an overall higher level of environmental adversity (Pascoe & Smart Richman, Reference Pascoe and Smart Richman2009; Williams et al., Reference Williams, Lawrence and Davis2019), making it a potential confounding factor.

Site refers to the study site location, that is, the geographic site where the subject is located. It was coded as either Philadelphia (0) or Phoenix (1).

Age refers to participants age at Baseline (in years), calculated as interview date minus the participant’s date of birth truncated to a whole number.

It was important to control for preexisting internalizing and externalizing problem behaviors, to make sure any results we uncover during the study period are not due to their confounding effects. To this end we made use of Early onset problem behaviors (proxy of primarily externalizing behaviors before age 11) and the BSI psychological distress scores at Baseline (proxy of primarily internalizing behaviors at recruitment).

Early onset of behavioral problems was measured by a series of five questions that assess whether the participant got in trouble for cheating, disturbing class, being drunk/stoned, stealing, or fighting, before the age of 11. The score is simply the count of the number of early onset problems that were endorsed.

Psychological distress at Baseline was measured by the global psychological distress scale of the BSI, as described above.

Missing data

Little’s test (Little, Reference Little1988) indicated that the data are not missing completely at random, χ 2(238) = 491, p < .001. However, pairwise comparisons of missingness patterns indicated that missingness in our outcome variables was systematically related to our explanatory variables, such that missing at random (MAR) could still be supported (Supplementary tables S12, S13, S14). We thus handled missing data using Full Information Maximum Likelihood (FIML), which is appropriate for cases of MAR and outperforms other approaches, such as listwise deletion (Baraldi & Enders, Reference Baraldi and Enders2010; Enders & Bandalos, Reference Enders and Bandalos2001). Thus, our sample consisted of all 1354 participants of the Pathways dataset. Fraction of missing values for primary variables are presented in Supplementary Table S11.

Data analytic plan

Our analytic plan consisted of two stages. In the first stage, we used linear mixed models (LMMs) to create individual trajectories of adversity exposure for each variable and used these to derive estimates of both typical levels of deprivation and threat exposure, and temporal variability in deprivation and threat exposure. In the second stage, we constructed a path analytic model to investigate the associations of these average and unpredictability adversity composite scores with later life mental health outcomes, as well as the mediating effect of future orientation. We now detail these stages in turn.

In order to derive indices of average and unpredictable adversity, we adapted the longitudinal modeling approach of Li and Belsky (Li et al., Reference Li, Liu, Hartman and Belsky2018; Li & Belsky, Reference Li and Belsky2022), which itself is based on earlier work by Hoffman (Reference Hoffman2007). This involved fitting a series of LMMs for each adversity indicator, with both a fixed effect of time, as well as subject-specific random intercept and slopes. Time was a mean-centered variable, with 7 levels, reflecting the Baseline (−3), and the 6 (−2), 12 (−1), 18 (0), 24 (1), 30 (2), and 36 (3) month follow-up assessments. These models thus resulted in predicted trajectories for each adversity indicator, for each individual. The estimated intercepts of these models served to represent typical levels of exposure to that adversity factor for each individual. The root mean squared error of the individual level models served to represent temporal unpredictability in exposure to that adversity factor across the studied period. The strength of this approach is that it allows us to separate systematic, and thus predictable variability from random variability. This is an important point, as most theoretical frameworks for the role of unpredictability in development, including the evolutionary developmental approach we adopt, concern the effects of primarily random variability (Ellis et al., Reference Ellis, Figueredo, Brumbach and Schlomer2009; Evans et al., Reference Evans, Gonnella, Marcynyszyn, Gentile and Salpekar2005; Young et al., Reference Young, Frankenhuis and Ellis2020). Furthermore, this approach also allows for a straightforward way of handling missing data at the indicator level, as both model-predicted intercepts and residuals can be calculated even for subjects with missing data on some timepoints, without any need for specific missing data approaches.

After these average and unpredictability scores were calculated for each indicator, they were then z-scored in the whole sample to equalize scale and summed to create specific composite scores for deprivation and threat. Thus, average scores on the Caring adults, Maternal warmth, Social capital, Neighbourhood physical disorder, and Unstructured socializing indicators were summed into a Deprivation average composite score, whereas unpredictability scores on these same indicators were summed into a Deprivation unpredictability composite score. In the same manner, average scores on the Exposure to violence, Maternal hostility, Peer delinquent behavior, Peer delinquent influence, and Neighbourhood social disorder indicators were summed into a Threat average composite score, whereas unpredictability scores on these same indicators were summed into a Threat unpredictability composite score. Finally, expanding on the methodology earlier work (Li et al., Reference Li, Liu, Hartman and Belsky2018; Li & Belsky, Reference Li and Belsky2022), Deprivation average × Deprivation unpredictability and Threat average × Threat unpredictability interactions terms were also calculated by multiplying the respective average and unpredictability variables. As a result, we were able to approximate individual exposure to multiple dimensions of adversity and separate the effects of mean levels of exposure from temporal unpredictability in exposure, as well as their interaction. In order to keep our model simple, and the lack of previous empirical demonstration of cross dimensional interactions, we decided not to add them into the model. Details on the models are provided in Supplementary Tables S1 to S10, and trajectories of individual indicators are plotted in Supplementary Figures S1−S10. We also computed a set of other unpredictability metrics and combined them with the same sum of z-scores approach. This included the standard deviation, the entropy, and the first order autocorrelation, inspired by Walasek et al. (Reference Walasek, Young and Frankenhuis2024), as well as a score that reflected the mean percentage change between timepoints, suggested by a reviewer. We investigated the agreement between these various metrics, as well as the similarity between their associations with outcomes by bivariate correlations.

We then related these six adversity composite scores (4 main effects + 2 interaction terms) to later mental health and mediator variables. Specifically, we created a path analytic, mediation model, in which the six adversity composites had direct effects on the three mental health outcomes (Antisocial traits, Borderline traits, Psychological distress), as well as indirect effects through Future orientation. Control variables of Early onset problem behaviors, Baseline psychological distress, Baseline age, Ethnicity, Sex, and Site were also incorporated into the model by regressing them on each variable. Covariances between the three mental health variables were also specified. This resulted in a saturated model. The model was fit using Robust Maximum Likelihood estimation (MLR estimator) and missing data was handled with FIML. We also perform sensitivity analyses with two alternative models. A first alternative model with listwise deletion of participants with missing values, instead of FIML; and a second alternative model with bootstrapped standard errors and standard ML estimation, instead of MLR.

All pre-processing, analysis and visualization was carried out in R version 4.2.3 (R Core Team, 2021), with the help of the tidyverse (Wickham et al., Reference Wickham, Averick, Bryan, Chang, McGowan, François, Grolemund, Hayes, Henry, Hester, Kuhn, Pedersen, Miller, Bache, Müller, Ooms, Robinson, Seidel, Spinu, Takahashi, Vaughan, Wilke, Woo and Yutani2019), ggplot2 (Wickham, Reference Wickham2016), afex (Singmann et al., Reference Singmann, Bolker, Westfall, Aust and Ben-Shachar2023), lavaan (Rosseel, Reference Rosseel2012), and psych (Revelle, Reference Revelle2022) packages. All p values reported are two-tailed, with alpha set to .05. Effect sizes are reported as standardized betas. No outliers were removed. As we only interpret the magnitudes and the overall patterns of correlations to determine the agreement between unpredictability metrics and their functional consequences, and draw no inference regarding any individual bivariate correlation, we do not correct these correlation matrices for multiple comparisons.

All pre-processed data and code necessary to reproduce all results in the paper are available on the OSF framework (https://osf.io/src3u/). The raw data that support the findings of this study are similarly available after the appropriate steps from the Pathways to Desistance study website (https://www.pathwaysstudy.pitt.edu/index.html). This study was not pre-registered.

Results

Unpredictability metrics

Our primary metric of unpredictability was a LMM residual based score (based on Li et al., Reference Li, Liu, Hartman and Belsky2018 and Li & Belsky, Reference Li and Belsky2022), which captures stochastic temporal variability of individual adversity trajectories around the overall group level linear trend. In addition to this, we also computed multiple unpredictability metrics within the deprivation and threat dimensions, including the standard deviation, the first order autocorrelation, the entropy (based on Walasek et al., Reference Walasek, Young and Frankenhuis2024), and a percentage change score. Bivariate correlations among these metrics are presented in Figures 1a,b, and between these metrics and our outcomes in Figure 1c, and their distributions are plotted in Supplementary Figure S11. Within both deprivation and threat, the highest agreements were found between our model residual score, the standard deviation and the percentage change score, first order autocorrelation had weaker associations with the other metrics. Surprisingly, entropy showed negative correlations with other unpredictability metrics. In a similar vein, bivariate correlations of the metrics with our outcome variables suggested that the model residual score, standard deviation, and percentage change score share similar associations with future orientation and mental health. Whereas the first order autocorrelation seemed the least related to all outcomes and entropy again showing associations in the opposing direction. All of this suggests that our proposed random effects model residual score, intended to measure stochastic temporal variability captures similar unpredictability types as other previously proposed measures. On the other hand, first order autocorrelation and entropy seemed to capture entirely independent sources of variability, even though, similarly to our model residual score, it is intended to capture unpredictable variability. This is likely due to our relatively small number of datapoints, making the estimation of autocorrelation and entropy inaccurate. Based on these patterns, we decided to use our model residual score as our primary unpredictability metric, and to ease readability, we henceforth refer to it as Threat unpredictability and Deprivation unpredictability.

Figure 1. Bivariate Spearman’s correlations of unpredictability metrics. (a) Correlations between multiple unpredictability metrics of the threat dimension. ( b) Correlations between multiple unpredictability metrics of the deprivation dimension. ( c) Correlations of multiple unpredictability metrics of both dimensions and outcomes. In all figures, p values are uncorrected for multiple comparisons. Our candidate linear mixed model residual based metric, that is used in the path analysis are highlighted in red.

Path analysis

We fit a path analytic, mediation model, that related composite scores reflecting adolescent deprivation and threat exposure to later life future orientation, and mental health outcomes. Importantly, we created separate terms for average adversity exposure levels, unpredictability in exposure levels, and the interaction between the two. Correlations between primary variables are presented in Table 2, and histograms showing their distribution are presented in Supplementary Figure S12. To aid the clarity of the presentation of the results, we refrain from quoting the statistics in the main text. Parameter estimates are reported in Table 3, and a simplified graphical representation of the model’s results is presented in Figure 2. The model was saturated, therefore standard goodness of fit indices are not available. R2 metrics indicate that the model explained 26% of variance in Antisocial traits, 22% of variance in Borderline traits, and 13% of variance in Psychological distress. The three mental health outcomes had moderate to strong positive covariances. Given the large correlation between the Threat unpredictability threat and Threat average scores, we tested the impact of multicollinearity on our estimates, by calculating VIFs from a regression model corresponding to the prediction of Future orientation from our set of adversity variables. This indicated that while the Threat average and Threat unpredictability variables do indeed have the highest VIF values, they are still only 2.29 and 2.73, respectively, much below the usually recommended cutoff of 10 (Kutner et al., Reference Kutner, Nachtsheim, Neter and Li2004; Myers, Reference Myers1990).

Figure 2. Simplified representation of the path analytic mediation model. Indicators and standardized parameter estimates. Statistically significant regression paths and covariances are represented by single and double headed arrows, respectively.

Table 2. Bivariate Spearman’s correlations between primary variables

P values are uncorrected for multiple comparisons.

* p < .05 ** p < .01 *** p < .001

Table 3. Parameter estimates of the path analytic mediation model

Statistically significant effects are highlighted in bold. Selected indirect effects that are suggested by the individual parameter estimates are also tested and included in the table. Effects of covariates are omitted for clarity. Full list of parameters is available in the .Rdata file provided in the supplementary materials.

We also performed two sensitivity analyses. A first alternative model has the same specification as our main model, but is fit with listwise deletion of participants with missing values, instead of FIML. This model allows us to detect which effects are highly dependent on our missing data handling method, however, due to the greatly reduced sample also has lower statistical power. A second alternative model also has the same specification as our main model, but is fit with bootstrapped standard errors and standard ML estimation, instead of MLR. This model allows us to obtain estimates of parameters and confidence intervals without relying on the assumption of normality, and investigate the precision of these estimates.

Direct effects of adversity dimensions on psychopathology

Results indicated that Threat average had significant, positive direct effects on all three of our mental health outcomes, and Deprivation unpredictability had positive direct effects on Borderline traits, and Psychological distress, with Threat average being associated with generally larger effect sizes (Table 3 and Figure 2). The interaction terms were not significant. This means that exposure to highly variable levels of deprivation (independently of its average level) and high average levels of threat (independently of its variability) during adolescence, both contribute to a higher number of Borderline and Antisocial personality features, and a greater degree of general psychological distress, later in life. Surprisingly, Threat unpredictability was found to negatively predict Psychological distress.

Direct effects of adversity dimensions on future orientation

Threat average and Deprivation average were also significantly negatively associated with Future orientation, indicating that high average levels of exposure to both dimensions of adversity is associated with a more present-oriented outlook (Table 3 and Figure 2). Interestingly, Deprivation unpredictability was positively associated with Future orientation, suggesting that contrary to average levels, a greater degree of variability in deprivation exposure might be associated with more, and not less future oriented thinking. While not reaching our set level of statistical significance, we observed a small effect of the Threat unpredictability × Threat average interaction, on future orientation. As the nature of such interactive effects is an unresolved question in the literature, we performed a simple slopes analysis to investigate which pattern the interaction effects observed here match, at least qualitatively. These revealed effects consistent with the buffering pattern, whereby high levels of threat were associated with the strongest negative effects on development (more present orientation), while coupled with low unpredictability (Supplementary Figure S13).

Direct effects of future orientation on psychopathology

Future orientation was significantly negatively associated with Borderline and Antisocial traits, but not with Psychological distress, indicating that a more present-oriented outlook is associated with a higher number of Borderline and Antisocial personality features, but not with general psychological distress (Table 3 and Figure 2).

Indirect effects of adversity dimensions on psychopathology through future orientation

We finally tested the indirect effects of the adversity dimensions on psychopathology, routed through future orientation, that were suggested by the path estimates (Figure 2). Specifically, we estimated the indirect effect of Deprivation average, Deprivation unpredictability, and Threat average on Antisocial and Borderline traits (Table 3). Deprivation average had a significant, positive indirect effect on both Antisocial (b = 0.115, 95% CI = [0.054, 0.176], p < .001) and Borderline traits (b = 0.126, 95% CI = [0.059, 0.176], p < .001). Threat average also had a significant, positive indirect effect on both Antisocial (b = 0.068, 95% CI = [0.006, 0.129], p = .031) and Borderline traits (b = 0.075, 95% CI = [0.008, 0.141], p = .028). The indirect effect of Deprivation unpredictability in association with greater future orientation, and thereby, lower Antisocial (b = −0.066, 95% CI = [−0.126, −0.007], p = .029) and Borderline traits (b = −0.073, 95% CI = [−0.139, −0.008], p = .029) also proved statistically significant.

Sensitivity analyses

Parameter estimates for the model proved relatively robust in a sensitivity analysis with listwise deletion of missing values (Supplementary Table S16). There were two important differences however: the link between Deprivation unpredictability and Future orientation turned non-significant, which in turn rendered the indirect effects of Deprivation unpredictability non-significant as well; and the direct effect of Deprivation unpredictability on Antisocial and Borderline traits turned significant. This suggests, that the estimates of these effects might be biased by our handling of missing data. However, it also has to be noted that this model had substantially smaller sample size (N = 736, instead of N = 1354 for the main model), which means it was more likely to be underpowered.

Parameters were entirely robust in a second alternative model with bootstrapped standard errors, and standard ML estimation (Supplementary Table S17). Both parameter estimates themselves and their confidence intervals were extremely similar compared to the main model. This suggests that, our main model does not suffer from a significant bias from violation of distributional assumptions.

Discussion

The present paper examined the effect of exposure to multiple dimensions of environmental adversity during adolescence, on later life Antisocial and Borderline personality traits, and psychological distress, as well as the mediating role of future orientation. Importantly, we distinguished between typical levels of adversity exposure, and variability in adversity exposure. All of this allowed for a fine-grained level of analysis. Results were mostly consistent with our hypotheses. High average levels of both deprivation (defined as mortality-morbidity risk from lack of environmental input and nurture) and threat (defined as mortality-morbidity risk from physical and psychological harm) were related to more present orientation. Present orientation, coupled with impulsivity, risk-taking, and steeper discount rates has been robustly linked to early life adversity, and is an important component of life history-inspired models of development (Copping et al., Reference Copping, Campbell and Muncer2014; Farkas et al., Reference Farkas, Chambon and Jacquet2021; Hill et al., Reference Hill, Jenkins and Farmer2008; Lee et al., Reference Lee, DeBruine and Jones2018; Martinez et al., Reference Martinez, Hasty, Morabito, Maranges, Schmidt and Maner2022; Wu et al., Reference Wu, Guo, Gao and Kou2020). A more present-oriented decision-making style, along with a lower perceived sense of control is a core component of what Pepper and Nettle (Reference Pepper and Nettle2017) called the behavioral constellation of deprivation, and has also been highlighted as an important mediator of socioeconomic status effects on decision-making by Sheehy-Skeffington (Reference Sheehy-Skeffington2020). In these theoretical accounts, such decision-making reflects a contextually appropriate response to adverse conditions, that lower the certainty of being able to collect and benefit from delayed rewards. In another, not mutually exclusive model, Mell et al. (Reference Mell, Baumard and André2021) argued that discounting can also reflect the costs of waiting, in the sense of losing out on potential gains in biological and social capital during the waiting period. Our results are consistent with either mechanism. It is also important to note, that temporal impulsivity is only an adaptive strategy under certain conditions, for example, when organisms are close to critical thresholds, resources are predictable, or interruptions are common (Fenneman et al., Reference Fenneman, Frankenhuis and Todd2022; Fenneman & Frankenhuis, Reference Fenneman and Frankenhuis2020). Given this, it is intriguing that whereas average levels of deprivation were associated with more present orientation, unpredictability in deprivation (corresponding with low levels of resource predictability) was associated with more future orientation. We did not observe a direct effect of average deprivation on any of the mental health outcomes. The lack of a direct deprivation effect on mental health is not surprising, given that it is usually more strongly associated with cognitive and linguistic outcomes, which stand in contrast of the more direct link of threat with emotional processing and mental health (McLaughlin et al., Reference McLaughlin, Weissman and Bitrán2019; Miller et al., Reference Miller, Sheridan, Hanson, McLaughlin, Bates, Lansford, Pettit and Dodge2018, Reference Miller, Machlin, McLaughlin and Sheridan2021). However, our results do suggest that deprivation can have an impact on mental health indirectly through more present orientation, and perhaps through the induction of psychosocial acceleration more broadly. This possibly important indirect link remains to be further explored.

The pattern of associations of present orientation with the mental health outcomes is also entirely in line with our hypotheses based on evolutionary approaches to psychopathology. Notably, in the model of Del Giudice (Del Giudice, Reference Del Giudice2018; Del Giudice & Haltigan, Reference Del Giudice and Haltigan2023), the taxonomic space of psychopathology is oriented along two, largely orthogonal axes of fast-slow life history strategy, and prolonged defense activation. Fast spectrum disorders comprise primarily externalizing conditions, such as Antisocial, Borderline and Narcissistic personality disorder, Schizophrenia spectrum disorders and some eating disorders. On the opposing end, Slow spectrum disorders comprise primarily subtypes of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder, Obsessive Compulsive Disorder and Autism spectrum disorders, as well as Obsessive Compulsive Personality Disorder. Finally, a third cluster is formed by defense activation type disorders, which comprise primarily internalizing conditions, including Depression, Post Traumatic Stress Disorder, Phobias and Generalized anxiety. While both fast spectrum and defense activation disorders are proposed to be more prevalent in dangerous and unpredictable environments, the mechanisms of such effects are somewhat different. Risk for fast spectrum conditions reflects an adaptive developmental response to adversity in the form of “fast” life history strategies, which as explained above, likely includes a psychological component of present-oriented decision-making. Risk for defense activation type conditions instead reflects the upregulation of adaptive defense mechanisms to intense and/or prolonged exposure to stress and is therefore relatively unrelated to broader life history strategies and future outlook. This line of reasoning would thus predict that while adversity should be associated with worse outcomes on all three measures, only the effect on fast spectrum traits should be mediated by present orientation. If we interpret Antisocial and Borderline personality features (as measured by the PAI) as markers of Fast spectrum symptoms, and general psychological distress (as measured by the BSI) as markers of defense activation type symptoms, this is exactly what we find.

The associations of unpredictability proved more complex. On the one hand, unpredictability in deprivation had positive direct effects on mental health outcomes, dovetailing a large body of previous findings, highlighting unpredictability as a major risk factor for “problematic” child development, above and beyond the effect of harshness (Brumbach et al., Reference Brumbach, Figueredo and Ellis2009; Doom et al., Reference Doom, Vanzomeren-Dohm and Simpson2016; Glynn et al., Reference Glynn, Howland, Sandman, Davis, Phelan, Baram and Stern2018; Li et al., Reference Li, Liu, Hartman and Belsky2018; Li & Belsky, Reference Li and Belsky2022; Simpson et al., Reference Simpson, Griskevicius, Kuo, Sung and Collins2012; Szepsenwol et al., Reference Szepsenwol, Griskevicius, Simpson, Young, Fleck and Jones2017; Wu, Reference Wu2024). One way in which our findings extend these earlier results is by suggesting that it is unpredictability in the dimension of deprivation that might be responsible for these associations. In our study, a high and stable level of danger, and inconsistency in resource availability and stimulation have emerged as the strongest drivers of mental health problems. However, in addition to these main effects, unpredictability in deprivation had a small, positive association with future orientation, i.e., in the opposing direction to the effect of average deprivation and threat. Similarly, unpredictability in threat had a negative effect on general psychological distress. Finally, while not reaching our set level of statistical significance, an interaction was observed between average and unpredictable threat on future orientation. This interaction seemed to suggest a buffering pattern, whereby high typical levels of threat had the strongest effect, when it was coupled with low unpredictability. These results are not entirely unexpected, as multiple studies have revealed similar interactions between harshness and unpredictability on developmental outcomes (Li et al., Reference Li, Liu, Hartman and Belsky2018; Li & Belsky, Reference Li and Belsky2022). Li and Belsky (Reference Li and Belsky2022) interpret these effects in light of Bayesian models of plasticity, proposing that environmental unpredictability effectively lowers the reliability of cues, that organisms use to guide development (Frankenhuis & Panchanathan, Reference Frankenhuis and Panchanathan2011; Stamps & Frankenhuis, Reference Stamps and Frankenhuis2016). All else being equal, a stable high level of adversity offers stronger evidence for future environmental adversity, than a variable level of adversity with the same mean. This reasoning also echoes findings from human reinforcement learning and decision-making, highlighting that certain kinds of uncertainty should downregulate learning rates, and that humans indeed behave in this way (Lee et al., Reference Lee, Rouault and Wyart2023; Piray & Daw, Reference Piray and Daw2021; Story et al., Reference Story, Kurth-Nelson, Moutoussis, Iigaya, Will, Hauser, Blain, Vlaev and Dolan2023). These effects argue in favor of the statistical learning framework for unpredictability, proposing that individuals track the level of unpredictability in their environment across time and update their internal models accordingly (Young et al., Reference Young, Frankenhuis and Ellis2020). Under this conceptualization one could imagine low variability leading to more imprecision in the estimation of adversity, and by virtue of that, less strong or delayed developmental adaptation. Nevertheless, this reasoning is difficult to reconcile with the detrimental main effects of unpredictability, when modeled from indicators such as parental and residential transitions, that consistently emerge in the literature, even in the absence of harshness effects. If unpredictability operated solely through a reduction of cue reliability, then it should not be associated with aspects of psychosocial acceleration on its own, when not paired with high average levels of harshness. Another possible explanation lies in the potential confounding of income unpredictability (the primary measure in the studies of Li et al. (Reference Li, Liu, Hartman and Belsky2018) and Li & Belsky (Reference Li and Belsky2022)) by income level, as families with low income do not have much income that could vary. The relatively high reported correlations between their harshness and unpredictability variables (r = −.66, and r = −.45) as well as between our Average threat and Unpredictable threat scores (r = .70) lends credibility to this explanation. Future work with more diverse indicators, a fine temporal resolution, and formal modeling will be indispensable in understanding these effects (Frankenhuis et al., Reference Frankenhuis, Nettle and Dall2019). Similarly, while not the focus of this study, testing cross dimensional interactions between deprivation and threat is another important avenue for future research.

The notable strengths of our work have to be balanced against a number of limitations. While our primary aim was to capture the effects of nonshared environments, phenotypic variability in all constructs we investigated has notable genetic components (Niv et al., Reference Niv, Tuvblad, Raine, Wang and Baker2012; Reichborn-Kjennerud et al., Reference Reichborn-Kjennerud, Czajkowski, Ystrøm, Ørstavik, Aggen, Tambs, Torgersen, Neale, Røysamb, Krueger, Knudsen and Kendler2015; Richardson et al., Reference Richardson, Barbaro, Nedelec and Liu2023; Zheng et al., Reference Zheng, Chen, Li and Gan2022). Future work with genetically informative designs will be necessary to tease apart environmental and genetic influences, and their interactions. Secondly, while we believe our operationalization of unpredictability to be one of the strengths of our work, it also comes with limitations. Our composite scores likely gloss over important differences in the timescale of variability and type of unpredictability that different individuals encountered. As not all kinds of unpredictability are expected to have the same effects on learning and development, this imprecision will be important to address in future studies (Young et al., Reference Young, Frankenhuis and Ellis2020). In a similar vein, we focused on the statistical learning approach to conceptualize unpredictability and were unable to assess how this source of signal interacted with ancestral cues. The recent study of Li et al. (Reference Li, Sturge-Apple, Platts and Davies2023) highlighted important differences in the mechanisms that mediate the effects of these different sources of information. Thirdly, while our bootstrapped alternative model (Supplementary Table S17) yielded entirely converging results to our main model, the other alternative model with listwise deletion of participants with missing data (Supplementary Table S16) suggested that our estimates of the effects of Deprivation unpredictability might be biased by our missing data handling (FIML). Therefore, our uncovered associations require additional support and tests using samples with more complete datasets. Finally, the nature of our sample necessarily limits the generalizability of our findings. As it has been designed as a cohort to understand juvenile offending, the Pathways study is a necessarily non-representative sample of youth, with predominantly male offender participants from highly adverse backgrounds. This precluded any investigation of sex differences and leaves open the question of whether the associations we uncovered hold in lower-risk populations. In addition, while our large, rich, and longitudinal dataset was ideally posed for investigating variability in multiple adversity factors, it has to be noted that the number of datapoints per individual is much lower than what would be desirable for the accurate calculation of more complex unpredictability metrics, such as the autocorrelation and entropy. This is especially important, given the surprising negative correlation of entropy with other unpredictability metrics and our mental health outcomes. It is intriguing that Walasek et al. (Reference Walasek, Young and Frankenhuis2024) also observe a similar effect, and that multiple recent studies have drawn attention to the potentially important differences between different timescales and different degrees of predictability of variability (Farkas et al., Reference Farkas, Baptista, Speranza, Wyart and Jacquet2024; Munakata et al., Reference Munakata, Placido and Zhuang2023; Ugarte & Hastings, Reference Ugarte and Hastings2023; Young et al., Reference Young, Frankenhuis and Ellis2020). The timing of adversity exposure that we have considered here is also much later than the sensitive periods during which early life adversity is generally thought to mark a child’s development (Lussier et al., Reference Lussier, Zhu, Smith, Cerutti, Fisher, Melton, Wood, Cohen-Woods, Huang, Mitchell, Schneper, Notterman, Simpkin, Smith, Suderman, Walton, Relton, Ressler and Dunn2023; Simpson et al., Reference Simpson, Griskevicius, Kuo, Sung and Collins2012). Even though our effects are adjusted for preexisting internalizing and externalizing behaviors and psychological distress, the proximity in time of our various measurements does not allow us to draw strong conclusions about whether these effects stem from external predictive adaptive responses. Nevertheless, due to environmental continuity adolescent experiences likely correlate with early life ones. For example, in a longitudinal study, Simpson et al. (Reference Simpson, Griskevicius, Kuo, Sung and Collins2012) report a correlation of r = .67 between early (ages 0–5) and late (ages 6–16) harshness and a correlation of r = .42 between early (ages 0–5) and late (ages 6–16) unpredictability. In addition, psychological and biological processes that translate environmental adversity to developmental changes likely do not stop fully after initial sensitive periods and continue to operate throughout the lifecourse, albeit with possibly reduced strength. For example, even adversity experienced during adulthood has considerable impact on mental and physical health (Hajat et al., Reference Hajat, Nurius and Song2020; Liu et al., Reference Liu, Hatch, Patalay, Schott and Richards2023), and there is plenty of evidence suggesting that stress induced epigenetic changes are not restricted to early life (Doherty & Roth, Reference Doherty and Roth2016).

Notwithstanding these important limitations, we believe our study contributes to a growing understanding of the complex effects of multiple dimensions of early life environments on development and mental health. By disentangling typical levels of exposure, and variability in exposure, separately in the dimensions of deprivation and threat, we were able to highlight important differences in their developmental sequalae. Our results suggest that while high stable levels of danger, and variable levels of resource availability increase fast spectrum and distress symptoms, unpredictability has more complex associations, possibly reflecting its effects on cue reliability. Our approach highlights both the value of evolutionary developmental frameworks for understanding psychopathology (Del Giudice & Ellis, Reference Del Giudice, Ellis and Cicchetti2016), and the need for a greater degree of precision in our conceptualization of early life adversity (McLaughlin et al., Reference McLaughlin, Sheridan, Humphreys, Belsky and Ellis2021).

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at http://doi.org/10.1017/S095457942400155X.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank all members of the Institut du Psychotraumatisme de l’Enfant et de l’Adolescent and the Cognitive Variability Lab of the École Normale Supérieure for their helpful comments, as well as Magdalena Sabat and Regina Vitányi for their valuable feedback on the figures.

Funding statement

P.O.J. was supported by the Agence Nationale de la Recherche grant ANR-22-CE28-0012-01 eLIFUN (JCJC).

Competing interests

None.

References

Baraldi, A. N., & Enders, C. K. (2010). An introduction to modern missing data analyses. Journal of School Psychology, 48(1), 537. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsp.2009.10.001 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Belsky, J., Schlomer, G. L., & Ellis, B. J. (2012). Beyond cumulative risk: Distinguishing harshness and unpredictability as determinants of parenting and early life history strategy. Developmental Psychology, 48(3), 662673. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024454 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berman, I. S., McLaughlin, K. A., Tottenham, N., Godfrey, K., Seeman, T., Loucks, E., Suomi, S., Danese, A., & Sheridan, M. A. (2022). Measuring early life adversity: A dimensional approach. Development and Psychopathology, 34(2), 499511. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579421001826 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boyle, G. J., & Lennon, T. J. (1994). Examination of the reliability and validity of the personality assessment inventory. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 16(3), 173187. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02229206 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brumbach, B. H., Figueredo, A. J., & Ellis, B. J. (2009). Effects of harsh and unpredictable environments in adolescence on development of life history strategies: A longitudinal test of an evolutionary model. Human Nature, 20(1), 2551. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-009-9059-3 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bulley, A., & Pepper, G. V. (2017). Cross-country relationships between life expectancy, intertemporal choice and age at first birth. Evolution and Human Behavior, 38(5), 652658. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2017.05.002 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, L. P., Murad, M. H., Paras, M. L., Colbenson, K. M., Sattler, A. L., Goranson, E. N., Elamin, M. B., Seime, R. J., Shinozaki, G., Prokop, L. J., & Zirakzadeh, A. (2010). Sexual abuse and lifetime diagnosis of psychiatric disorders: Systematic review and meta-analysis. Mayo Clinic Proceedings, 85(7), 618629. https://doi.org/10.4065/mcp.2009.0583 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, S., & Wills, T. A. (1985). Stress, social support, and the buffering hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin, 98(2), 310357. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.98.2.310 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Conger, R. D., Ge, X., Elder, G. H., Lorenz, F. O., & Simons, R. L. (1994). Economic stress, coercive family process, and developmental problems of adolescents. Child Development, 65(2), 541. https://doi.org/10.2307/1131401 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Copping, L. T., Campbell, A., & Muncer, S. (2014). Conceptualizing time preference: A life-history analysis. Evolutionary Psychology, 12(4), 829847. https://doi.org/10.1177/147470491401200411 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Vries, C., Galipaud, M., & Kokko, H. (2023). Extrinsic mortality and senescence: A guide for the perplexed. Peer Community Journal, 3, e29. https://doi.org/10.24072/pcjournal.253 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Del Giudice, M. (2018). Evolutionary psychopathology: A unified approach. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Del Giudice, M., & Ellis, B. J. (2016). Evolutionary foundations of developmental psychopathology. In Cicchetti, D. (Ed.), Developmental psychopathology. (vol. 1, pp. 158). John Wiley & Sons, Inc. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781119125556.devpsy201 Google Scholar
Del Giudice, M., Gangestad, S. W., & Kaplan, H. S. (2015). Life history theory and evolutionary psychology. In Buss, D. M. (Ed.), The handbook of evolutionary psychology. (vol. 1, 2nd ed. pp. 88114). Wiley.Google Scholar
Del Giudice, M., & Haltigan, J. D. (2023). An integrative evolutionary framework for psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 35(1), 111. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579421000870 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Derogatis, L. R., & Melisaratos, N. (1983). The brief symptom inventory: An introductory report. Psychological Medicine, 13(3), 595605. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291700048017 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Doherty, T. S., & Roth, T. L. (2016). Insight from animal models of environmentally driven epigenetic changes in the developing and adult brain. Development and Psychopathology, 28(4pt2), 12291243. https://doi.org/10.1017/S095457941600081X CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Domino, J. L., Whiteman, S. E., Weathers, F. W., Blevins, C. T., & Davis, M. T. (2020). Predicting PTSD and depression following sexual assault: The role of perceived life threat, post-traumatic cognitions, victim-perpetrator relationship, and social support. Journal of Aggression, Maltreatment & Trauma, 29(6), 680698. https://doi.org/10.1080/10926771.2019.1710634 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Doom, J. R., Vanzomeren-Dohm, A. A., & Simpson, J. A. (2016). Early unpredictability predicts increased adolescent externalizing behaviors and substance use: A life history perspective. Development and Psychopathology, 28(4pt2), 15051516. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579415001169 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dworkin, E. R., Menon, S. V., Bystrynski, J., & Allen, N. E. (2017). Sexual assault victimization and psychopathology: A review and meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 56, 6581. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2017.06.002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ellis, B. J., Del Giudice, M., Dishion, T. J., Figueredo, A. J., Gray, P., Griskevicius, V., Hawley, P. H., Jacobs, W. J., James, J., Volk, A. A., & Wilson, D. S. (2012). The evolutionary basis of risky adolescent behavior: Implications for science, policy, and practice. Developmental Psychology, 48(3), 598623. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026220 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ellis, B. J., Figueredo, A. J., Brumbach, B. H., & Schlomer, G. L. (2009). Fundamental dimensions of environmental risk: The impact of harsh versus unpredictable environments on the evolution and development of life history strategies. Human Nature, 20(2), 204268. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-009-9063-7 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ellis, B. J., Sheridan, M. A., Belsky, J., & McLaughlin, K. A. (2022). Why and how does early adversity influence development? Toward an integrated model of dimensions of environmental experience. Development and Psychopathology, 34(2), 125. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579421001838 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Enders, C., & Bandalos, D. (2001). The relative performance of full information maximum likelihood estimation for missing data in structural equation models. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 8(3), 430457. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15328007SEM0803_5 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Evans, G. W., Gonnella, C., Marcynyszyn, L. A., Gentile, L., & Salpekar, N. (2005). The role of chaos in poverty and children’s socioemotional adjustment. Psychological Science, 16(7), 560565. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.01575.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Evans, G. W., Li, D., & Whipple, S. S. (2013). Cumulative risk and child development. Psychological Bulletin, 139(6), 13421396. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031808 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Farkas, B. C., Baptista, A., Speranza, M., Wyart, V., & Jacquet, P. O. (2024). Specifying the timescale of early life unpredictability helps explain the development of internalising and externalising behaviours. Scientific Reports, 14(1), 3563. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-024-54093-x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Farkas, B. C., Chambon, V., & Jacquet, P. O. (2021). Do perceived control and time orientation mediate the effect of early life adversity on reproductive behaviour and health status? Insights from the european value study and the european social survey. Humanities and Social Sciences Communications, 9(52), 114. https://doi.org/10.1057/s41599-022-01066-y Google Scholar
Fenneman, J., & Frankenhuis, W. E. (2020). Is impulsive behavior adaptive in harsh and unpredictable environments? A formal model. Evolution and Human Behavior, 41(4), 261273. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2020.02.005 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fenneman, J., Frankenhuis, W. E., & Todd, P. M. (2022). In which environments is impulsive behavior adaptive? A cross-discipline review and integration of formal models. Psychological Bulletin, 148(7-8), 555587. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000375 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frankenhuis, W. E., & Amir, D. (2022). What is the expected human childhood? Insights from evolutionary anthropology. Development and Psychopathology, 34(2), 473497. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579421001401 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frankenhuis, W. E., Nettle, D., & Dall, S. R. X. (2019). A case for environmental statistics of early-life effects. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 374(1770), 20180110. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2018.0110 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frankenhuis, W. E., & Panchanathan, K. (2011). Individual differences in developmental plasticity may result from stochastic sampling. Perspectives On Psychological Science, 6(4), 336347. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691611412602 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Glynn, L. M., Howland, M. A., Sandman, C. A., Davis, E. P., Phelan, M., Baram, T. Z., & Stern, H. S. (2018). Prenatal maternal mood patterns predict child temperament and adolescent mental health. Journal of Affective Disorders, 228, 8390. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2017.11.065 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Golding, J. M. (1996). Sexual assault history and women’s reproductive and sexual health. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 20(1), 101121. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1471-6402.1996.tb00667.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grummitt, L. R., Kreski, N. T., Kim, S. G., Platt, J., Keyes, K. M., & McLaughlin, K. A. (2021). Association of childhood adversity with morbidity and mortality in US adults: A systematic review. JAMA Pediatrics, 175(12), 1269. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapediatrics.2021.2320 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hajat, A., Nurius, P., & Song, C. (2020). Differing trajectories of adversity over the life course: Implications for adult health and well-being. Child Abuse & Neglect, 102, 104392. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2020.104392 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hill, E. M., Jenkins, J., & Farmer, L. (2008). Family unpredictability, future discounting, and risk taking. The Journal of Socio-Economics, 37(4), 13811396. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socec.2006.12.081 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoffman, L. (2007). Multilevel models for examining individual differences in within-person variation and covariation over time. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 42(4), 609629. https://doi.org/10.1080/00273170701710072 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kutner, M. H., Nachtsheim, C. J., Neter, J., & Li, W. (2004). Applied linear statistical models (5th ed.). McGraw-Hill Irwin.Google Scholar
Lee, A. J., DeBruine, L. M., & Jones, B. C. (2018). Individual-specific mortality is associated with how individuals evaluate future discounting decisions. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 285(1880), 20180304. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2018.0304 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, J. K., Rouault, M., & Wyart, V. (2023). Adaptive tuning of human learning and choice variability to unexpected uncertainty. Science Advances, 9(13), 114. https://doi.org/10.1126/sciadv.add0501 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Li, Z., & Belsky, J. (2022). Indirect effects, via parental factors, of income harshness and unpredictability on kindergarteners’ socioemotional functioning. Development and Psychopathology, 34(2), 635646. https://doi.org/10.1017/S095457942100136X CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Li, Z., Liu, S., Hartman, S., & Belsky, J. (2018). Interactive effects of early-life income harshness and unpredictability on children’s socioemotional and academic functioning in kindergarten and adolescence. Developmental Psychology, 54(11), 21012112. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000601 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Li, Z., Sturge-Apple, M. L., Platts, C. R., & Davies, P. T. (2023). Testing different sources of environmental unpredictability on adolescent functioning: Ancestral cue versus statistical learning and the role of temperament. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 64(3), 437448. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.13714 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Little, R. J. A. (1988). A test of missing completely at random for multivariate data with missing values. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 83(404), 11981202. https://doi.org/10.1080/01621459.1988.10478722 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liu, Y., Hatch, S. L., Patalay, P., Schott, J. M., & Richards, M. (2023). A lifecourse approach in examining the association between accumulation of adversity and mental health in older adulthood. Journal of Affective Disorders, 339, 211218. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2023.07.001 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lussier, A. A., Zhu, Y., Smith, B. J., Cerutti, J., Fisher, J., Melton, P. E., Wood, N. M., Cohen-Woods, S., Huang, R.-C., Mitchell, C., Schneper, L., Notterman, D. A., Simpkin, A. J., Smith, A. D. A. C., Suderman, M. J., Walton, E., Relton, C. L., Ressler, K. J., & Dunn, E. C. (2023). Association between the timing of childhood adversity and epigenetic patterns across childhood and adolescence: Findings from the avon longitudinal study of parents and children (ALSPAC) prospective cohort. The Lancet Child & Adolescent Health, 7(8), 532543. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2352-4642(23)00127-X CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martinez, J. L., Hasty, C., Morabito, D., Maranges, H. M., Schmidt, N. B., & Maner, J. K. (2022). Perceptions of childhood unpredictability, delay discounting, risk-taking, and adult externalizing behaviors: A life-history approach. Development and Psychopathology, 34(2), 113. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579421001607 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McLaughlin, K. A., Sheridan, M. A., Humphreys, K. L., Belsky, J., & Ellis, B. J. (2021). The value of dimensional models of early experience: Thinking clearly about concepts and categories. Perspectives On Psychological Science, 16(6), 14631472. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691621992346 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McLaughlin, K. A., Weissman, D., & Bitrán, D. (2019). Childhood adversity and neural development: A systematic review. Annual Review of Developmental Psychology, 1(1), 277312. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-devpsych-121318-084950 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mell, H., Baumard, N., & André, J.-B. (2021). Time is money. Waiting costs explain why selection favors steeper time discounting in deprived environments. Evolution and Human Behavior, S109051382(4), 1000143–387. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2021.02.003 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mell, H., Safra, L., Algan, Y., Baumard, N., & Chevallier, C. (2018). Childhood environmental harshness predicts coordinated health and reproductive strategies: A cross-sectional study of a nationally representative sample from France. Evolution and Human Behavior, 39(1), 18. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2017.08.006 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, A. B., Machlin, L., McLaughlin, K. A., & Sheridan, M. A. (2021). Deprivation and psychopathology in the fragile families study: A 15-year longitudinal investigation. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 62(4), 382391. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.13260 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, A. B., Sheridan, M. A., Hanson, J. L., McLaughlin, K. A., Bates, J. E., Lansford, J. E., Pettit, G. S., & Dodge, K. A. (2018). Dimensions of deprivation and threat, psychopathology, and potential mediators: A multi-year longitudinal analysis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 127(2), 160170. https://doi.org/10.1037/abn0000331 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morey, L. C. (1991). Personality assessment inventory, professional manual. Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc.Google Scholar
Munakata, Y., Placido, D., & Zhuang, W. (2023). What’s next? Advances and challenges in understanding how environmental predictability shapes the development of cognitive control. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 32(6), 431438. https://doi.org/10.1177/09637214231199102 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Myers, R. (1990). Classical and modern regression with applications (2nd ed.). Duxbury.Google Scholar
Nagin, D. S., & Paternoster, R. (1994). Personal capital and social control: The deterrence implications of a theory of individual differences in criminal offending. Criminology, 32(4), 581606. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-9125.1994.tb01166.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nettle, D. (2010). Dying young and living fast: Variation in life history across English neighborhoods. Behavioral Ecology, 21(2), 387395. https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arp202 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nickerson, A., Steenkamp, M., Aerka, I. M., Salters-Pedneault, K., Carper, T. L., Barnes, J. B., & Litz, B. T. (2013). Prospective investigation of mental health following sexual assault. Depression and Anxiety, 30(5), 444450. https://doi.org/10.1002/da.22023 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Niv, S., Tuvblad, C., Raine, A., Wang, P., & Baker, L. A. (2012). Heritability and longitudinal stability of impulsivity in adolescence. Behavior Genetics, 42(3), 378392. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10519-011-9518-6 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Osgood, D. W., Wilson, J. K., O’Malley, P. M., Bachman, J. G., & Johnston, L. D. (1996). Routine activities and individual deviant behavior. American Sociological Review, 61(4), 635655. https://doi.org/10.2307/2096397 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pascoe, E. A., & Smart Richman, L. (2009). Perceived discrimination and health: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 135(4), 531554. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0016059 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pepper, G. V., & Nettle, D. (2017). The behavioural constellation of deprivation: Causes and consequences. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 40, e314. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X1600234X CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Piray, P., & Daw, N. D. (2021). A model for learning based on the joint estimation of stochasticity and volatility. Nature Communications, 12(1), Article–1. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-021-26731-9 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Promislow, D. E. L. (1991). Senescence in natural populations of mammals: A comparative study. Evolution, 45(8), 18691887. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1558-5646.1991.tb02693.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Promislow, D. E. L., & Harvey, P. H. (1990). Living fast and dying young: A comparative analysis of life-history variation among mammals. Journal of Zoology, 220(3), 417437. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7998.1990.tb04316.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
R Core Team (2021). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing. https://www.R-project.org/ Google Scholar
Reichborn-Kjennerud, T., Czajkowski, N., Ystrøm, E., Ørstavik, R., Aggen, S. H., Tambs, K., Torgersen, S., Neale, M. C., Røysamb, E., Krueger, R. F., Knudsen, G. P., & Kendler, K. S. (2015). A longitudinal twin study of borderline and antisocial personality disorder traits in early to middle adulthood. Psychological Medicine, 45(14), 31213131. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291715001117 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Revelle, W. (2022). psych: Procedures for psychological, psychometric, and personality research. Northwestern University. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=psych Google Scholar
Richardson, G. B., Barbaro, N., Nedelec, J. L., & Liu, H. (2023). Testing environmental effects on age at menarche and sexual debut within a genetically informative twin design. Human Nature, 34(2), 324356. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-023-09451-5 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosseel, Y. (2012). Lavaan: An R package for structural equation modeling. Journal of Statistical Software, 48(2), 136. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v048.i02 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Šaffa, G., Kubicka, A. M., Hromada, M., & Kramer, K. L. (2019). Is the timing of menarche correlated with mortality and fertility rates? PLOS ONE, 14(4), e0215462. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0215462 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sampson, R. J., & Raudenbush, S. W. (1999). Systematic social observation of public spaces: A new look at disorder in urban neighborhoods. American Journal of Sociology, 105(3), 603651. https://doi.org/10.1086/210356 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scheier, M. F., & Carver, C. S. (1985). Optimism, coping, and health: Assessment and implications of generalized outcome expectancies. Health Psychology, 4(3), 219247. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-6133.4.3.219 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schubert, C. A., Mulvey, E. P., Steinberg, L., Cauffman, E., Losoya, S. H., Hecker, T., Chassin, L., & Knight, G. P. (2004). Operational lessons from the pathways to desistance project. Youth Violence and Juvenile Justice, 2(3), 237255. https://doi.org/10.1177/1541204004265875 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sear, R. (2020). Do human life history strategies exist? Evolution and Human Behavior, 41(6), 513526. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2020.09.004 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sear, R., Sheppard, P., & Coall, D. A. (2019). Cross-cultural evidence does not support universal acceleration of puberty in father-absent households. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 374(1770), 20180124. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2018.0124 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Selner-O’Hagan, M. B., Kindlon, D. J., Buka, S. L., Raudenbush, S. W., & Earls, F. J. (1998). Assessing exposure to violence in urban youth. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39(2), 215224. https://doi.org/10.1017/S002196309700187X CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sheehy-Skeffington, J. (2020). The effects of low socioeconomic status on decision-making processes. Current Opinion in Psychology, 33, 183188. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2019.07.043 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sheridan, M. A., & McLaughlin, K. A. (2014). Dimensions of early experience and neural development: Deprivation and threat. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(11), 580585. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2014.09.001 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Siegel, J. M., Golding, J. M., Stein, J. A., Burnam, M. A., & Sorenson, S. B. (1990). Reactions to sexual assault: A community study. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 5(2), 229246. https://doi.org/10.1177/088626090005002007 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simpson, J. A., Griskevicius, V., Kuo, S. I.-C., Sung, S., & Collins, W. A. (2012). Evolution, stress, and sensitive periods: The influence of unpredictability in early versus late childhood on sex and risky behavior. Developmental Psychology, 48(3), 674686. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027293 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Singmann, H., Bolker, B., Westfall, J., Aust, F., & Ben-Shachar, M. S. (2023). Afex: Analysis of factorial experiments. Computer Software. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=afex Google Scholar
Stamps, J. A., & Frankenhuis, W. E. (2016). Bayesian models of development. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 31(4), 260268. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tree.2016.01.012 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stearns, S. C. (1992). The evolution of life histories. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Story, G. W., Kurth-Nelson, Z., Moutoussis, M., Iigaya, K., Will, G.-J., Hauser, T. U., Blain, B., Vlaev, I., & Dolan, R. J. (2023). Discounting future reward in an uncertain world. Decision, 11(2), 255282. https://doi.org/10.1037/dec0000219 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Strathman, A., Gleicher, F., Boninger, D. S., & Edwards, C. S. (1994). The consideration of future consequences: Weighing immediate and distant outcomes of behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66(4), 742752. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.66.4.742 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Szepsenwol, O., Griskevicius, V., Simpson, J. A., Young, E. S., Fleck, C., & Jones, R. E. (2017). The effect of predictable early childhood environments on sociosexuality in early adulthood. Evolutionary Behavioral Sciences, 11(2), 131145. https://doi.org/10.1037/ebs0000082 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thornberry, T. P., Lizotte, A. J., Krohn, M. D., Farnworth, M., & Jang, S. J. (1994). Delinquent peers, beliefs, and delinquent behavior: A longitudinal test of interactional theory. Criminology, 32(1), 4783. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-9125.1994.tb01146.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ugarte, E., & Hastings, P. D. (2023). Assessing unpredictability in caregiver-child relationships: Insights from theoretical and empirical perspectives. Development and Psychopathology, 120. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579423000305 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Usacheva, M., Choe, D., Liu, S., Timmer, S., & Belsky, J. (2022). Testing the empirical integration of threat-deprivation and harshness-unpredictability dimensional models of adversity. Development and Psychopathology, 34(2), 114. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579422000013 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Verona, E., Patrick, C. J., & Joiner, T. E. (2001). Psychopathy, antisocial personality, and suicide risk. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110(3), 462470. https://doi.org/10.1037//0021-843x.110.3.462 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Walasek, N., Young, E. S., & Frankenhuis, W. E. (2024). A framework for studying environmental statistics in developmental science. Psychological Methods, 114. https://doi.org/10.1037/met0000651 Google ScholarPubMed
Wickham, H. (2016). ggplot2: Elegant graphics for data analysis. Springer-Verlag. https://ggplot2.tidyverse.org CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wickham, H., Averick, M., Bryan, J., Chang, W., McGowan, L., François, R., Grolemund, G., Hayes, A., Henry, L., Hester, J., Kuhn, M., Pedersen, T., Miller, E., Bache, S., Müller, K., Ooms, J., Robinson, D., Seidel, D., Spinu, V., Takahashi, K., Vaughan, D., Wilke, C., Woo, K., & Yutani, H. (2019). Welcome to the tidyverse. Journal of Open Source Software, 4(43), 1686. https://doi.org/10.21105/joss.01686 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Williams, D. R., Lawrence, J. A., & Davis, B. A. (2019). Racism and health: Evidence and needed research. Annual Review of Public Health, 40(1), 105125. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-publhealth-040218-043750 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wu, J., Guo, Z., Gao, X., & Kou, Y. (2020). The relations between early-life stress and risk, time, and prosocial preferences in adulthood: A meta-analytic review. Evolution and Human Behavior, 41(6), 557572. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2020.09.001 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wu, Q. (2024). The degree of fluctuations in maternal depressive symptoms in early childhood is associated with children’s depression risk: Initial evidence and replication between two independent samples. Research On Child and Adolescent Psychopathology, 52(5), 727741. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-023-01159-5 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Young, E. S., Frankenhuis, W. E., & Ellis, B. J. (2020). Theory and measurement of environmental unpredictability. Evolution and Human Behavior, 41(6), Article–6. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2020.08.006 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zachrisson, H. D., & Dearing, E. (2015). Family income dynamics, early childhood education and care, and early child behavior problems in Norway. Child Development, 86(2), 425440. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12306 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zheng, L., Chen, J., Li, X., & Gan, Y. (2022). Inherited dreams: A twin study of future orientation and heritability among chinese adolescents. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 19(2), 213233. https://doi.org/10.1080/17405629.2021.1889504 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zimbardo, P. G., & Boyd, J. N. (1999). Putting time in perspective: A valid, reliable individual-differences metric. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77(6), 12711288. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.77.6.1271 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Sample descriptive statistics

Figure 1

Figure 1. Bivariate Spearman’s correlations of unpredictability metrics. (a) Correlations between multiple unpredictability metrics of the threat dimension. (b) Correlations between multiple unpredictability metrics of the deprivation dimension. (c) Correlations of multiple unpredictability metrics of both dimensions and outcomes. In all figures, p values are uncorrected for multiple comparisons. Our candidate linear mixed model residual based metric, that is used in the path analysis are highlighted in red.

Figure 2

Figure 2. Simplified representation of the path analytic mediation model. Indicators and standardized parameter estimates. Statistically significant regression paths and covariances are represented by single and double headed arrows, respectively.

Figure 3

Table 2. Bivariate Spearman’s correlations between primary variables

Figure 4

Table 3. Parameter estimates of the path analytic mediation model

Supplementary material: File

Farkas and Jacquet supplementary material

Farkas and Jacquet supplementary material
Download Farkas and Jacquet supplementary material(File)
File 2.2 MB