Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-lnqnp Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T08:45:56.187Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Emotionality, emotion regulation, and preschoolers' social adaptation

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 March 2009

Kenneth H. Rubin*
Affiliation:
University of Waterloo
Robert J. Coplan
Affiliation:
University of Waterloo
Nathan A. Fox
Affiliation:
University of Maryland
Susan D. Calkins
Affiliation:
University of Maryland
*
Kenneth H. Rubin, Department of Psychology, University of Waterloo, Waterloo, Ontario, CanadaN2L 3G1.

Abstract

It was proposed that the interaction between the constructs of emotion regulation and social interaction would predict social adaptation in preschoolers. Ninety-six 4-year-olds were observed in quartets of unfamiliar same-sex peers. Based on parent temperament ratings and observed free play behaviors, 68 children from the original sample were classified as: low social interaction, good emotion regulators; low social interaction, poor emotion regulators; high social interaction, good emotion regulators; high social interaction, poor emotion regulators; or average. The results indicated that the low social interaction children who were poor regulators displayed more wary and anxious behaviors during free play and other episodes, and were rated as having more internalizing problems than both the low social interaction children who were good regulators and the average group. The high social interaction children who were poor regulators were rated as having more externalizing problems than either the high social interaction children who were good regulators or the average group. Thus, it seems as if emotion dysregulation is associated with psychological maladaptation, but that this association is tempered by the degree to which children engage in social interaction.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1995

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Achenbach, T. M., & Edelbrock, C. S. (1981). Behavioral problems and competencies reported by parents of normal and disturbed children aged four through sixteen. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 46, (1, Serial No. 188).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Asendorpf, J. B. (1993). Beyond temperament: A two factorial coping model of the development of inhibition during childhood. In Rubin, K. H. & Asendorpf, J. (Eds.), Social withdrawal, inhibition, and shyness in childhood (pp. 265289). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Asendorpf, J. B. (in press). Social and personality development. In Weinert, F. (Ed.), The Munich Longitudinal Study on the Genesis of Individual Competencies (LOGIC). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Belsky, J., & Rovine, M. (1987). Temperament and attachment security in the Strange Situation: An empirical rapprochement. Child Development, 58, 787795.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Broberg, A. G. (1993). Inhibitions and children's experiences of out-of-home care. In Rubin, K. H. & Asendorpf, J. (Eds.), Social withdrawal, inhibition, and shyness in childhood (pp. 151176). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Buss, A. H., & Plomin, R. (1984). Temperament: Early developing personality traits. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Calkins, S., & Fox, N. (1992). The relations between infant temperament, security of attachment and behavioral inhibition at 24 months. Child Development, 63, 14561472.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Calkins, S. D., Fox, N. A., & Marshall, T. R. (in press). Behavioral and physiological correlates of inhibition in infancy. Child Development.Google Scholar
Calkins, S. D., Fox, N. A., Rubin, K. H., & Coplan, R. J. (in preparation). Longitudinal outcomes behavioral inhibition: Implications for behavior in a peer setting.Google Scholar
Campos, J. J., Campos, R. G., & Barrett, K. C. (1989). Emergent themes in the study of emotional development and emotion regulation. Developmental Psychology, 25, 394402.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cole, P., Michel, M. K., & O'Donnell, L. (1994). The development of emotional regulation and dysregulation. In Fox, N. A. (Ed.), The development of emotion regulation: Biological and behavioral considerations. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development (pp. 73100). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Coplan, R. J., & Rubin, K. H. (1993). Multiple forms of social withdrawal in young children: Reticence and solitary-passive behaviors. Paper presented at the Biennial Meetings of the International Society for the Study of Behavioral Development. Recife, Brazil.Google Scholar
Coplan, R. J., Rubin, K. H., Fox, N. A., Calkins, S. D., & Stewart, S. L. (1994). Being alone, playing alone, and acting alone: Distinguishing among reticence, and passive- and active-solitude in young children. Child Development, 65, 129138.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davidson, R. J. (1993). Childhood temperament and cerebral asymmetry: A neurobiological substrate of behavioral inhibition. In Rubin, K. H. & Asendorpf, J. (Eds.), Social withdrawal, inhibition, and shyness in childhood (pp. 3148). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Derryberry, D., & Rothbart, M. K. (1984). Emotion, attention, and temperament. In Izard, C. E., Kagan, J., & Zajonc, R. B. (Eds.), Emotions, cognitions, & behavior. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Dishion, T. J., Patterson, G. R., Stoolmiller, M., & Skinner, M. L. (1991). Family, school, and behavioral antecedents to early adolescent involvement with antisocial peers. Developmental Psychology, 27, 172180.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dodge, K. A. (1989). Coordinating responses to aversive stimuli. Developmental Psychology, 25, 339342.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dodge, K. A. (1991). Emotion and social information processing. In Garber, J. & Dodge, K. A. (Eds.), The development of emotion regulation and dysregulation (pp. 159182). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fox, N. A., & Calkins, S. D. (1993). Pathways to aggression and social withdrawal: Interactions among temperament, attachment, and regulation. In Rubin, K. & Asendorpf, J. (Eds.), Social withdrawal, inhibition, and shyness in childhood. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Garber, J., Braafladt, N., & Zeman, J. (1991). The regulation of sad affect: An information processing perspective. In Garber, J. & Dodge, K. A. (Eds.), The development of emotion regulation and dysregulation (pp. 208242). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garber, J., & Dodge, K. A. (Eds.). (1991). The development of emotion regulation and dysregulation. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hartup, W. W. (1992). Peer relations in early and middle childhood. In Van Hasset, V. B. & Hersen, M. (Eds.), Handbook of social development (pp. 257281). New York: Plenum Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kagan, J. (1989). Temperamental contributions to social behavior. American Psychologist, Vol. 44 (4), 668674.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kagan, J., Gibbons, J. L., Johnson, M. O., Reznick, J. S., & Snidman, N. (1990). A temperamental disposition to the state of uncertainty. In Rolf, J., Masten, A. S., Cicchetti, D., Nuechterlein, K. H., & Weintraub, S. (Eds.), Risk and protective factors in the development of psychopathology (pp. 164178). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kagan, J., & Snidman, N. (1991). Infant predictors of inhibited and uninhibited profiles. Psychological Science, 2, 4044.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kagan, J., Reznick, J. S., Clarke, C., Snidman, N., & Garcia-Coll, C. (1984). Behavioral inhibition to the unfamiliar. Child Development, 55, 22122225.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kagan, J., Reznick, J. S., & Gibbons, J. (1989). Inhibited and uninhibited types of children. Child Development, 60, 838845.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kerr, M., Lambert, W. W., Stattin, H., & Klackenberg-Larsson, I. (1994). Stability of inhibition in a Swedish longitudinal sample. Child Development, 65, 138146.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kopp, C. B. (1989). Regulation and distress of negative emotions: A developmental view. Developmental Psychology, 25, 343354.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pepler, D. J., & Rubin, K. H. (Eds.) (1991). The development and treatment of childhood aggression. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Porges, S. W. (1991). Autonomic regulation and attention. In Campbell, B. A., Hayne, H., & Richardson, R. (Eds.), Attention and information processing in infants and adults. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Rothbart, M. K. (1988). Attention and emotion in the development of temperament. In Posner, M. I., symposium organizer, The role of attention in normal development and psychopathology. University of Oregon, Center for the Study of Emotion (Technical Report No. 88–3, Eugene, OR).Google Scholar
Rothbart, M. K., & Derryberry, D. (1981). Development of individual differences in temperament. In Lamb, M. E. & Brown, A. N. (Eds.), Advances in developmental psychology (pp. 3885). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Rowe, D. C., & Plomin, R. (1977). Temperament in early childhood. Journal of Personality Assessment, 41, 150156.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rubin, K. H. (1982). Non-social play in preschoolers: Necessary evil? Child Development, 53, 651657.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rubin, K. H. (1989). The play observation scale (POS). University of Waterloo.Google Scholar
Rubin, K. H. (1993). The Waterloo Longitudinal Project: Correlates and consequences of social withdrawal from childhood to adolescence. In Rubin, K. H. & Asendorpf, J. (Eds.), Social withdrawal, inhibition, and shyness in childhood (pp. 291314). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Rubin, K. H., Chen, X., & Hymel, S. (1993). Socioemotional characteristics of aggressive and withdrawn children. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 49, 518534.Google Scholar
Rubin, K. H., & Coplan, R. J. (1992). Peer relationships in childhood. In Bornstein, M. & Lamb, M. (Eds.), Developmental psychology: An advanced textbook (pp. 519578). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Rubin, K. H., Hymel, S., Mills, R. S. L., & Rose-Krasnor, L. (1991). Conceptualizing different pathways to and from social isolation in childhood. In Cicchetti, D. & Toth, S. (Eds.), The Rochester symposium on developmental psychopathology, Vol. 2, Internalizing and externalizing expressions of dysfunction (pp. 91122). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Rubin, K. H., & Krasnor, L. R. (1986). Socialcognitive and social behavioral perspectives on problem solving. In Perlmutter, M. (Ed.), Cognitive perspectives on children's social and behavioral development (Vol. 18, pp. 168). The Minnesota Symposia on Child Psychology. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Rubin, K. H., & Mills, R. S. L. (1991). Conceptualizing developmental pathways to internalizing disorders in childhood. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science, 23, 300317.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rubin, K. H., Stewart, S. L., & Coplan, R. J. (in press). Social withdrawal in childhood: Conceptual and empirical perspectives. In Ollendick, T. & Prinz, R. (Eds.) Advances in clinical child psychology, Vol. 17. New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
Stifter, C. A., & Fox, N. A. (1990). Infant reactivity and regulation: Physiological correlates of newborn and five month temperament. Developmental Psychology, 26, 582588.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thompson, R. A. (1990). Emotion and self-regulation. In Thompson, R. A. (Ed.), Socioemotional development: Nebraska symposium on motivation (Vol. 36, pp. 367467). Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Thompson, R. A. (1994). Emotion regulation: A theme in search of a definition. In Fox, N. A. (Ed.), The development of emotion regulation: Biological and behavioral considerations. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development (pp. 2552). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar