Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-mkpzs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T06:35:22.037Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Parental mentalization and children’s externalizing problems: A systematic review and meta-analysis

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 October 2024

Maitane Nieto-Retuerto*
Affiliation:
Department of Clinical and Health Psychology & Research Methods, University of the Basque Country UPV/EHU, Donostia-San Sebastian, Spain
Barbara Torres-Gomez*
Affiliation:
Department of Clinical and Health Psychology & Research Methods, University of the Basque Country UPV/EHU, Donostia-San Sebastian, Spain
Itziar Alonso-Arbiol
Affiliation:
Department of Clinical and Health Psychology & Research Methods, University of the Basque Country UPV/EHU, Donostia-San Sebastian, Spain
*
Corresponding authors: Maitane Nieto-Retuerto; Email: maitane.nieto@ehu.eus, Barbara Torres-Gomez; Email: barbara.torres@ehu.eus
Corresponding authors: Maitane Nieto-Retuerto; Email: maitane.nieto@ehu.eus, Barbara Torres-Gomez; Email: barbara.torres@ehu.eus
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Parental mentalization, as the ability to understand mental states (e.g., desires) behind their children’s actions, may play a relevant role in the prevention of future externalizing problems. We conducted a meta-analysis to examine the relationship between parental mentalization and children’s externalizing problems. Six electronic databases were searched for studies, published in English or Spanish, linking empirically those two variables. Participants included caregivers and children between 0 and 18 years. The filtering process yielded 42 studies with 52 effect sizes. Random-effect analysis revealed higher parental mentalization associated with fewer externalizing problems, with an effect size of r = −.19 (95% CI [−.25, −.13]). Due to high heterogeneity (I2 = 83.750), further analyses were conducted to explore factors affecting such association. Parenting experience and children’s developmental stage moderated the relationship, but approaches to operationalize mentalization (MM or PRF), sample type (clinical/at-risk vs. community), and reporting figure (primary caregiver vs. other informants) did not. The study highlights the significance of parental mentalization as a potential contributor to the prevention of externalizing behaviors among infants, children, and adolescents. Our findings may underscore practical implications for equipping caregivers with mentalization skills, helping them to answer appropriately to their children needs.

Type
Regular Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

Early manifestations of externalizing behaviors, such as tantrums, disobedience, and aggression (Peterson et al., Reference Peterson, Dando, D’Souza, Waldie, Carr, Mohal and Morton2018), are observed as early as infancy (Perra et al., Reference Perra, Paine and Hay2021). Recent meta-analysis results in children aged 1–7 years (Vasileva et al., Reference Vasileva, Graf, Reinelt, Petermann and Petermann2021) reveal a 10% prevalence of externalizing psychopathology, including attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder, conduct disorder, and oppositional defiant disorder. Failure to address these maladaptive behaviors in their early stages increases the risk of future issues, including substance abuse and violence (Moffit, Reference Moffit2017), antisocial personality disorder (Diamantopoulou et al., Reference Diamantopoulou, Verhulst and van der Ende2010), and criminal records (Bevilacqua et al., Reference Bevilacqua, Hale, Barker and Viner2018). In fact, early externalizing problems have shown to predict criminal convictions two decades later (Kassing et al., Reference Kassing, Godwin, Lochman and Coie2019). Hence, it is essential to prevent the emergence of externalizing behaviors and/or to avoid their persistence at early ages.

Externalizing behaviors result from a complex interplay of genetics, neurological changes, and environmental factors (Rovira et al., Reference Rovira, Demontis, Sánchez-Mora, Zayats, Klein, Mota, Weber, Garcia-Martínez, Pagerols, Vilar-Ribó, Arribas, Richarte, Corrales, Fadeuilhe, Bosch, Martin, Almos, Doyle, Grevet and Ribasés2020), including biological disposition (Dodge & Pettit, Reference Dodge and Pettit2003), parent–child relationship characteristics (Hewitt-Ramírez & Moreno-Méndez, Reference Hewitt-Ramírez and Moreno-Méndez2018), and the interaction between genetics (e.g., temperament) and environmental (e.g., family) elements (Goodnight et al., Reference Goodnight, Donahue, Waldman, Van Hulle, Rathouz, Lahey and D’Onofrio2016). Since interventions and preventive measures are not applicable to genetic factors, a relevant area of attention and intervention is the realm of the parent–child relationship. Parenting is considered a significant contributing factor of the children’s externalizing behavior (Rothbaum & Weisz, Reference Rothbaum and Weisz1994). Thus, parental abilities become a primary target for interventions. Encouraging the development of specific parenting skills can enhance the bond between parents and children and reduce various adverse effects, particularly externalizing problems (e.g., see Cooke et al.’s Reference Cooke, Deneault, Devereux, Eirich, Fearon and Madigan2022, meta-analysis on parental sensitivity and child behavioral problems). Therefore, the parent–child relationship emerges as an explicative factor worth studying in preventing juvenile crime (Webster-Stratton & Reid, Reference Webster-Stratton, Reid, Weisz and Kazdin2018). While the influence of parenting is well established in the case of both externalizing (Pinquart, Reference Pinquart2017a) and internalizing problems (Lin et al., Reference Lin, Kehoe, Pozzi, Liontos and Whittle2024; Pinquart, Reference Pinquart2017b), the present review focuses on children’s and adolescents’ externalizing behaviors since including both internalizing and externalizing problems is beyond the scope of the present review.

From a developmental perspective, infants’ survival hinges on their caregivers’ capacity to interpret their thoughts, feelings, and behaviors to meet their needs (Lavender et al., Reference Lavender, Waters and Hobson2023). Parental mentalization is closely linked to children’s inner growth, assisting them in managing emotional conflicts and promoting positive psychological development (Caldarera et al., Reference Caldarera, Vitiello, Turcich, Bechis and Baietto2022). In fact, there is evidence showing the links between parental mentalization and positive parenting behaviors (for a systematic review, see Stuhrmann et al., Reference Byrne, Murphy and Connon2022). Specifically, parental mentalization has been robustly related to secure child–parent attachment (Zeegers et al., Reference Zeegers, Colonnesi, Stams and Meins2017), which, in turn, has been strongly associated with a lower incidence of externalizing problems (based on meta-analyses for results with mothers by Fearon et al., Reference Fearon, Bakermans-Kranenburg, Van IJzendoorn, Lapsley and Roisman2010, and with fathers by Deneault et al., Reference Deneault, Bakermans-Kranenburg, Groh, Fearon and Madigan2021).

Despite the existent growing number of studies in the field, there are discrepancies regarding the consistency of the relationship between parental mentalization and children’s externalizing behaviors. Specifically, research findings show varying degrees of such association. For example, research shows that parental mentalization and externalizing problems are positively associated (Meins et al., Reference Meins, Centifanti, Fernyhough and Fishburn2013, Wade et al., Reference Wade, Plamondon and Jenkins2021), weakly negatively related (e.g., Dollberg et al., Reference Dollberg, Hanetz-Gamliel and Levy2023), or strongly inversely correlated (e.g., Khoshroo & Seyed Mousavi, Reference Khoshroo and Seyed Mousavi2022). Such disparities across studies reveal the need of exploring potential reasons behind them. Therefore, the meta-analytic technique, including the use of meta-regression, is instrumental in overcoming inconsistencies in results. The current study represents the first meta-analysis that synthesizes research examining the issue.

Parental mentalization linked to children’s externalizing problems

Overall, mentalization is a dynamic and developmental capacity mainly context- and relationship-specific. Parental mentalization primarily emerges within the context of secure attachment relationships, serving as a key construct in children’s development and beneficial parent–child relationships (Sharp & Fonagy, Reference Sharp and Fonagy2008). Within that framework, parental mentalization is a psychological mechanism that facilitates parental self-regulation and the co-regulation of the child’s emotional states (Stuhrmann et al., Reference Stuhrmann, Göbel, Bindt and Mudra2022). It encompasses parents’ capacity to perceive their child as a psychological entity, differentiating between their children’s mental experiences and their own. Parental ability to comprehend not only their child’s external behaviors but also the internal mental activity (e.g., motives and emotions) that drive those behaviors (Medrea & Benga, Reference Medrea and Benga2021), is closely associated to responding to the child appropriately through affectively attuned parenting behavior (Kelly et al., Reference Kelly, Slade and Grienenberger2005).

In contrast, parents with lower mentalizing capacities may struggle to perceive their children’s mental states, failing to comprehend the motivations behind their children’s actions and to meet their needs, especially in challenging situations. Consequently, this lower mentalizing capacity may result in punitive responses exacerbating the child’s distress and dysregulation (Dollberg et al., Reference Dollberg, Hanetz-Gamliel and Levy2021; Ensink, Bégin et al., Reference Ensink, Bégin, Normandin, Godbout and Fonagy2017). Evidence suggests that poor parental mentalization is correlated with heightened negative emotionality (Smaling et al., Reference Smaling, Huijbregts, van der Heijden, van Goozen and Swaab2016), increased tendencies towards controlling behaviors (Suardi et al., Reference Suardi, Moser, Sancho Rossignol, Manini, Vital, Merminod, Kreis, Ansermet, Serpa and Schechter2020), and with issues in socioemotional development (Nijssens et al., Reference Nijssens, Vliegen and Luyten2020).

In fact, when it comes to the relationship with children’s externalizing behaviors, it has been found that high parental mentalization can contribute to the prevention of the onset of these problems (Suardi et al., Reference Suardi, Moser, Sancho Rossignol, Manini, Vital, Merminod, Kreis, Ansermet, Serpa and Schechter2020), and poor parental mentalization is associated with higher externalizing behaviors (Khoshroo & Seyed Mousavi, Reference Khoshroo and Seyed Mousavi2022). Nevertheless, there are some studies that have reported a null (Zeegers et al., Reference Zeegers, Colonnesi, Noom, Polderman and Stams2020) or even a positive relationship (Kochanska & An, Reference Kochanska and An2023; Wade et al., Reference Wade, Plamondon and Jenkins2021) between parental mentalization and externalizing problems. Therefore, it is evident the need to clarify the association between these variables. In view of the above, we expect a negative relationship. Yet, several circumstances need to be considered in the analysis. A brief account of possible moderators follows.

Parental mentalization: operationalization approaches

Mentalization consists of four primary dimensions: Aspect (automatic vs. controlled), Focus (internal vs. external), Person (self vs. other), and Mode (cognitive vs. affective), being essential that these dimensions maintain a balanced interaction for effective mentalization (Bateman & Fonagy, Reference Bateman, Fonagy, Kealy and Ogrodniczuk2019). Parental mentalization is investigated through three approaches – mind-mindedness (MM; Meins, Reference Meins2013), parental reflective functioning (PRF; Slade et al., Reference Slade, Grienenberger, Bernbach, Levy and Locker2005), and insightfulness (Oppenheim & Koren-Karie, Reference Oppenheim and Koren-Karie2002) – all focusing on parents’ comprehension of their children’s internal states (Zeegers et al., Reference Zeegers, Colonnesi, Stams and Meins2017). Each one operationalizes caregivers’ capacities differently and offers a unique perspective on comprehending parent–child dialogue and interaction (Medrea & Benga, Reference Medrea and Benga2021). A simple way to understand how to operationalize parental mentalization is conceptualizing it, following the terminology of Meins and Fernyhough (Reference Meins and Fernyhough2015), as ‘offline’ (representational) and ‘online’ (interactional) mentalizing abilities.

MM is the only measure that can be evaluated from both online and offline perspectives, while PRF and insightfulness are operationalized in a representational way (offline). Appropriate MM can be measured in: (a) parent–child interactions, where parents provide accurate and spontaneous comments on the child’s mental state, matching the infant’s experience or behavior (online) and (b) via the caregiver’s description of the child in terms of mental attributes (offline; Dollberg, Reference Dollberg2022). PRF can be evaluated by coding an interview (e.g., the Parent Development Interview: PDI; Slade et al., Reference Slade, Grienenberger, Bernbach, Levy and Locker2005; Sleed et al., Reference Sleed, Slade and Fonagy2020), or via self-report using a questionnaire (e.g., The Parental Reflective Functioning Questionnaire: PRFQ; Carlone et al., Reference Carlone, Milan, Decoste, Borelli, McMahon and Suchman2023; Luyten et al., Reference Luyten, Mayes, Nijssens and Fonagy2017). Within the PRFQ, the dimensions include pre-mentalizing (PM), certainty about mental states (CMS), and interest and curiosity (IC), with adequate mentalization characterized by low PM and average CMS and IC scores. The PM subscale, which assesses distortions in mentalizing, reveals particularly detrimental effects of serious mentalization impairments, and is the most closely related dimension to emotional unavailability and insecure attachment (Luyten et al., Reference Luyten, Mayes, Nijssens and Fonagy2017). Insightfulness is assessed through caregivers’ reflections on their relationship with the child in response to questions about the child’s thoughts and feelings (Oppenheim & Koren-Karie, Reference Oppenheim and Koren-Karie2002).

Nevertheless, the strength and direction among these three approaches (MM, PRF, insightfulness) and children’s externalizing problems seems to vary across studies. For instance, the correlation between MM and externalizing problems has been reported as strong (Camisasca et al., Reference Camisasca, Miragoli, Ionio, Milani and Di Blasio2018), weak (Dollberg et al., Reference Dollberg, Hanetz-Gamliel and Levy2023), or nonexistent (Zeegers et al., Reference Zeegers, Colonnesi, Noom, Polderman and Stams2020). Similarly, PRF’s association with externalizing has been observed as strongly negative (Khoshroo & Seyed Mousavi, Reference Khoshroo and Seyed Mousavi2022) or positive (Borelli et al., Reference Borelli, Yates, Hecht, Cervantes, Russo, Arreola, Leal, Torres and Guerra2021). Lastly, analyses of the relationship between insightfulness and externalizing problems have showed positive (Gray et al., Reference Gray, Forbes, Briggs-Gowan and Carter2015) and negative associations (Feniger-Schaal & Koren-Karie, Reference Feniger-Schaal and Koren-Karie2021).

Therefore, in view of the aforementioned discrepancies between studies, further research is required. Thus, the present meta-analysis will explore whether MM, PRF or insightfulness has stronger power to predict the expected negative relationship between parental mentalization and children’s externalizing problems. We anticipate the relationship between parental mentalization and externalizing problems to vary depending on whether it is assessed representationally or through actual interactions. Some parents may possess adequate mentalization skills at the representational level (assessed via PRF), but struggle to demonstrate them in real-time interactions, when measured online through their verbalizations about the child’s mental states during direct play (Shai et al., Reference Shai, Dollberg and Szepsenwol2017). Hence, we expect that, when parental mentalization is analyzed in real-time (online) interactions, the relationship between mentalization and externalizing problems will be stronger than when the measure is a ‘representational’ approach (offline).

Children’s developmental stage

Following normative developmental trajectories, externalizing problems decrease from early childhood to preadolescence, increase during adolescence, and then decrease again from late adolescence into adulthood (Petersen et al., Reference Petersen, Bates, Dodge, Lansford and Pettit2015). Yet the impact of parental mentalization on reducing these behaviors may vary depending on the child’s developmental stage. For example, some studies have found that the relationship between parental mentalization and externalizing problems in adolescents is close to zero (e.g., Benbassat & Priel, Reference Benbassat and Priel2012; Borelli et al., Reference Borelli, Palmer, Vanwoerden and Sharp2019). However, it has been found to be negative during infancy (e.g., Salo et al., Reference Salo, Lipsanen, Sourander, Pajulo and Kalland2022; Smaling et al., Reference Smaling, Huijbregts, van der Heijden, van Goozen and Swaab2016), in young children (e.g., Colonnesi et al., Reference Colonnesi, Zeegers, Majdandžić, van Steensel and Bögels2019; Dollberg et al., Reference Dollberg, Hanetz-Gamliel and Levy2021) and during middle childhood (e.g., Condon et al., Reference Condon, Holland, Slade, Redeker, Mayes and Sadler2019; Ensink et al., Reference Ensink, Bégin, Normandin and Fonagy2016). Moreover, a study comparing both early and middle childhood found a stronger negative effect between parental mentalization and externalizing problems in the older group (Khoshroo & Seyed Mousavi, Reference Khoshroo and Seyed Mousavi2022). Thus, younger children may require higher parental mentalization skills to effectively regulate their emotions and behaviors. Considering the above findings, we may posit a stronger negative relationship between mentalization and externalizing problems during middle childhood and a smaller effect during early childhood and adolescence.

Parenting experience

Given the inconsistency of research evidence, we suggest that parenting experience may be another moderator. While certain studies involving first-time parents revealed a modest association (Salo et al., Reference Salo, Pajulo, Vinzce, Raittila, Sourander and Kalland2021), research involving experienced caregivers uncovered a higher correlation (Lunn et al., Reference Lunn, Lewis and Gannon2019). Hence, we hypothesize that the negative relationship between parental mentalization and children’s externalizing problems will be stronger in experienced parents as compared to first-time parents.

Population type

Given the processes underlying mentalization, psychopathology will invariably entail challenges related to mentalizing skills (Bateman, Reference Bateman2022). As a matter of fact, in a recent systematic review and meta-analysis (Georg et al., Reference Georg, Meyerhöfer, Taubner and Volkert2023), parental depression has been associated to lower parental mentalization. Yet, the relationship between parental mentalization and children’s externalizing problems might differ between clinical and nonclinical populations. In fact, there is research evidence showing a likely stronger effect of parental mentalization in clinical/at risk samples (e.g., Hughes et al., Reference Hughes, Aldercotte and Foley2017). Specifically, maternal mentalization has been found to have a stronger protective role in mitigating externalizing symptoms in challenging situations or with at-risk families, as opposed to general community samples (Meins et al., Reference Meins, Centifanti, Fernyhough and Fishburn2013). Therefore, we expect the strength of the negative association between parental mentalization and externalizing behaviors to be more pronounced in clinical or at-risk samples compared to studies involving the general community.

Parental education

Research exploring various education levels has shown differential associations of parental mentalization with child externalizing problems. MM was a predictor of child externalizing problems at childhood, but only with parents with no secondary education (Meins et al., Reference Meins, Centifanti, Fernyhough and Fishburn2013). However, some studies reported no association between parental mentalization and externalizing behavior when controlling for parental education (e.g., Colonnesi et al., Reference Colonnesi, Zeegers, Majdandžić, van Steensel and Bögels2019; Dollberg et al., Reference Dollberg, Hanetz-Gamliel and Levy2021). Given the mixed nature of the evidence, we refrain from proposing a specific hypothesis regarding this matter.

Reporting figure

From a methodological perspective, the reporting figure may also affect the relationship between parental mentalization and behavior. There may be differences between parents’ and teachers’ reports of children’s externalizing problems, as these informants rely on distinct experiences (Deneault et al., Reference Deneault, Hammond and Madigan2023). In fact, a meta-analysis reported that parents often perceive more externalizing behaviors than teachers (Carneiro et al., Reference Carneiro, Soares, Rescorla and Dias2021). In addition, when the same observer evaluates two different variables, the correlation between them may increase due to covariance bias (Hoyt, Reference Hoyt2000). Thus, it is reasonable to think that the relationship between parental mentalization and children’s externalizing problems would be stronger when the primary caregivers are the reporting figure of the latter variable (as opposed to teachers or other reporting figures).

Aims of the meta-analysis

In summary, the evidence that parental mentalization is associated with a decrease in children’s externalizing problems is inconclusive. Therefore, one of the objectives of this meta-analysis is to confirm the type of relationship between these variables. We also seek to determine the magnitude of this association, as well as to investigate factors that may moderate this relationship. The present meta-analysis aims at examining the relationship among infants, children, and adolescents. It will consider the following moderators: (a) different approaches for the operationalization of parental mentalization (MM, PRF or insightfulness), (b) children’s developmental stage (early childhood, middle childhood or adolescence), (c) parenting experience (first-time vs. experienced parenting), (d) sample type (clinical/at-risk vs. community), (e) parental education, and (f) reporting figure (primary caregiver vs. others).

The main hypothesis is that higher parental mentalization will be associated with fewer externalizing problems in children and adolescents. Furthermore, we expect a stronger negative association between higher levels of parental mentalization and externalizing problems in real time interactions (vs. representational approaches), in middle childhood (vs. younger children and adolescents), in experienced (vs. first-time) parenting family situations, in clinical/at-risk (vs. community), and in primary caregiver (vs. others’ reports). Lastly, based on conflicting evidence when examining the possible moderating effects of parental education, we will take an exploratory approach regarding this question.

Methods

Inclusion and exclusion criteria

This systematic review and meta-analysis followed the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) guidelines (Page et al., Reference Page, McKenzie, Bossuyt, Boutron, Hoffmann, Mulrow, Shamseer, Tetzlaff, Akl, Brennan, Chou, Glanville, Grimshaw, Hróbjartsson, Lalu, Li, Loder, Mayo-Wilson and Moher2021). For their inclusion, studies had to meet the following criteria: (a) written in English or Spanish; (b) empirical in nature; (c) reported in peer-reviewed journal articles; and (d) included children aged between 0 and 18 years in the sample. Studies were excluded if: (a) they provided insufficient data for effect size analysis, and additional data could not be obtained from the respective authors; (b) they did not evaluate parental mentalization;Footnote 1 (c) they did not assess children’s externalizing behavior in the age range under analysis (0-18 years); (d) they did not investigate the relationship between parental mentalization and child externalizing symptoms; or (e) were dissertations or book chapters.

Search strategy criteria and selection of studies

The initial search was performed in June 2021; it was updated in January 2024 to retrieve documents included in databases until December 2023. First, six electronic databases were searched to identify records related to parental mentalization and externalizing problems. These databases included APA PsycArticles, APA PsycInfo, PsycBooks, PSICODOC, Scopus, and Web of Science. The following combination of terms was used in the abstract, keywords, and subjects: Mentali* OR reflective function* OR mind-mind* OR mind-related OR insightful AND external* OR internal* OR behav* disorder* OR behav* difficult* OR behav* problem* OR conduct disorder* OR conduct* difficult* OR conduct problem* OR disruptive behav* OR child* problem OR antisocial behav* OR ADHD OR CBCL. Initially, we retrieved 5,442 records; eventually, we included 42 studies (N = 7,761 participants) with 52 effect sizes in the meta-analysis (see Figure 1).

Figure 1. Flow diagram.

To ensure consistence and replicability of the meta-analysis, a random sample of every fifth screened paper was independently coded by the second author; disagreements were resolved by having the first author and the second author review the article in question and jointly come to a consensus regarding the appropriate coding. Subsequently, the first and the second author independently reviewed all full-text level records. A high agreement was obtained (Kappa = 0.98). The third author resolved discrepancies.

The first and the third authors independently assessed the risk of bias of each of the included studies and discussed their assessments to achieve consensus. An adapted version of the Newcastle–Ottawa Scale for cross-sectional studies (NOS; Herzog et al., Reference Herzog, Álvarez-Pasquin, Díaz, Del Barrio, Estrada and Gil2013; Wells et al., Reference Wells, Shea, O’Connell, Peterson, Welch, Losos and Tugwell2000) was used to evaluate the quality of the methodology used in the included studies. The NOS is a checklist that assesses the appropriateness of research design, the recruitment strategy, the representativeness of sample, the objectivity/reliability of outcome determination, the power calculation provided, and appropriate statistical analyses. Score disagreements were resolved by consensus and a final agreed-upon rating was assigned to each study (see S1 of Supplementary Online Materials).

Finally, the data extraction process involved two stages. The first author retrieved the necessary data for analysis (see Table 1), and the third author independently checked the values. Any disagreements were resolved by all authors.

Table 1. Information of the studies included in the meta-analysis

Notes. BASC − 2 = Behavior Assessment Scale for Children−Second Edition; BITSEA = Brief Infant−Toddler Social and Emotional Assessment; CBCL = Child Behavior Checklist; ECBI = The Eyberg Child Behavior Inventory; FMSS = Five-Minute Speech Sample; ICQ = Infant’s Characteristics Questionnaire; ITSEA = Infant−Toddler Social and Emotional Assessment; nd = No data; Ohio = Ohio Youth Problem Functioning, and Satisfaction Scales; PDI = Parent Development Interview; SCBE − 30 = Social Competence and Behavior Evaluation − 30; SDQ = Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire; TRF = Teacher Report Form; YSR = Youth Self−Report.

Analysis plan

Our primary analysis included 52 effect sizes assessing parental mentalization and externalizing problems (see Table 1 for overall effect sizes and study characteristics). The following moderators were examined: approaches for the operationalization of parental mentalization (MM, PRF, or insightfulness), children’s developmental stage (early childhood, middle childhood, or adolescence), parenting experience (first-time vs. experienced), the sample type (clinical/at-risk vs. community), and reporting figure (primary caregiver vs. others). Only categorical moderator variables that had at least five contrasts in the categories were used (Bakermans-Kranenburg et al., Reference Bakermans-Kranenburg, Van Ijzendoorn and Juffer2003); thus, we were unable to analyze parental insightfulness (k = 3). The data from parental education was noncomparable among the studies to perform moderation analyses (i.e., studies reporting the percentages of participants’ educational attainment). When studies reported both maternal and paternal correlations, we chose the maternal correlation for consistency, as more studies focused on maternal mentalization. In longitudinal studies and interventions, we selected the effect size reported at baseline. In order to maintain a comparable number of studies, the operationalization of PRF was prioritized when both PRF and MM were reported, aiming to balance moderation analyses.

Taking into account the three dimensions of PRFQ, we have not considered the CMS or the IC subscales. The reason is that the optimal levels of both subscales are at the midpoint; consequently, a curvilinear analysis might be more appropriate, but it complicates the linear regression required for meta-analytic techniques. Therefore, we have chosen the PM correlation in order to perform the analyses. Accordingly, correlation scores were reverted because it assesses a non-mentalizing stance, indicating that higher scores correspond to lower mentalizing abilities. Moreover, to test whether the relationship between mentalization and externalizing problems differed across the various approaches for the operationalization of mentalization, we compared effect sizes between offline vs. online measures. To define age groups, we have categorized individuals into early childhood (0-5 years), middle childhood (6-12 years), and adolescence (13-18 years) (see S2 of Supplementary Online Materials).

The analysis was conducted using R v4.2.2 (R Core Team, 2022) and the metafor package v4.0.0. Random effects model was chosen due to its ability to account for the excess variance (Viechtbauer, Reference Viechtbauer2010). Effect sizes (Pearson’s r) were computed to have the same statistic depicting the relationship between parental mentalization (MM or PRF) and externalizing problems. Standard formulas allowed for the transformation of diverse effect sizes, such as group differences (Wilson, Reference Wilson2023) and regression coefficients (Peterson & Brown, Reference Peterson and Brown2005). In order to reduce variability, pooled effect sizes were computed with corresponding 95% confidence intervals (Borenstein & Hedges, Reference Borenstein, Hedges, Cooper and Hedges2019).

Results

Characteristics of the studies

The following lines provide a summary of some noteworthy aspects of the included research in the meta-analyses. A total of 42 studies with 52 effect sizes were incorporated in the meta-analysis. The investigations were conducted in various countries, including Australia, Belgium, Canada, Finland, Israel, Italy, the Netherlands, Switzerland, Türkiye, the UK, and the USA. The meta-analysis included 7,761 caregivers and their children. Most studies (k = 38) included birth parents, while a few involved other caregivers (k = 4), such as adoptive and foster parents. Twenty-six studies included participants from the community, nine studies used clinical or risk samples (i.e., children who experienced abuse or low socioeconomic families), and seven studies compared both sample types. Intervention programs were included in seven research papers. Studies analyzing parental mentalization as a mediator between parent/child mental health (i.e., parental depression or psychopathological impact of sexual abuse in children) on externalizing problems found higher levels of this ability related to fewer externalizing problems. This suggests both direct and indirect effects of parental mentalization on externalizing behaviors (see S3 of Supplementary Online Materials for details of variables analyzed in the studies).

Meta-analytic effects

Effect sizes (Pearson’s r) were computed to examine the relationship between parental mentalization and externalizing problems. The coefficient based on the random-effects model of r was −.19 (95% [CI: −.24, −.13]), ranging from −.56 to .23, with most estimates being negative (83%). As expected, there was a negative relationship between parental mentalization and externalizing problems. This indicates a small to moderate negative relationship between the variables, implying that higher parental mentalization is associated with fewer externalizing problems observed in children. Figure 2 displays a forest plot illustrating observed outcomes and estimates.

Figure 2. Forest plot.

The Q-test suggests heterogeneity among the true outcomes (Q(51) = 383.760, p = .001, τ2 = .029, I 2 = 83.750), denoting that a significant proportion of the variability in effect sizes is due to differences between studies rather than sampling error. The high I2 value suggests that approximately 84% of the total variation across studies is attributable to heterogeneity. Consequently, despite the average outcome being estimated as negative, some studies may indicate a positive true outcome (e.g., higher mentalization, elevated externalizing problems). Figure 3 presents a funnel plot of the estimates. Neither the rank correlation (p = .894) nor the regression (p = .849) test indicated any funnel plot asymmetry, suggesting no publication bias.

Figure 3. Funnel plot.

We conducted further exploration of the possible sources of heterogeneity through moderation analyses. The mixed-effects model indicated that the association between parental mentalization and externalizing behaviors varied significantly across different developmental stages. As hypothesized, it was stronger for the middle childhood category (r = −.328, p < .001) than for younger children (r = −.112, p = .002), but was nonsignificant in the adolescence group (r = −.129, p = .065). Also, in line with our expectations, parenting experience also turned out to be a significant moderator, such that the inverse relationship between parental mentalization and children’s externalizing problems was greater in the case of experienced parents (r = −.208, p < .001). In the rest of the moderation analyses – (a) different approaches for the operationalization of parental mentalization (MM or PRF), (b) sample type (clinical/at-risk or community), and (c) reporting figure (primary caregiver vs. others) –, there were no differences between the levels of the moderators (see Table 2). Therefore, our hypotheses in relation to these variables were not confirmed.

Table 2. Statistical test for heterogeneity and moderators included in the meta-analysis

Notes. CI = confidence Interval, QE = Q-statistic for residual heterogeneity, QM = Q-statistic for moderators.

Discussion

The purpose of this study was to review the literature on the relationship between parental mentalization and children’s externalizing problems, analyzing results from a quantitative perspective. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first attempt at summarizing the empirical results on the topic. The included studies were conducted in different countries, predominantly involved biological parents with their children, and were drawn from a combination of community and clinical/at-risk samples. We observed that the expected negative association between parental mentalization and child externalizing problems was predominantly consistent across studies. The magnitude of the effect size may be considered small to moderate (Funder & Ozer, Reference Funder and Ozer2019), being slightly stronger than similar meta-analyses in the field, such as those reviewing the predictive power of parental sensitivity on children’s behavior problems – e.g., r = −.14 in Cooke et al.’s (Reference Cooke, Deneault, Devereux, Eirich, Fearon and Madigan2022) and r = −.08 in Rodrigues et al.’s (Reference Rodrigues, Sokolovic, Madigan, Luo, Silva, Misra and Jenkins2021).

Thus, it seems that parental ability to understand a child’s mental states allows caregivers to better respond to the child’s needs, likely prompting a decrease in externalizing problems and confirming the relevance of paying special attention to parenting mentalization. This finding reveals substantive effects that underline the importance of intervening with parents on mentalization as a useful strategy that can contribute to prevent later children’s externalizing problems.

Beyond the overall negative association between parental mentalization and externalizing problems in offspring, the significant heterogeneity in this relationship suggests the need to consider potential moderators. With respect to the possible moderators examined, the different approaches and measures to parental mentalization did not reveal significant effects. Children’s developmental stage (early and middle childhood) and parenting experience (experienced parenting vs. first-time parents) were found to strengthen the negative relationship between parental mentalization and externalizing problems. Population type (clinical/at risk vs. community), and reporting figure (primary caregivers vs. others) failed to reach statistical significance. Insightfulness and parental education could not be analyzed due to lack of data. Below we outline some reflections on the possible meaning of our results.

Contrary to what expected, different operationalization of parental mentalization (offline-representational vs. online-interactional) did not moderate the relationship between parental mentalization and externalizing problems. Although different approaches to parental mentalization are reported in the literature (e.g., Medrea & Benga, Reference Medrea and Benga2021), our results suggest that these may be closer to each other. This may indicate that parental mentalization might be a singular construct that can be operationalized in various ways. In line with our results, Yatziv et al. (Reference Yatziv, Kessler and Atzaba-Poria2020) found PRF (offline-representational) and MM (online-interactional) to be positively correlated, that is, mothers who tended to spontaneously describe their child in a mentalistic manner during interactions (MM) were more likely to have more elaborated representations of their child’s mind (PRF). Hence, both operationalizations appear to explore the common domain of the parent–child relationship through the lenses of parent’s mentalizing abilities.

In line with our expectations, children’s developmental stage moderated the negative relationship between parental mentalization and externalizing behaviors. These results show that parental mentalization is inversely associated with externalizing problems both in the early and middle childhood age groups as compared to adolescence. In addition, the relationship was stronger during middle childhood as compared to early childhood. Differences in developmental competencies and in the relative weight of contextual influences among these three age groups may explain these results.

Even though in the context of younger children there is a significant reduction in heterogeneity, child’s temperament may play an important role in predicting externalizing symptoms (Frick & Morris, Reference Frick and Morris2004; Muris & Ollendick, Reference Muris and Ollendick2005; Putnam et al., Reference Putnam, Ellis, Rothbart, Eliasz and Angleitner2001), and temperament influences seem to be stronger in the early years (Bates et al., Reference Bates, Schermerhorn, Petersen, Zentner, Shiner, Zentner and Shiner2012). Thus, the effect of child temperament might be associated with increased attenuation of the relationship between parental mentalization and less externalizing behaviors in the younger children group. Furthermore, greater competences in different domains of development in middle childhood (vs. early childhood) may be responsible for the stronger negative relationship between parental mentalization and externalizing problems in this age group. For example, some characteristic advances in cognitive competences in middle childhood underlie the increase in socio-cognitive skills typical of this stage (Collins et al., Reference Collins, Madsen, Susman-Stillman and Bornstein2013), such as children’s mentalization capacities. Middle childhood can be considered a key phase in the development of lifelong mentalization because it can only confidently be stated that a normally developing child has a full and explicit mentalization capacity from the age of six onwards (Frith & Frith, Reference Frith and Frith2003). Since children’s and adolescents’ own mentalization has been associated with externalizing problems (Midgley et al., Reference Midgley, Ensink, Lindqvist, Malberg, Muller, Lindqvist, Malberg and Muller2017), better mentalizing equipment in middle childhood (as compared to the earlier stage) may support a stronger association between parental mentalization and children’s externalizing behaviors.

In addition to this tentative explanation, and closely related to mentalization, deficits in self-regulation have also been linked to externalizing problems (Eisenberg et al., Reference Eisenberg, Hernández and Spinrad2017, Perry et al., Reference Perry, Calkins, Dollar, Keane and Shanahan2018). There is longitudinal evidence of age-related increases in self-regulation, especially from early to middle childhood. Specifically, in Raffaelli et al.’s (Reference Raffaelli, Crockett and Shen2005) study, a significant increase in self-regulation only was observed between early and middle childhood, whereas the difference between middle childhood and early adolescence was not significant. Therefore, developmental differences in self-regulation may contribute to the relationship between parental mentalization and fewer externalizing problems being stronger in middle than in early childhood.

Regarding the absence of a link between parental mentalization on adolescents’ externalizing problems, peer influences may partly account for this finding. Although there is an increase of the influence of peers and other adults outside the family sphere on children’s development in middle childhood, it is not until early adolescence that contact with peers dominate their social networks (Steinberg & Silk, Reference Steinberg, Silk and Bornstein2013). In fact, during adolescence peer influence extends particularly to externalizing behaviors (Giletta et al., Reference Giletta, Choukas-Bradley, Maes, Linthicum, Card and Prinstein2021). Adolescents tend to influence each other’s externalizing problems within the same social circle, often mirroring and adopting behaviors over time through mechanisms such as encouragement, emulation, and peer pressure (Fortuin et al., Reference Fortuin, van Geel and Vedder2015). Specifically, mild externalizing problems significantly affect adolescents’ likability within their peer groups, leading them to align their behaviors with those of their peers. Hence, in the case of adolescence, the influence of peers may be more significant, thus diminishing the importance of the negative relationship between parental mentalization and externalizing problems. Other influences may refer to other parental skills, where harsh and psychological control are associated with more externalizing problems over time (Pinquart, Reference Pinquart2017a), while parental monitoring predicted lower externalizing behaviors (Bailey et al., Reference Bailey, Hill, Oesterle and Hawkins2009; Lopez-Tamayo et al., Reference Lopez-Tamayo, LaVome Robinson, Lambert, Jason and Ialongo2016). Such findings may indicate that, during adolescence, both positive and negative parenting may exert a stronger effect on externalizing behaviors than parental mentalization.

Overall, other factors (i.e., temperament during early childhood, increased developmental competences in middle childhood, and peer influence and/or parental monitoring during adolescence) may diminish the effect of parental mentalization on children’s externalizing problems. These third variables may explain the stronger decrease of externalizing problems in middle childhood as compared to the other two age groups. In support of this idea, Lo and Wong’s (Reference Lo and Wong2022) meta-analysis demonstrates that interventions designed to enhance PRF were more effective for parents of school-aged children compared to parents of infants. Thus, interventions to enhance parental mentalization in order to prevent or reduce children’s externalizing problems, should be precisely directed to the middle childhood age group. In this regard, given that low-quality studies seem to prevail in middle childhood research (Midgley et al., Reference Midgley, Sprecher and Sleed2021), it is imperative to attain a solid foundation for mentalization-based interventions at this stage of development.

In line with our assumption, parenting experience also moderated the association between parental mentalization and children’s externalizing problems, indicating that in the case of experienced parents the negative relationship is stronger. In other words, with the experience gained in raising previous children (Whiteman et al., Reference Whiteman, McHale and Crouter2003), parental mentalization exerts a stronger effect in diminishing externalizing problems. Moreover, other parental characteristics such as parental stress (Mak et al., Reference Mak, Yin, Li, Cheung and Oon2020), low parental self-efficacy (Bodalski et al., Reference Bodalski, Joshua Bradley, Neger, Shi, Bridges and Flory2023), and low marital satisfaction (Vaez et al., Reference Vaez, Indran, Abdollahi, Juhari and Mansor2015) are associated with children’s externalizing symptoms and may influence the relationship between the target variables of this meta-analysis. In fact, some studies reveal the above factors to be typically present in first-time parents (i.e., low parental self-efficacy in Amin et al., Reference Amin, Tam and Shorey2018; high parental stress in Lévesque et al., Reference Lévesque, Bisson, Charton and Fernet2020, and low marital satisfaction in Bogdan et al., Reference Bogdan, Turliuc and Candel2022). Hence, these parental characteristics may affect the relationship between parental mentalization and externalizing problems. Consequently, in order to reduce externalizing problems through improving parental mentalization skills, practitioners may deliver their interventions focusing on the experienced parents.

We hypothesized that sample type would influence the relationship between parental mentalization and children’s externalizing problems. Surprisingly, our analysis revealed no significant impact of the population type on this association. This aligns with recent meta-analyses investigating parental sensitivity and behavior problems, which similarly found no substantial moderating effect when comparing clinical/at-risk samples against the general population (Cooke et al., Reference Cooke, Deneault, Devereux, Eirich, Fearon and Madigan2022; Rodrigues et al., Reference Rodrigues, Sokolovic, Madigan, Luo, Silva, Misra and Jenkins2021). Despite empirical studies suggesting a potentially stronger influence of parental mentalization in clinical/at-risk samples (e.g., Hughes et al., Reference Hughes, Aldercotte and Foley2017), our meta-analysis did not find such association.

The vast diversity (i.e., from low socioeconomic status to inpatient psychiatric facility) and the broad severity (i.e., COVID vs. childhood sexual abuse) within the clinical/at-risk sample might explain the unexpected absence of moderation effects. Such variability within these populations could lead to differing correlations. For instance, Suardi et al. (Reference Suardi, Moser, Sancho Rossignol, Manini, Vital, Merminod, Kreis, Ansermet, Serpa and Schechter2020) reported a more robust relationship between parental mentalization and externalizing problems in a posttraumatic stress disordered parents’ sample, while Walker et al. (Reference Walker, Wheatcroft and Camic2012) found a stronger relationship in the general population. These findings underscore the complexity of controlling for severity sources within clinical/at-risk samples and emphasize the need for further research to delineate the nuanced influences of different risk factors within diverse samples on the extent of moderation effects.

Finally, we expected that the relationship would be stronger when primary caregivers reported on both mentalization and externalizing behaviors; yet, the results show no influence of the reporting figure on this relation. This finding may be explained due to children behaving differently at home or at school, allowing both teachers and caregivers to provide unique responses. It might also be due to the unique way parents perceive their child’s behavior depending on gender. For example, Mowlem et al. (Reference Mowlem, Agnew-Blais, Taylor and Asherson2019) found that parents overrated externalizing symptoms (e.g., hyperactivity/impulsivity) in boys but not in girls. Nevertheless, other authors reported similar results on externalizing problems both on parents and teachers after the pandemic (Watts & Pattnaik, Reference Watts and Pattnaik2023). In any case, further consideration of both parental and teacher perspective in reporting may provide valuable insights given the distinct experiences and observations of these informants (Deneault et al., Reference Deneault, Hammond and Madigan2023).

Limitations and future directions

The current meta-analytic review is not without limitations. Firstly, although certain possible moderators may be influential, eventually they could not be examined due to the limited number of studies. This was the case of factors such as both the caregivers’ and children’s gender. Upon data availability in the future, these questions may be explored. In fact, parents often respond differently to the child based on their children’s gender, which included variations in displays of affection (Morawska, Reference Morawska2020). Boys tend to display more anger and aggression (Perra et al., Reference Perra, Paine and Hay2021), while girls are perceived as being more cuddly (Sechi et al., Reference Sechi, Vismara, Rollè, Prino and Lucarelli2020). Research also suggests that parents typically display higher PM tendencies with daughters compared to sons, suggesting greater challenges in understanding daughters’ mental experiences (Pazzagli et al., Reference Pazzagli, Delvecchio, Raspa, Mazzeschi and Luyten2018). Moreover, bearing in mind that the significance of a child’s temperament in relation to parenting and developmental aspects is widely acknowledged (e.g., Belsky & van IJzendoorn, Reference Belsky and van IJzendoorn2017; Clark et al., Reference Clark, Kochanska and Ready2000), its examination as a possible moderator should be warranted. Despite its importance, this variable was not included in the meta-analysis due to the limited existing studies focusing on a child’s temperament as a possible moderator. Nonetheless, future research endeavors should investigate the potential moderating role of a child’s temperament in the relationships between parental mentalization and child externalizing problems.

Secondly, we should acknowledge that the conceptualization of ‘externalizing problems’ as a unitary construct is troublesome. As pointed out by Bongers et al. (Reference Bongers, Koot, Van Der Ende and Verhulst2004), there are various manifestations within the spectrum of ‘externalizing behaviors’ – opposition, aggression, property violations, and status violations, according to Frick et al.’s (Reference Frick, Lahey, Loeber, Tannenbaum, Van Horn, Christ, Hart and Hanson1993) taxonomy. These various manifestations of symptoms may follow distinct developmental paths, and the influence of parental mentalization may vary across each trajectory.

A further possible limitation concerns the transactional nature of the relationship between parenting and externalizing problems in offspring. As the meta-analysis by Yan et al. (Reference Yan, Ansari and Peng2021) shows, not only do parental behaviors predict externalizing problems, but such problems have comparable effects on subsequent parental functioning. Given that there are not enough longitudinal studies to fully understand these dynamics over time, more prospective research is needed to gain deeper insights into how these transactions evolve and inform more effective intervention strategies. Finally, since we only analyzed externalizing problems, future research may benefit from the comparison of effects on the relationship between parental mentalization and both externalizing and internalizing behaviors.

Final conclusions

In the context of practical applications, equipping caregivers with appropriate mentalization skills could enhance their ability to regulate their own and their children’s emotions, and responding with attuned behaviors to address appropriately the needs of their sons or daughters. Evidence shows that improvement in parental mentalization is associated with lower levels of children’s externalizing problems (e.g., Camoirano, Reference Camoirano2017). Our results suggest that parental programs aimed at improving parental mentalization skills to prevent and/or mitigate children’s externalizing behaviors are particularly recommended in the case of experienced parents and during the middle childhood developmental stage. Beyond the reduction of externalizing problems, mentalization-based interventions are suggested to be beneficial for both parents and children. Notably, improvements have been observed in caregivers’ psychological distress and reflective function (Byrne et al., Reference Byrne, Murphy and Connon2020) and in children’s general well-being (Midgley et al., Reference Midgley, Sprecher and Sleed2021). All in all, our findings pave the way for practitioners to design parental mentalization interventions that may prevent potential future criminal trajectories.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579424001391.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank researchers who promptly answered our requests for additional data and/or analyses from their studies.

Authors contributions

M.N.R. – Conceptualized and designed the study, funding acquisition, literature search, data coding, data extraction, statistical analyses, interpretation of data, and drafting and first write-up of the manuscript.

B.T.G. – Conceptualized and designed the study, funding acquisition, interpretation of data, overall supervision, and write-up of the manuscript.

I.A.A. – Conceptualized and designed the study, funding acquisition, interpretation of data, overall supervision, and write-up of the manuscript.

All authors discussed the results and contributed to the final version of the manuscript.

Funding statement

This research was supported by a pre-doctoral grant from the Education Department of the Basque Government (PRE_2021_2_0050) awarded to the first author under second and third author’s supervision, a grant from the Spanish Ministry of Science and Innovation (PID2020-115738 GB-I00, funded by MCIN/AEI/10.13039/501100011033/) and a grant from the Basque Government to Research Groups (‘Culture, Cognition, and Emotion’ Consolidated Group; IT1598-22). Open Access funding provided by the University of the Basque Country, UPV/EHU.

Competing interests

None.

Footnotes

1 The study was excluded if the total or a nonvalidated version of the PRFQ scale was reported, and no further data could be obtained.

References

Amin, N. A. L., Tam, W. W., & Shorey, S. (2018). Enhancing first-time parents’ self-efficacy: A systematic review and meta-analysis of universal parent education interventions’ efficacy. International Journal of Nursing Studies, 82, 149162. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijnurstu.2018.03.021 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bailey, J. A., Hill, K. G., Oesterle, S., & Hawkins, J. D. (2009). Parenting practices and problem behavior across three generations: Monitoring, harsh discipline, and drug use in the intergenerational transmission of externalizing behavior. Developmental Psychology, 45(5), 12141226. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0016129 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., Van Ijzendoorn, M. H., & Juffer, F. (2003). Less is more: Meta-analyses of sensitivity and attachment interventions in early childhood. Psychological Bulletin, 129(2), 195215. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.129.2.195 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bateman, A. W. (2022). Mentalization-based treatment. Psychoanalytic Inquiry, 33(6), 595613. https://doi.org/10.1080/07351690.2013.835170 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bateman, A. W., & Fonagy, P. (2019). Mentalization-based treatment for borderline and antisocial personality disorder. In Kealy, D. & Ogrodniczuk, J. S. (Eds.), Contemporary psychodynamic psychotherapy (pp. 133148). Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bates, J. E., Schermerhorn, A. C., Petersen, I. T., Zentner, M., & Shiner, R. L. (2012). Temperament and parenting in developmental perspective. In Zentner, M. & Shiner, R. L. (Eds.), Handbook of temperament (pp. 425441). The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Belsky, J., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2017). Genetic differential susceptibility to the effects of parenting. Current Opinion in Psychology, 15, 125130. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.02.021 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benbassat, N., & Priel, B. (2012). Parenting and adolescent adjustment: The role of parental reflective function. Journal of Adolescence, 35(1), 163174. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2011.03.004 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bevilacqua, L., Hale, D., Barker, E. D., & Viner, R. (2018). Conduct problems trajectories and psychosocial outcomes: A systematic review and meta-analysis. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 27(10), 12391260. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-017-1053-4 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bodalski, E. A., Joshua Bradley, W., Neger, E., Shi, D., Bridges, R., & Flory, K. (2023). Parenting self-efficacy and internalizing/externalizing problems: Child age as a moderator. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 32(4), 11381147. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-022-02402-1 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bogdan, I., Turliuc, M. N., & Candel, O. S. (2022). Transition to parenthood and marital satisfaction: A meta-analysis. Frontiers in Psychology, 13, 901362. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.901362 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bongers, I. L., Koot, H. M., Van Der Ende, J., & Verhulst, F. C. (2004). Developmental trajectories of externalizing behaviors in childhood and adolescence. Child Development, 75(5), 15231537. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00755.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Borelli, J. L., Palmer, A., Vanwoerden, S., & Sharp, C. (2019). Convergence in reports of adolescents’ psychopathology: A focus on disorganized attachment and reflective functioning. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 58(4), 568581. https://doi.org/10.1080/15374416.2017.1399400 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Borelli, J. L., Yates, T. M., Hecht, H. K., Cervantes, B. R., Russo, L. N., Arreola, J., Leal, F., Torres, G., & Guerra, N. (2021). Confía en mí, confío en ti: Applying developmental theory to mitigate sociocultural risk in Latinx families. Development and Psychopathology, 33(2), 581597. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579420001364 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Borenstein, M., & Hedges, L. V. (2019). Effect sizes for meta-analysis. In Cooper, H., Hedges, L. V. & , J. C. Valentine(Eds.), The handbook of research synthesis and meta-analysis (3rd ed., pp. 207243). Russell Sage Foundation.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, M. P., Ng, R., Lisle, J., Koenig, M., Sannes, D., Rogosch, F., & Cicchetti, D. (2023). Mind-mindedness in a high-risk sample: Differential benefits for developmental outcomes based on child maltreatment. Developmental Psychology, 59(6), 11261135. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0001506 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Byrne, G., Murphy, S., & Connon, G. (2020). Mentalization-based treatments with children and families: A systematic review of the literature. Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 25(4), 10221048. https://doi.org/10.1177/1359104520920689 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caldarera, A. M., Vitiello, B., Turcich, C., Bechis, D., & Baietto, C. (2022). The association of attachment, mentalization and reflective functioning with mental health in gender diverse children and adolescents: A systematic review. Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 27(4), 11241140. https://doi.org/10.1177/13591045221075527 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Camisasca, E., Miragoli, S., Ionio, C., Milani, L., & Di Blasio, P. (2018). Post-partum depressive symptoms and child behavior: The mediational role of maternal mind-mindedness. Children’s Health Care, 47(2), 165183. https://doi.org/10.1080/14616734.2020.1734641 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Camoirano, A. (2017). Mentalizing makes parenting work: A review about parental reflective functioning and clinical interventions to improve it. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 244245. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00014 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carlone, C., & Milan, S. (2021). Does your child need therapy? Maternal reflective functioning and perceived need for and use of child mental health treatment. Attachment & Human Development, 23(3), 310327. https://doi.org/10.1080/02739615.2017.1318389 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carlone, C., Milan, S., Decoste, C., Borelli, J. L., McMahon, T. J., & Suchman, N. E. (2023). Self-report measure of parental reflective functioning: A study of reliability and validity across three samples of varying clinical risk. Infant Mental Health Journal, 44(2), 240254. https://doi.org/10.1002/imhj.22046 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carneiro, A., Soares, I., Rescorla, L., & Dias, P. (2021). Meta-analysis on parent-teacher agreement on preschoolers’ emotional and behavioural problems. Child Psychiatry & Human Development, 52(4), 609618. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10578-020-01044-y CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Centifanti, L. C., Meins, E., & Fernyhough, C. (2016). Callous-unemotional traits and impulsivity: Distinct longitudinal relations with mind-mindedness and understanding of others. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 57(1), 8492. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12445 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Charpentier Mora, S., Bastianoni, C., Koren-Karie, N., Cavanna, D., Tironi, M., & Bizzi, F. (2022). Parental mentalizing during middle childhood: How is the adoption of a reflective stance associated with child’s psychological outcomes? International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 19(10), 6205. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19106205 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, L. A., Kochanska, G., & Ready, R. (2000). Mothers’ personality and its interaction with child temperament as predictors of parenting behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79(2), 274285. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.79.2.274 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Collins, W. A., Madsen, S. D., & Susman-Stillman, A. (2013). Parenting during middle childhood. In Bornstein, M. H. (Ed.), Handbook of parenting: Vol. I. Children and parenting (2nd ed., pp. 73102). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Colonnesi, C., Konijn, C., Kroneman, L., Lindauer, R. J., & Stams, G. J. J. (2022). Mind-mindedness in out-of-home care for children: Implications for caregivers and child. Current Psychology, 41(11), 77187730. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-020-01271-5 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Colonnesi, C., Zeegers, M. A., Majdandžić, M., van Steensel, F. J., & Bögels, S. M. (2019). Correction: Fathers’ and mothers’ early mind-mindedness predicts social competence and behavior problems in childhood. Research on Child and Adolescent Psychopathology, 51(6), 905. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-019-00537-2 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Condon, E. M., Holland, M. L., Slade, A., Redeker, N. S., Mayes, L. C., & Sadler, L. S. (2019). Associations between maternal caregiving and child indicators of toxic stress among multiethnic, urban families. Journal of Pediatric Health Care, 33(4), 425436. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pedhc.2018.12.002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cooke, J. E., Deneault, A. A., Devereux, C., Eirich, R., Fearon, R. P., & Madigan, S. (2022). Parental sensitivity and child behavioral problems: A meta-analytic review. Child Development, 93(5), 12311248. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13764 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deneault, A.A., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., Groh, A. M., Fearon, P. R. M., & Madigan, S. (2021). Child-father attachment in early childhood and behavior problems: A meta-analysis. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 2021(180), 4366. https://doi.org/10.1002/cad.20434 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deneault, A. A., Hammond, S. I., & Madigan, S. (2023). A meta-analysis of child-parent attachment in early childhood and prosociality. Developmental Psychology, 59(2), 236255. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0001484.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diamantopoulou, S., Verhulst, F. C., & van der Ende, J. (2010). Testing developmental pathways to antisocial personality problems. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 38, 91103. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-009-9348-7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dieleman, L. M., Soenens, B., De Pauw, S. S., Prinzie, P., Vansteenkiste, M., & Luyten, P. (2020). The role of parental reflective functioning in the relation between parents’ self-critical perfectionism and psychologically controlling parenting towards adolescents. Parenting, 20(1), 127. https://doi.org/10.1080/15295192.2019.1642087 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dodge, K. A., & Pettit, G. S. (2003). A biopsychosocial model of the development of chronic conduct problems in adolescence. Developmental Psychology, 39(2), 349371. https://doi.org/10.1037//0012-1649.39.2.349 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dollberg, D. G. (2022). Mothers’ parental mentalization, attachment dimensions and mother-infant relational patterns. Attachment & Human Development, 24(2), 189207. https://doi.org/10.1080/14616734.2021.1901297 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dollberg, D. G., Hanetz-Gamliel, K., & Levy, S. (2021). Mediating and moderating links between coparenting, parental mentalization, parents’ anxiety, and children’s behavior problems. Journal of Family Psychology, 35(3), 324334. https://doi.org/10.1037/fam0000728 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dollberg, D. G., Hanetz-Gamliel, K., & Levy, S. (2023). COVID-19, child’s behavior problems, and mother’s anxiety and mentalization: A mediated moderation model. Current Psychology, 42(14), 1173311744. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-021-02476-y CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eisenberg, N., Hernández, M. M., & Spinrad, T. L. (2017). The relation of self-regulation to children’s externalizing and internalizing problems. Emotion Regulation and Psychopathology in Children and Adolescents, 1, 1842. https://doi.org/10.1093/med:psych/9780198765844.003.0002 Google Scholar
Ensink, K., Bégin, M., Normandin, L., & Fonagy, P. (2016). Maternal and child reflective functioning in the context of child sexual abuse: Pathways to depression and externalising difficulties. European Journal of Psychotraumatology, 7(1), 30611. https://doi.org/10.3402/ejpt.v7.30611 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ensink, K., Bégin, M., Normandin, L., & Fonagy, P. (2017). Parental reflective functioning as a moderator of child internalizing difficulties in the context of child sexual abuse. Psychiatry Research, 257, 361366. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2017.07.051 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ensink, K., Bégin, M., Normandin, L., Godbout, N., & Fonagy, P. (2017). Mentalization and dissociation in the context of trauma: Implications for child psychopathology. Journal of Trauma & Dissociation, 18(1), 1130. https://doi.org/10.1080/15299732.2016.1172536 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ensink, K., Leroux, A., Normandin, L., Biberdzic, M., & Fonagy, P. (2017). Assessing reflective parenting in interaction with school-aged children. Journal of Personality Assessment, 99(6), 585595. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223891.2016.1270289 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fearon, R. P., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., Van IJzendoorn, M. H., Lapsley, A. M., & Roisman, G. I. (2010). The significance of insecure attachment and disorganization in the development of children’s externalizing behavior: A meta-analytic study. Child Development, 81(2), 435456. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01405.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Feniger-Schaal, R., & Koren-Karie, N. (2021). Using drama therapy to enhance maternal insightfulness and reduce children’s behavior problems. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 586630. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.586630 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fortuin, J., van Geel, M., & Vedder, P. (2015). Peer influences on internalizing and externalizing problems among adolescents: A longitudinal social network analysis. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 44(4), 887897. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-014-0168-x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frick, P. J., Lahey, B. B., Loeber, R., Tannenbaum, L., Van Horn, Y., Christ, M. A. G., Hart, E. A., & Hanson, K. (1993). Oppositional defiant disorder and conduct disorder: A meta-analytic review of factor analyses and cross-validation in a clinic sample. Clinical Psychology Review, 13(4), 319340. https://doi.org/10.1016/0272-7358(93)90016-F CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frick, P. J., & Morris, A. S. (2004). Temperament and developmental pathways to conduct problems. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 33(1), 5468. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15374424JCCP3301_6 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frith, U., & Frith, C. D. (2003). Development and neurophysiology of mentalizing. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, 358(1431), 459473. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2002.1218 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Funder, D. C., & Ozer, D. J. (2019). Evaluating effect size in psychological research: Sense and nonsense. Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science, 2, 156168. https://doi.org/10.1177/2515245919847202 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Georg, A. K., Meyerhöfer, S., Taubner, S., & Volkert, J. (2023). Is parental depression related to parental mentalizing? A systematic review and three-level meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 104, 102322. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2023.102322 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ghanbari, S., Goudarzi, Z., Ebrahimi, F., Sabzalizadeh, F., Erfanian Delavar, D., & Javaherian, N. (2023). Temperament and emotional and behavioral problems in children: The moderating role of maternal reflective functioning. Journal of Infant, Child, and Adolescent Psychotherapy, 22(2), 138153. https://doi.org/10.1080/15289168.2023.2193132 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Giletta, M., Choukas-Bradley, S., Maes, M., Linthicum, K. P., Card, N. A., & Prinstein, M. J. (2021). A meta-analysis of longitudinal peer influence effects in childhood and adolescence. Psychological Bulletin, 147(7), 719747. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000329 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodnight, J. A., Donahue, K. L., Waldman, I. D., Van Hulle, C. A., Rathouz, P. J., Lahey, B. B., & D’Onofrio, B. M. (2016). Genetic and environmental contributions to associations between infant fussy temperament and antisocial behavior in childhood and adolescence. Behavior Genetics, 46(5), 680692. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10519-016-9794-2 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gray, S. A., Forbes, D., Briggs-Gowan, M. J., & Carter, A. S. (2015). Caregiver insightfulness and young children’s violence exposure: Testing a relational model of risk and resilience. Attachment & Human Development, 17(6), 615634. https://doi.org/10.1080/14616734.2015.1100207 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ha, C., Sharp, C., & Goodyer, I. (2011). The role of child and parental mentalizing for the development of conduct problems over time. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 20(6), 291300. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-011-0174-4 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Herzog, R., Álvarez-Pasquin, M. J., Díaz, C., Del Barrio, J. L., Estrada, J. M., & Gil, Á. (2013). Are healthcare workers’ intentions to vaccinate related to their knowledge, beliefs and attitudes? A systematic review. BMC Public Health, 13(1), 117. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2458-13-154 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hewitt-Ramírez, N., & Moreno-Méndez, J. H. (2018). Intervención psicológica para comportamientos externalizados e internalizados en niños de 8 a 12 años. Revista de Psicología Clínica con Niños y Adolescentes, 5(2), 3742. https://doi.org/10.21134/rpcna.2018.05.2.5 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hobby, L., Bird, A. L., Townsend, M. L., & Barnes, J. (2023). Mind-mindedness and preschool children’s behavioral difficulties: The moderating role of maternal parenting distress. Development and Psychopathology, 35(4), 15841596. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579422000311 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoyt, W. T. (2000). Rater bias in psychological research: When is it a problem and what can we do about it? Psychological Methods, 5(1), 6486. https://doi.org/10.1037//I082-9S9X.5.1.64 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hughes, C., Aldercotte, A., & Foley, S. (2017). Maternal mind-mindedness provides a buffer for pre-adolescents at risk for disruptive behavior. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 45(2), 225235. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-016-0165-5 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kassing, F., Godwin, J., Lochman, J. E., Coie, J. D., & Conduct Problems Prevention Research Group (2019). Using early childhood behavior problems to predict adult convictions. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 47(5), 765778. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-018-0478-7 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kelly, K., Slade, A., & Grienenberger, J. F. (2005). Maternal reflective functioning, mother-infant affective communication, and infant attachment: Exploring the link between mental states and observed caregiving behavior in the intergenerational transmission of attachment. Attachment & Human Development, 7(3), 299311. https://doi.org/10.1080/14616730500245963 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Khoshroo, S., & Seyed Mousavi, P. S. (2022). Parental reflective functioning as a moderator for the relationship between maternal depression and child internalizing and externalizing problems. Child Psychiatry & Human Development, 53(6), 13191329. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10578-021-01214-6 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kochanska, G., & An, D. (2023). The parent’s and the child’s internal working models of each other moderate cascades from child difficulty to socialization outcomes: Preliminary evidence for dual moderation? Development and Psychopathology, 36(2), 114. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0954579422001365 Google Scholar
Konijn, C., Colonnesi, C., Kroneman, L., Liefferink, N., Lindauer, R. J., & Stams, G. J. J. (2020). ‘Caring for children who have experienced trauma’ -an evaluation of a training for foster parents. European Journal of Psychotraumatology, 11(1), 1756563. https://doi.org/10.1080/20008198.2020.1756563 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koren Swisa, R., Hanetz-Gamliel, K., & Dollberg, D. G. (2024). Mothers’ and fathers’ mentalization as moderators of the association between marital conflict and children’s internalizing and externalizing behaviors. Family Relations, 73(2), 703719. https://doi.org/10.1111/fare.12926 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lavender, S. R., Waters, C. S., & Hobson, C. W. (2023). The efficacy of group delivered mentalization-based parenting interventions: A systematic review of the literature. Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 28(2), 761784. https://doi.org/10.1177/13591045221113392 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lévesque, S., Bisson, V., Charton, L., & Fernet, M. (2020). Parenting and relational well-being during the transition to parenthood: Challenges for first-time parents. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 29(7), 19381956. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-020-01727-z CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lin, S. C., Kehoe, C., Pozzi, E., Liontos, D., & Whittle, S. (2024). Research review: Child emotion regulation mediates the association between family factors and internalizing symptoms in children and adolescents-a meta-analysis. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 65(3), 260274. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.13894 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lo, C. K., & Wong, S. Y. (2022). The effectiveness of parenting programs in regard to improving parental reflective functioning: A meta-analysis. Attachment & Human Development, 24(1), 7692. https://doi.org/10.1080/14616734.2020.1844247 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lopez-Tamayo, R., LaVome Robinson, W., Lambert, S. F., Jason, L. A., & Ialongo, N. S. (2016). Parental monitoring, association with externalized behavior, and academic outcomes in urban African-American youth: A moderated mediation analysis. American Journal of Community Psychology, 57(3-4), 366379. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajcp.12056 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lunn, J., Lewis, C., & Gannon, E. (2019). Parent-child mentalizing in pediatric epilepsy. Epilepsy & Behavior, 96, 612. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yebeh.2019.03.052 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luyten, P., Mayes, L. C., Nijssens, L., & Fonagy, P. (2017). The Parental Reflective Functioning Questionnaire: Development and preliminary validation. PloS ONE, 12(5), e0176218. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0176218 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mak, M. C. K., Yin, L., Li, M., Cheung, R. Y. H., & Oon, P. T. (2020). The relation between parenting stress and child behavior problems: Negative parenting styles as mediator. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 29(11), 29933003. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-020-01785-3 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Medrea, F. L., & Benga, O. (2021). Parental mentalization: A critical literature review of mind-mindedness, parental insightfulness and parental reflective functioning. Cognition, Brain, Behavior, 25(1), 69105. https://doi.org/10.24193/cbb.2021.25.05 Google Scholar
Meins, E. (2013). Sensitive attunement to infants’ internal states: Operationalizing the construct of mind-mindedness. Attachment & Human Development, 15, 524544. https://doi.org/10.1080/14616734.2013 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meins, E., Centifanti, L. C. M., Fernyhough, C., & Fishburn, S. (2013). Maternal mind-mindedness and children’s behavioral difficulties: Mitigating the impact of low socioeconomic status. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 41(4), 543553. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-012-9699-3 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meins, E., & Fernyhough, C (2015). Mind-mindedness coding manual, Version 2.2. University of York (Unpublished manuscript).Google Scholar
Menashe-Grinberg, A., & Atzaba-Poria, N. (2023). The predictors of change in reflective parenting therapy: Uncovering the influence of parental reflective functioning and child temperament in predicting the improvement in parent-child relationship and child outcome following DUET group intervention. Development and Psychopathology, 35(4), 19011912. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0954579422000566 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Midgley, N., Ensink, K., Lindqvist, K., Malberg, N., & Muller, N. (2017). When the capacity for mentalizing is underdeveloped or breaks down. In Lindqvist, K., Malberg, N., & Muller, N. (Eds.), Mentalization-based treatment for children: A time-limited approach (pp. 3960). American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/0000028-003 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Midgley, N., Sprecher, E. A., & Sleed, M. (2021). Mentalization-based interventions for children aged 6-12 and their carers: A narrative systematic review. Journal of Infant, Child, and Adolescent Psychotherapy, 20(2), 169189. https://doi.org/10.1080/15289168.2021.1915654 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moffit, T. E. (2017). Adolescence-limited and life-course-persistent antisocial behavior: A developmental taxonomy. Psychological Review, 100(4), 674701. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781315096278-3 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morawska, A. (2020). The effects of gendered parenting on child development outcomes: A systematic review. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 23(4), 553576. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10567-020-00321-5 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mowlem, F., Agnew-Blais, J., Taylor, E., & Asherson, P. (2019). Do different factors influence whether girls versus boys meet ADHD diagnostic criteria? Sex differences among children with high ADHD symptoms. Psychiatry Research, 272, 765773. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2018.12.128 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muris, P., & Ollendick, T. H. (2005). The role of temperament in the etiology of child psychopathology. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 8(4), 271289. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10567-005-8809-y CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nijssens, L., Vliegen, N., & Luyten, P. (2020). The mediating role of parental reflective functioning in child social-emotional development. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 29(8), 23422354. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-020-01767-5 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oppenheim, D., & Koren-Karie, N. (2002). Mothers’ insightfulness regarding their children’s internal worlds: The capacity underlying secure child-mother relationships. Infant Mental Health Journal, 23(6), 593605. https://doi.org/10.1002/imhj.10035 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Page, M. J., McKenzie, J. E., Bossuyt, P. M., Boutron, I., Hoffmann, T. C., Mulrow, C. D., Shamseer, L., Tetzlaff, J. M., Akl, E. A., Brennan, S. E., Chou, R., Glanville, J., Grimshaw, J. M., Hróbjartsson, Aørn, Lalu, M. M., Li, T., Loder, E. W., Mayo-Wilson, E., ... Moher, D. (2021). The PRISMA. 2020 statement: An updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. British Medical Journal, 372(71). https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.n71 Google ScholarPubMed
Pazzagli, C., Delvecchio, E., Raspa, V., Mazzeschi, C., & Luyten, P. (2018). The Parental Reflective Functioning Questionnaire in mothers and fathers of school-aged children. Journal of Child & Family Studies, 27(1), 8090. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-017-0856-8 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perra, O., Paine, A. L., & Hay, D. F. (2021). Continuity and change in anger and aggressiveness from infancy to childhood: The protective effects of positive parenting. Development and Psychopathology, 33(3), 937956. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579420000243 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perry, N. B., Calkins, S. D., Dollar, J. M., Keane, S. P., & Shanahan, L. (2018). Self-regulation as a predictor of patterns of change in externalizing behaviors from infancy to adolescence. Development and Psychopathology, 30(2), 497510. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579417000992 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Petersen, I. T., Bates, J. E., Dodge, K. A., Lansford, J. E., & Pettit, G. S. (2015). Describing and predicting developmental profiles of externalizing problems from childhood to adulthood. Development and Psychopathology, 27(3), 791818. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579414000789 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peterson, E. R., Dando, E., D’Souza, S., Waldie, K. E., Carr, A. E., Mohal, J., & Morton, S. M. (2018). Can infant temperament be used to predict which toddlers are likely to have increased emotional and behavioral problems? Early Education and Development, 29(4), 435449. https://doi.org/10.1080/10409289.2018.1457391 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peterson, R. A., & Brown, S. P. (2005). On the use of beta coefficients in meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 90(1), 175181. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.90.1.175 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pinquart, M. (2017a). Associations of parenting dimensions and styles with externalizing problems of children and adolescents: An updated meta-analysis. Developmental Psychology, 53(5), 873932. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000295 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pinquart, M. (2017b). Associations of parenting dimensions and styles with internalizing symptoms in children and adolescents: A meta-analysis. Marriage & Family Review, 53(7), 613664. https://doi.org/10.1080/01494929.2016.1247761 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Putnam, S. P., Ellis, L. K., & Rothbart, M. K. (2001). The structure of temperament from infancy through adolescence. In Eliasz, A. & Angleitner, A. (Eds.), Advances in research on temperament (pp. 165182). Pabst Science.Google Scholar
R Core Team (2022). R: A language and environment for statistical computing (Version 4.2.2). R Foundation for Statistical Computing.Google Scholar
Raffaelli, M., Crockett, L. J., & Shen, Y. L. (2005). Developmental stability and change in self-regulation from childhood to adolescence. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 166(1), 5476. https://doi.org/10.3200/GNTP.166.1.54-76 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rodrigues, M., Sokolovic, N., Madigan, S., Luo, Y., Silva, V., Misra, S., & Jenkins, J. (2021). Paternal sensitivity and children’s cognitive and socioemotional outcomes: A meta-analytic review. Child Development, 92(2), 554577. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13545 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rothbaum, F., & Weisz, J. R. (1994). Parental caregiving and child externalizing behavior in nonclinical samples: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 116(1), 5574. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.116.1.55 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rovira, P., Demontis, D., Sánchez-Mora, C., Zayats, T., Klein, M., Mota, N. R., Weber, H., Garcia-Martínez, I., Pagerols, M., Vilar-Ribó, L., Arribas, L., Richarte, V., Corrales, M., Fadeuilhe, C., Bosch, R., Martin, G. E., Almos, P., Doyle, A. E., Grevet, E. H., ... Ribasés, M. (2020). Shared genetic background between children and adults with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology, 45(10), 16171626. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41386-020-0664-5 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salo, S. J., Lipsanen, J. O., Sourander, J., Pajulo, M., & Kalland, M. (2022). Parental relationship satisfaction, reflective functioning, and toddler behavioral problems: A longitudinal study from pregnancy to 2 years postpartum. Frontiers in Psychology, 13, 904409. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.904409 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salo, S. J., Pajulo, M., Vinzce, L., Raittila, S., Sourander, J., & Kalland, M. (2021). Parent relationship satisfaction and reflective functioning as predictors of emotional availability and infant behavior. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 30(5), 12141228. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-021-01934-2 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sechi, C., Vismara, L., Rollè, L., Prino, L. E., & Lucarelli, L. (2020). First-time mothers’ and fathers’ developmental changes in the perception of their daughters’ and sons’ temperament: Its association with parents’ mental health. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 2066. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.02066 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shai, D., & Belsky, J. (2017). Parental embodied mentalizing: How the nonverbal dance between parents and infants predicts children`s socio-emotional functioning. Attachment & Human Development, 19(2), 191219. https://doi.org/10.1080/14616734.2016.1255653 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shai, D., Dollberg, D., & Szepsenwol, O. (2017). The importance of parental verbal and embodied mentalizing in shaping parental experiences of stress and coparenting. Infant Behavior and Development, 49, 8796. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2017.08.003 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shalev, I., Sharon, N., Uzefovsky, F., & Atzaba-Poria, N. (2023). Parental guilt and children’s internalizing and externalizing behavior: The moderating role of parental reflective functioning. Journal of Family Psychology, 37(8), 12411252. https://doi.org/10.1037/fam0001156 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sharp, C., & Fonagy, P. (2008). The parent’s capacity to treat the child as a psychological agent: Constructs, measures and implications for developmental psychopathology. Social Development, 17(3), 737754. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00457 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slade, A., Grienenberger, J., Bernbach, E., Levy, D., & Locker, A. (2005). Maternal reflective functioning, attachment, and the transmission gap: A preliminary study. Attachment & Human Development, 7(3), 283298. https://doi.org/10.1080/14616730500245880 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sleed, M., Slade, A., & Fonagy, P. (2020). Reflective functioning on the Parent Development Interview: Validity and reliability in relation to socio-demographic factors. Attachment & Human Development, 22(3), 310331. https://doi.org/10.1080/14616734.2018.1555603 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smaling, H. J. A., Huijbregts, S. C. J., van der Heijden, K. B., van Goozen, S. H. M., & Swaab, H. (2016). Maternal reflective functioning as a multidimensional construct: Differential associations with children’s temperament and externalizing behavior. Infant Behavior and Development, 44, 263274. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2016.06.007 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Steinberg, L., & Silk, J. S. (2013). Parenting adolescents. In Bornstein, M. H. (Ed.), Handbook of parenting: Vol. I. Children and parenting (2nd ed., pp. 103134). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Stuhrmann, L. Y., Göbel, A., Bindt, C., & Mudra, S. (2022). Parental reflective functioning and its association with parenting behaviors in infancy and early childhood: A systematic review. Frontiers in Psychology, 13, 765312. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.765312 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Suardi, F., Moser, D. A., Sancho Rossignol, A., Manini, A., Vital, M., Merminod, G., Kreis, A., Ansermet, F., Serpa, S. R., & Schechter, D. S. (2020). Maternal reflective functioning, interpersonal violence-related posttraumatic stress disorder, and risk for psychopathology in early childhood. Attachment & Human Development, 22(2), 225245. https://doi.org/10.1080/14616734.2018.1555602 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vaez, E., Indran, R., Abdollahi, A., Juhari, R., & Mansor, M. (2015). How marital relations affect child behavior: Review of recent research. Vulnerable Children and Youth Studies, 10(4), 321336. https://doi.org/10.1080/17450128.2015.1112454 Google Scholar
Vasileva, M., Graf, R. K., Reinelt, T., Petermann, U., & Petermann, F. (2021). Research review: A meta-analysis of the international prevalence and comorbidity of mental disorders in children between 1 and 7 years. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 62(4), 372381. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.13261 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Viechtbauer, W. (2010). Conducting meta-analyses in R with the metafor package. Journal of Statistical Software, 36(3), 148. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v036.i03 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wade, M., Plamondon, A., & Jenkins, J. M. (2021). A family socialization model of transdiagnostic risk for psychopathology in preschool children. Research on Child and Adolescent Psychopathology, 49(8), 975988. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-021-00789-x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walker, T. M., Wheatcroft, R., & Camic, P. M. (2012). Mind-mindedness in parents of pre-schoolers: A comparison between clinical and community samples. Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 17(3), 318335. https://doi.org/10.1177/1359104511409142 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Watts, R., & Pattnaik, J. (2023). Perspectives of parents and teachers on the impact of the COVID-19 pandemic on children’s socio-emotional well-being. Early Childhood Education Journal, 51(8), 15411552. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10643-022-01405-3 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Webster-Stratton, C., & Reid, M. J. (2018). The Incredible Years parents, teachers, and children training series: A multifaceted treatment approach for young children with conduct problems. In Weisz, J. R. & Kazdin, A. E. (Eds.), Evidence-based psychotherapies for children and adolescents (pp. 122141). Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Wells, G. A., Shea, B., O’Connell, D., Peterson, J., Welch, V., Losos, M., & Tugwell, P. (2000). The Newcastle-Ottawa Scale (NOS) for assessing the quality of nonrandomised studies in meta-analyses. Google Scholar
Whiteman, S. D., McHale, S. M., & Crouter, A. C. (2003). What parents learn from experience: The first child as a first draft? Journal of Marriage and the Family, 65, 608621. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1741-3737.2003.00608.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilson, D. B. (2023). Practical meta-analysis effect size calculator (Version 2023.11.27)https://www.campbellcollaboration.org/escalc/html/EffectSizeCalculator-Home.php.Google Scholar
Yan, N., Ansari, A., & Peng, P. (2021). Reconsidering the relation between parental functioning and child externalizing behaviors: A meta-analysis on child-driven effects. Journal of Family Psychology, 35(2), 225235. https://doi.org/10.1037/fam0000805 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yatziv, T., Kessler, Y., & Atzaba-Poria, N. (2020). When do mothers’ executive functions contribute to their representations of their child’s mind? A contextual view on parental reflective functioning and mind-mindedness. Developmental Psychology, 56(6), 11911206. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000931.supp CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zeegers, M. A. J., Colonnesi, C., Noom, M. J., Polderman, N., & Stams, G. J. J. (2020). Remediating child attachment insecurity: Evaluating the basic trust intervention in adoptive families. Research on Social Work Practice, 30(7), 736749. https://doi.org/10.1177/1049731519863106 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zeegers, M. A. J., Colonnesi, C., Stams, G.-J. J. M., & Meins, E. (2017). Mind matters: A meta-analysis on parental mentalization and sensitivity as predictors of infant-parent attachment. Psychological Bulletin, 143(12), 12451272. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000114 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zimmer-Gembeck, M. J., Kerin, J. L., Webb, H. J., Gardner, A. A., Campbell, S. M., Swan, K., & Timmer, S. G. (2019). Improved perceptions of emotion regulation and reflective functioning in parents: Two additional positive outcomes of parent-child interaction therapy. Behavior Therapy, 50(2), 340352. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2018.07.002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Figure 1. Flow diagram.

Figure 1

Table 1. Information of the studies included in the meta-analysis

Figure 2

Figure 2. Forest plot.

Figure 3

Figure 3. Funnel plot.

Figure 4

Table 2. Statistical test for heterogeneity and moderators included in the meta-analysis

Supplementary material: File

Nieto-Retuerto et al. supplementary material

Nieto-Retuerto et al. supplementary material
Download Nieto-Retuerto et al. supplementary material(File)
File 46.2 KB