Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-8bhkd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-10T05:23:57.094Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Predicting childhood ADHD-linked symptoms from prenatal and perinatal data in the ABCD cohort

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 March 2023

Niamh Dooley*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Royal College of Surgeons in Ireland, Dublin, Ireland
Colm Healy
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Royal College of Surgeons in Ireland, Dublin, Ireland
David Cotter
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Royal College of Surgeons in Ireland, Dublin, Ireland Department of Psychiatry, Beaumont Hospital, Dublin, Ireland
Mary Clarke
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Royal College of Surgeons in Ireland, Dublin, Ireland Department of Psychology, Royal College of Surgeons in Ireland, Dublin, Ireland
Mary Cannon
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Royal College of Surgeons in Ireland, Dublin, Ireland Department of Psychiatry, Beaumont Hospital, Dublin, Ireland
*
Corresponding author: Niamh Dooley, email: niamhdooley@rcsi.com
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

This study investigates the capacity of pre/perinatal factors to predict attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) symptoms in childhood. It also explores whether predictive accuracy of a pre/perinatal model varies for different groups in the population. We used the ABCD (Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development) cohort from the United States (N = 9975). Pre/perinatal information and the Child Behavior Checklist were reported by the parent when the child was aged 9–10. Forty variables which are generally known by birth were input as potential predictors including maternal substance-use, obstetric complications and child demographics. Elastic net regression with 5-fold validation was performed, and subsequently stratified by sex, race/ethnicity, household income and parental psychopathology. Seventeen pre/perinatal variables were identified as robust predictors of ADHD symptoms in this cohort. The model explained just 8.13% of the variance in ADHD symptoms on average (95% CI = 5.6%–11.5%). Predictive accuracy of the model varied significantly by subgroup, particularly across income groups, and several pre/perinatal factors appeared to be sex-specific. Results suggest we may be able to predict childhood ADHD symptoms with modest accuracy from birth. This study needs to be replicated using prospectively measured pre/perinatal data.

Type
Regular Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Adverse prenatal and perinatal experiences are linked with neurodevelopmental disorders such as attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). For instance, increased risk of ADHD has been associated with: maternal alcohol, smoking and drug-use in pregnancy (He et al., Reference He, Chen, Zhu, Hua and Ke2020; Lees et al., Reference Lees, Mewton, Jacobus, Valadez, Stapinski, Teesson, Tapert and Squeglia2020; Paul et al., Reference Paul, Hatoum, Fine, Johnson, Hansen, Karcher, Moreau, Bondy, Qu, Carter, Rogers, Agrawal, Barch and Bogdan2021), pregnancy complications such as anemia, genitourinary infections and pre-eclampsia (Mann & McDermott, Reference Mann and McDermott2011; Wiegersma et al., Reference Wiegersma, Dalman, Lee, Karlsson and Gardner2019), preterm birth, and low birth weight (Franz et al., Reference Franz, Bolat, Bolat, Matijasevich, Santos, Silveira, Procianoy, Rohde and Moreira-Maia2018; Momany et al., Reference Momany, Kamradt and Nikolas2018). Biological mechanisms suggested to explain these links have included maternal hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis dysregulation, excess glucocorticoids, inflammation, and insufficient oxygen and blood supply to the fetus, which can alter fetal gene expression (Kim et al., Reference Kim, Bale and Epperson2015; Smith et al., Reference Smith, Schmidt-Kastner, McGeary, Kaczorowski and Knopik2016).

The causal order of such pre/perinatal risks is difficult to establish, as many co-occur (e.g., maternal smoking & low birth weight). Furthermore, both pre/perinatal adversities and child mental health are confounded by socioeconomic and demographic context. This paper therefore focuses on the pragmatic predictiveness of information known at birth, rather than their causal order. We include various types of obstetric complication and substances used in pregnancy, but also basic demographic information known at birth such as parental age, sex, race/ethnicity of the child, and presense of a multiple birth. The primary aims of this study were to assess which of these factors best predicted ADHD-linked symptoms, and how well they can predict such symptoms, in a large cohort of 9- to 10-year-old children from the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development study (ABCD; United States).

A secondary exploratory aim was to assess whether accuracy of our predictive model varied for different subgroups in the population. The United States is a demographically-heterogenous nation, and prediction of ADHD symptoms from birth may be more accurate (and useful) for specific groups within this population. Socioeconomics, race/ethnicity or family psychiatric history may confound the relationship between prenatal adversity and ADHD risk, or may moderate the effects of prenatal adversity on ADHD risk. For instance, both ADHD and pre/perinatal complications are associated with lower socioeconomic status (Finch, Reference Finch2003; Madden, Reference Madden2014; Martinson & Reichman, Reference Martinson and Reichman2016; Russell et al., Reference Russell, Ford, Williams and Russell2016). Regarding race/ethnicity, Black women in the United States are at elevated risk of giving birth to preterm and low birth weight children compared to white women (Catov et al., Reference Catov, Lee, Roberts, Xu and Simhan2015; Giscombé & Lobel, Reference Giscombé and Lobel2005). The latter-cited studies suggest maternal health issues such as gestational hypertension or diabetes, and stress related to experiences of racism, may explain these race/ethnicity-based discrepancies. Prenatal effects on ADHD risk may also be moderated by genetic (Brinksma et al., Reference Brinksma, Hoekstra, van den Hoofdakker, de Bildt, Buitelaar, Hartman and Dietrich2017; O'Donnell et al., Reference O'Donnell, Glover, Lahti, Lahti, Edgar, Raikkonen and O'Connor2017) or familial predisposition (Clarke et al., Reference Clarke, Tanskanen, Huttunen, Whittaker and Cannon2009). For example, Brinksma et al. (Reference Brinksma, Hoekstra, van den Hoofdakker, de Bildt, Buitelaar, Hartman and Dietrich2017) found that low birth weight was more strongly associated with ADHD symptoms in children with a low activity (vs high activity) MAOA genotype. They also found that both pregnancy/delivery complications and maternal smoking interacted with 5-HTTLPR genotype to influence ADHD symptom severity. By predicting ADHD-like symptoms in groups stratified by income (e.g., low, middle, high), race/ethnicity, and family psychiatric history, we may be able to identify for whom the link between pre/perinatal factors and ADHD is most relevant.

We also wished to explore whether predictive capacity of a pre/perinatal model of ADHD risk varied by sex. Several reviews suggest males are more susceptible than females, to poor health outcomes following the same pre/perinatal adversities (DiPietro & Voegtline, Reference DiPietro and Voegtline2017; Inkster et al., Reference Inkster, Fernandez-Boyano and Robinson2021). Two studies have shown that the effects of low birth weight on attention problems is moderated by sex such that the association is stronger in males (Dooley et al., Reference Dooley, Clarke, Cotter and Cannon2022; Momany et al., Reference Momany, Kamradt, Ullsperger, Elmore, Nigg and Nikolas2017). While not formally testing moderation-by-sex, others have also found that the association between birth weight and ADHD symptoms scales is stronger in males compared to females (McNicholas et al., Reference McNicholas, Healy, White, Sheridan-Pereira, O’Connor, Coakley and Dooley2016; Martel et al., Reference Martel, Lucia, Nigg and Breslau2007). It is currently not well-understood why a “male vulnerability” might exist prenatally. Possible explanations include: a slower maturation of fetal organ systems in males; the foreign Y chromosome of males leading to greater placental immune response in the mother; and a greater prioritization of fetal growth over other repair functions in males (DiPietro & Voegtline, Reference DiPietro and Voegtline2017). There is also a well-known sex difference (male > female) in the prevalence of ADHD diagnosis and symptoms (Murray et al., Reference Murray, Booth, Eisner, Auyeung, Murray and Ribeaud2019; Willcutt, Reference Willcutt2012). A pre/perinatal model of ADHD risk could have greater predictive capacity, and therefore be more useful, for males compared to females.

One of the challenges with pre/perinatal prediction models is that inputs are highly correlated (e.g., prenatal smoking and lower birth weight). Two main methodological approaches have been used to overcome this collinearity issue. The first is to observe the bivariate association between each perinatal predictor and ADHD outcome, and to include only significant predictors in a multivariate prediction model (Getahun et al., Reference Getahun, Rhoads, Demissie, Lu, Quinn, Fassett, Wing and Jacobsen2013; Schwenke et al., Reference Schwenke, Fasching, Faschingbauer, Pretscher, Kehl, Peretz, Keller, Haberle, Eichler, Irlbauer-Muller, Dammer, Beckmann and Schneider2018; Sciberras et al., Reference Sciberras, Ukoumunne and Efron2011; Silva et al., Reference Silva, Colvin, Hagemann and Bower2014; Willoughby et al., Reference Willoughby, Williams, Mills-Koonce and Blair2020). However, this does not capture inter-dependencies of multiple exposures, and can ignore important confounds (e.g., sex). A second popular approach is to perform dimension reduction, and to use the resultant set of latent factors to predict childhood ADHD (Milberger et al., Reference Milberger, Biederman, Faraone, Guite and Tsuang1997; Wiggs et al., Reference Wiggs, Elmore, Nigg and Nikolas2016). While this acknowledges the correlated structure among predictors, it can be difficult to compare results across studies and to translate units of a latent factor to real-life measures. Similarly, a sum total of risks can be made to capture “cumulative prenatal adversity.” Roffman et al. (Reference Roffman, Sipahi, Dowling, Hughes, Hopkinson, Lee, Eryilmaz, Cohen, Gilman, Doyle and Dunn2021) did so in the ABCD cohort and found that the number of prenatal adversities (0–8) was significantly and linearly associated with Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) attention problems. Two or more prenatal adversities was associated with an 86% increased odds of high attention problems (T ≥ 60) compared with children with no prenatal adversity. However, the study did not report which prenatal factors were the best predictors of attention problems, and their solution was not internally validated. Elastic net regression has not yet been used to predict ADHD from pre/perinatal factors. This penalized regression algorithm can predict an outcome from a large number of predictors, and avoids overfitting by tuning penalty terms which reduce the number of predictors in the model. Elastic nets also tend to keep correlated predictors together, retaining or eliminating them as a group (Zou & Hastie, Reference Zou and Hastie2005) which may help to highlight broad groups of risk factors for intervention.

Improving prediction of ADHD is important for two reasons. First, given the strong genetic basis in ADHD, early identification of risk may be the best way to prevent or minimize symptoms (Fusar-Poli et al., Reference Fusar-Poli, Bauer, Borgwardt, Bechdolf, Correll, Do, Domschke, Galderisi, Kessing, Koutsouleris, Krebs, Lennox, McGuire, Meyer-Lindenberg, Millan, Nieman, Pfennig, Sand, Whenert and Arango2019). Secondly, ADHD in childhood has long-lasting consequences on other areas of mental health and wellbeing throughout adolescent and adult life (Yoshimasu et al., Reference Yoshimasu, Barbaresi, Colligan, Voigt, Killian, Weaver and Katusic2012; Agnew-Blais et al., Reference Agnew-Blais, Polanczyk, Danese, Wertz, Moffitt and Arseneault2018). Early intervention may be efficient in minimizing the “snowballing” of issues and the development of complex cases. To date, studies that have used pre/perinatal data to predict variance in childhood ADHD symptoms have shown relatively low predictive capacity. For instance, Smidts & Oosterlaan, Reference Smidts and Oosterlaan2007 found that prenatal factors (maternal age, disease, smoking or alcohol during pregnancy) explained a further 1%–4% of the variance in ADHD symptoms at 3–6 years, after demographic, socioeconomic, and parent psychiatric factors were accounted for. O'Donnell et al. (Reference O'Donnell, Glover, Lahti, Lahti, Edgar, Raikkonen and O'Connor2017) explained 4%–10% of the variance in ADHD symptoms (age 4–15) using information known at birth (sex, birth weight, gestational age, maternal age, maternal perinatal anxiety, smoking and alcohol use), but also included family demographics, COMT genotype, and toddler parenting styles. In this study, we wanted to quantify the variance in childhood ADHD symptoms explained solely by information available at the birth. Further, those previous studies pre-selected a small number of pre/perinatal variables (<15) to avoid overfitting their standard linear regressions. Elastic net regression provides a more data-driven approach to variable selection and model fitting, deciding among many potential predictors, which are most relevant to the outcome.

Other models designed to predict ADHD diagnosis are either reliant on magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) data, or are not relevant to children. MRIs remain expensive and non-routine in child psychiatry assessment, and the current MRI-based classifiers are only capable of concurrently identifying ADHD children that have already been clinical diagnosed (e.g., Sen et al., Reference Sen, Borle, Greiner and Brown2018). One prospective risk prediction model for adult ADHD has shown good predictive accuracy (Caye et al., Reference Caye, Agnew-Blais, Arseneault, Goncalves, Kieling, Langley, Menezes, Moffitt, Passos, Rocha, Sibley, Swanson, Thapar, Wehrmeister and Rohde2019), however, childhood ADHD symptoms were used as a predictor in this model and the continuity of ADHD symptoms from childhood to adulthood is well-known (Biederman et al., Reference Biederman, Monuteaux, Mick, Spencer, Wilens, Silva, Snyder and Faraone2006). It may be more clinically useful to be able to predict ADHD at a younger age when interventions are most effective (Ornoy & Spivak, Reference Ornoy and Spivak2019; Sampaio et al., Reference Sampaio, Feldman, Lavelle and Skokauskas2021).

While all pre/perinatal data were collected retrospectively in this study (age 9–10), all “pre/perinatal” variables are typically available by birth. Our study therefore investigates which pre/perinatal variables are most relevant to childhood ADHD symptoms, how well we can predict ADHD symptoms using such data, and how that predictive accuracy varies across subgroups within the population.

Method

Participants

The Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) study is a large cohort study of children aged 9–10 from the United States (https://abcdstudy.org). The baseline data contains rich information on pregnancy and delivery, albeit retrospective, as well as detailed measures on the mental health of participants and their parents. Participating children were born between 2007 and 2009, making ABCD one of the most recently born large-scale child cohorts. The importance of choosing a relatively recently born cohort is underscored by significant changes over the past two decades in: rates of neonatal mortality and mortality (Goldenberg et al., Reference Goldenberg, Culhane, Iams and Romero2008; Hug et al., Reference Hug, Alexander, You and Alkema2019), maternal behavior during pregnancy (Cnattingius, Reference Cnattingius2004), and the prevalence of ADHD (Boyle et al., Reference Boyle, Boulet, Schieve, Cohen, Blumberg, Yeargin-Allsopp, Visser and Kogan2011).

Exclusion criteria imposed by ABCD researchers included contraindications to MRI, non-fluency in English, history of major neurological disorders, traumatic brain injury, extreme prematurity (<28 weeks gestation), and diagnoses of schizophrenia, moderate to severe autism spectrum disorder, intellectual disability, or substance-use disorder. In cases of sibling participants, the eldest was retained in the sample. All remaining subjects with outcome data were included in the analysis (N = 9,975).

The primary caregiver was the biological mother in 85% of cases. The remaining caregivers were biological fathers (10%), adoptive parents (2.5%), custodial parents (1%) and “other” (1.5%). A sensitivity analysis tested whether using biological mother reports only affected findings.

The 22 geographic locations that comprise the ABCD research sites are nationally distributed and were chosen in an attempt to capture the range of demographic and socioeconomic diversity of the United States. Within study sites, consenting parents and assenting children were primarily recruited through a probability sample of schools and summer camp programs and community volunteers.

The University of California at San Diego (San Diego, CA, USA) Institutional Review Board was responsible for the ethical oversight of the ABCD study. The secondary analysis of the data was approved by the Research Ethics Committee for the Royal College of Surgeons in Ireland.

Measures

Outcome: CBCL attention problems

The attention problems sub-scale of the CBCL was used to capture ADHD symptoms (Achenbach & Rescorla, Reference Achenbach and Rescorla2001). This parent-rated questionnaire contains 119 items in total, 10 of which are summed to form an attention problems score. Items cover behaviors such as inattention, hyperactivity and impulsivity (item list in Supplementary Material). Items are rated on a 3-point Likert scale (0 = not at all true; 1 = somewhat true; 2 = very true). For most analyses, we treat CBCL attention problems as a continuous scale (0–20) and use the R 2 statistic to capture predictive capacity of the model. However, we also predicted “likely ADHD diagnosis,” defined as a score of 9 or above on this scale (equivalent to T-score of 64 in males; 66 in females). A cutoff of 9/20 was used based on Lampert et al. (Reference Lampert, Polanczyk, Tramontina, Mardini and Rohde2004) who found this was the optimum cutoff for predicting ADHD in a mixed group of clinic-referred and general community children. A meta-analysis showed that this CBCL scale, with cutoffs around T = 65, can discriminate ADHD cases and controls with good accuracy (Chang et al., Reference Chang, Wang and Tsai2016; pooled sensitivity & specificity = 77% & 73%). The attention problems score shows good reliability as a general factor of its 10 constituent items in this sample (omega total = 0.86).

Prenatal and perinatal predictors

Any variable measured by the ABCD study that referred to the pre/perinatal period, or that would typically be known by birth (e.g., child’s sex), was included as a potential predictor to the model. All variables were reported retrospectively by the primary caregiver in the cohort’s Developmental History Questionnaire (list of variables available online: https://nda.nih.gov/data_structure.html?short_name=dhx01). Maternal retrospective reporting on pre/perinatal factors has been shown to be generally valid: Liu et al. (Reference Liu, Tuvblad, Li, Raine and Baker2013) found that maternal recall of most prenatal variables showed “substantial” to “perfect” agreement with medical records 8–10 years postpartum (κ = 0.60–1.00). Notable exceptions included substance-use when mother provide continuous data (e.g., number of cigarettes per day) and certain medical problems during pregnancy (proteinuria, nausea & vomiting; κ ≤ 0.40). We therefore used dichotomous substance-use measurers (yes/no). Rice et al. (Reference Rice, Lewis, Harold, van den Bree, Boivin, Hay, Owen and Thapar2007) found maternal recall of birth weight, Neonatal Intensive Care Unit admission, delivery method, and gestational hypertension 4–9 years postpartum to show very good consistency with medical records. Finally, Ramos et al. (Reference Ramos, Marceau, Neiderhiser, De Araujo-Greecher, Natsuaki and Leve2020) found that maternal reports of substance-use and pregnancy complications at 9 months and 8 years postpartum were mostly consistent with one another (65%–98% agreement).

Birth information

Birth weight was reported by the parent in pounds and ounces, which was converted to kilograms. Some cases (N = 683) reported in ounces only and were removed for being too small likely mis-entered. One individual was removed from the analysis due to improbable birth weight for their gestational age (6 weeks early at 6.7 KG). Prematurity referred to the number of weeks prior to gestational week 40. Parental ages at birth (mother and father) was simplified into “years under 20” and “years over 35” given specific risks to the child associated with these cutoffs (Chang et al., Reference Chang, Lichtenstein, D’Onofrio, Almqvist, Kuja-Halkola, Sjölander and Larsson2014; Cleary-Goldman et al., Reference Cleary-Goldman, Malone, Vidaver, Ball, Nyberg, Comstock, Saade, Eddleman, Klugman, Dugoff, Timor-Tritsch, Craigo, Carr, Wolfe, Bianchi and D'Alton2005).

Pregnancy and delivery complications

Binary data on 13 pregnancy complications was available (see Table 1). “Persistent proteinuria” was merged with “pre-eclampsia, eclampsia or toxemia” due to low prevalence (n = 47). Binary data on eight delivery complications was available (Table 1). Two summary variables, total pregnancy complications and total delivery complications were created. Low rates of gestational rubella (n = 12), convulsions at birth (n = 13) and blood transfusion at birth (n = 37) were observed so these were excluded from analysis, though counted within sum totals. Caesarian section delivery and days spent in incubator were also included.

Table 1. Descriptive statistics of all pre/perinatal variables and selection frequency of each in full-sample prediction of age 9 ADHD symptoms

a Frequencies (%) for categorical/binary variables; means (M), standard deviations (SD) and ranges (min-max) for continuous variables.

b Proportion of models in which the variable received a non-zero coefficient. Selection frequency over 95% was considered robust.

Maternal substance-use in pregnancy

Maternal smoking, alcohol use and drug-use referred to use of these substances at any point during pregnancy and at any frequency. Prevalence was low (<1%) for maternal use of oxycontin (n = 32), heroin/morphine (n = 20), cocaine/crack (n = 68) and “other” drugs (n = 91) so these were merged into an “other” drug class (n = 182) distinguishing them from marijuana/cannabis. Use of prenatal vitamin supplements was also included.

Subgroups

Child’s race/ethnicity was captured by 5 groups: White, Black, Hispanic, Asian or Other, however stratified prediction was only performed for White, Black and Hispanic subgroups due to small sample sizes for Asian and Other (Table 1). Household income referred to total annual income from all sources before tax/deductions. Low income was defined as below $50,000, middle income as $50,000–99,000 and high income as over $100,000 (corresponding to tertile split points). Familial liability for ADHD referred to (a) parental ADHD symptoms and (b) parental psychiatric history. Parent ADHD symptoms were captured by the Adult Self-Report attention problems scale (Achenbach & Verhulst, Reference Achenbach and Verhulst2010), as completed by the primary caregiver. Low, moderate and high scorers on this scale were defined as scoring <50th percentile, 50th–80th percentile, or >80th percentile respectively. Parental psychiatric history was captured by the number of lifetime mental health issues (from a list of 10) among biological parents, and was reported by the primary caregiver. Parental psychiatric history was defined as none (0 issues), average (1 or 2 issues) and strong (3+ issues), with the middle category based on the average number of issues reported (median = 1; mean = 1.7). Further information on these grouping variables is available in supplementary material.

Statistics

R code for all data processing and analysis is available on http://rpubs.com/dooleyr/.

Elastic net

Elastic net regression was chosen for several reasons. First, we have a large number of potential predictors (40), and we wish to produce the most parsimonious model possible. Second, many of our pre/perinatal factors are correlated (various pregnancy complications, prenatal smoking, etc.), which elastic net is well designed for (Zou & Hastie, Reference Zou and Hastie2005). Finally, we trained the elastic net within one section of the data, and validated/tested it in another (5-fold validation), thus ensuring that the subset of predictors chosen by the elastic net are generalizable to other samples, and not just that which it was trained on. We used the glmnet package in R (cv.glmnet function), which uses a quadratic approximation to the log-likelihood and then a cyclical coordinate descent algorithm (Friedman et al., Reference Friedman, Hastie and Tibshirani2010).

Simply put, the elastic net is a standard linear regression with additional limits on the number of predictors and the size of beta coefficients. These degree of coefficient shrinkage is decided interactively by two penalty terms “alpha” (value between 0 and 1) and “lambda” (value > 0). When alpha = 0, ridge regression is run, which retains all predictors in the final model albeit with shrunk coefficients, and when alpha = 1, Least Absolute Shrinkage and Selection Operator (LASSO) regression is run which shrinks many coefficients to zero thus retaining only a few predictors in the model. In elastic net regression, we try multiple values for alpha between 0 and 1 and choose the one which provides the best predictive accuracy. Both LASSO and ridge approaches can be employed: shrinkage of some coefficients while setting others to 0. The lambda value determines the extent of shrinkage to each coefficient. Given some values for alpha and lambda values, elastic net produces a regression equation with a subset of predictors, their B coefficients, and an R 2 reflecting how well predicted outcome matched actual outcome.

Five-fold cross-validation was used to avoid overfitting. Five folds were chosen to provide ∼1000 individuals for validation and ∼1000 for testing in the primary analysis, samples which would likely contain a wide distribution of attention problems. Training-validation-test split was 80%–10%–10%. The optimal lambda value was found for 20 different alphas in the training set, we used the validation set to identify the alpha-lambda combination with the smallest mean absolute error using grid search, and finally we tested predictive performance on the test set. Mean test set R 2 and the number of times each predictor had a non-zero B coefficient (“selection frequency”) were calculated, as were the 95% confidence intervals for these metrics. This process was repeated for a different test set (n = 998), until all participants had been in the test set once. This 5-fold validation process in turn was repeated 20 times, leading to 100 test set results (i.e., 100 R 2 estimates; 100 versions of predictor coefficients). Figure S1 describes the 5-fold cross-validation process in greater detail. Predictors with a selection frequency of at least 95% (95/100 runs) were defined as robust.

We use the terms risk vs protective factors to indicate positive Vs negative association with attention problems. However, we note that these associations bear no causal direction.

Sampling weights were not used in this study given the limitations of our elastic net algorithm to use weights when predicting unseen data (i.e., validation and test sets).

Models

First, we used all available pre/perinatal information (40 variables; Table 1) to predict CBCL attention problems in the full sample.

Second, the prediction model was stratified by sex (male/female), race/ethnicity (White/Black/Hispanic), family income (high/mid/low), parent attention problems (high/mid/low) and parent psychiatric history (high/mid/none). Predictive accuracy, measured by test set R 2, was compared across subgroups visually, with 1-way analysis of variances or Welch’s t-tests where appropriate. One hundred test set R2 values were available for each analysis (e.g., 100 R 2s for males and 100 for females), leading to difference tests between groups with 100 observations each. A more conservative p-threshold of 0.01 was used to interpret subgroup results given that the sample was stratified five times (Bonferroni correction = 0.05/5 = 0.01).

We opted to conduct stratified prediction rather than including interactions with each contextual variable, because it allowed us to train and optimize the elastic net to each subgroup. As such, models were tailored to each subgroup rather than applying a “one-model-fits-all” approach. Group-specific results are also be easier to interpret and may be more translatable to community-specific interventions.

Three sensitivity analyses were performed: (a) to predict binary CBCL attention problem score ≥ 9; (b) limiting the sample to cases where the respondent was the biological mother (n = 8,495); and (3) limiting the sample to cases with no missing data (n = 7,429). For (1), we used an elastic net with a binary logistic outcome and under-sampled those with CBCL scores < 9 (i.e., the majority class) such that the model could train on a balanced data set (N cases ∼ N controls ∼ 100). That is, the model is designed to discriminate cases from controls, given approximately 50:50 prevalence. Randomly under-sampling from the majority class is a way to avoid models becoming biased and resulting in a high proportion of false negatives (Mohammed et al., Reference Mohammed, Rawashdeh and Abdullah2020).

Imputation

Any predictor with ≥10% missing values was excluded. Validation and test set missing values were replaced with the training set mean to maintain complete independence of data. Mean imputation has been shown to perform similarly to multiple imputation approaches when the proportion of missingness is <10% (Shrive et al., Reference Shrive, Stuart, Quan and Ghali2006; Waljee et al., Reference Waljee, Mukherjee, Singal, Zhang, Warren, Balis, Marrero, Zhu and Higgins2013). Furthermore, few variables had rates of missingness above 5% (maternal medication and alcohol use, infant incubator-use, paternal age; Table S9). A sensitivity analysis (see (3) above) with complete observations only tested whether imputation affected results.

Results

The pre/perinatal model, including sex and race/ethnicity, explained approximately 8% of the variance in CBCL attention problems at age 9 (Mean R 2 = 8.13%; 95% CI = 5.61%–11.47%). Robust risk factors included: male sex, maternal illicit drug-use and smoking, total number of pregnancy and delivery complications, urinary tract infection (UTI), anemia and “other” pregnancy complication, medication-use, younger parental ages, Black and “other” race/ethnicity (Figure 1). No specific type of delivery complication was robustly linked with attention problems, nor was C-section, birth weight or gestational age. Asian children had lower attention problem scores compared to White children, and non-singletons (e.g., twins) had lower scores compared to singleton-born children, controlling for all other factors.

Figure 1. Robust pre/perinatal predictors of ADHD symptoms, in order of strength (mean B coefficient) in the full sample. Error bars indicate 95% confidence intervals (N = 9,975).

Selection frequencies for all variables are shown in Table 1 while the mean coefficients for robust predictors are shown in Figure 1.

Note, averaged B coefficients in Figure 1 represent the average change on the CBCL attention problem scale (0–20) for each unit change in predictor, holding all other terms constant. There was no one “final” model – results represent the average of 100 runs of the prediction model (Figure S1). Mean values of penalty parameters alpha and lambda were 0.46 and 0.18 respectively (Table S8). Bivariate correlations between all potential predictors showed multi-collinearity, particularly between the different types of obstetric complication, validating the use of elastic net (Figure S6).

Subgroups

Model performance varied significantly across subgroups (Figure 2). Mean R 2 was lower for all subgroups compared to full-sample analysis, with the exception of the low-income group (Table 2).

Figure 2. Subgroup variation in capacity to predict age 9 CBCL attention problems from pre/perinatal factors. R-squared averaged over 100 test sets. Error bars indicate ±1 standard deviation.

Table 2. Group-stratified results

a Intercept refers to group mean on the CBCL attention problem scale when all other predictors are 0 or at the reference level (e.g., mean birth weight, White race, male/female average).

b Number of pre/perinatal predictors with a selection frequency of 95% or more.

c Restricted sample only includes participants with full data on all pre/perinatal variables.

Income

A strong difference in model performance was observed across income groups as determined by 1-way analysis of variance on test set R 2 (F(2,297) = 285.1, p < .001). The pre/perinatal model was most predictive of attention problems in children of low-income homes (mean R 2 = 9.61%), less predictive for middle-income homes (mean R 2 = 4.09%) and performed poorly for children of high-income homes (mean R 2 = 2.90%). Risk factors specific to the low-income setting included maternal anemia, UTI, “other” complication in pregnancy, younger paternal age, maternal smoking and alcohol use. Only three risk factors were identified as robust in high-income settings: male sex, maternal drug-use in pregnancy and total number of pregnancy complications. Asian race/ethnicity was protective in high-income families, while Hispanic race/ethnicity was protective in low-income families (Table S2).

Race/ethnicity

Model R2 differed significantly across racial/ethnic groups (F(2,297) = 36.93, p < .001). The pre/perinatal prediction model performed slightly better for Black children (mean R2 = 7.10%,), compared to White children (mean R 2 = 6.43%), though this difference was not significant (t(127.68) = −1.39, p = 0.17). Mean R 2 values were significantly higher for White and Black children compared to Hispanic children (mean R 2 = 3.61%; Figure 2; Table 2). Confidence intervals for R2 included zero in Hispanic (95% CI = −1.85% to 6.94%) and Black children (95% CI = −2.90% to 14.22%), suggesting that pre/perinatal did not predict attention problems reliably in these groups. Low model precision was also evident from wide CIs in the sample of Black children (95% CI = −2.90% to 14.22%), particularly compared to White children (95% CI = 3.06%–9.69%). Robust risk factors common to all race/ethnicity groups were: male sex, pregnancy complications and birth complications. Robust risk factors specific to White children included UTI and severe nausea in pregnancy, while those specific to Black children included exposure to gestational anemia and diabetes. Maternal marijuana-use was a robust risk factor for attention problems in Hispanic children only (Table S3).

Familial liability to ADHD

R 2 varied significantly across the different levels of parental psychiatric history (F(2,297) = 63.86, p < .001) and parent ADHD symptoms (F(2,297) = 15.6, p < .001). Variance explained by the model was low for parents with no psychiatric history (mean R 2 = 2.88%), higher for parents with a history of 1/2 issues (mean R 2 = 4.96%) and higher again for parents with a strong history, that is, 3+ issues (mean R 2 = 5.98%). Similarly, pre/perinatal factors predicted attention problems significantly better in children whose parents had high attention problems themselves (mean R 2 = 5.32%) compared to those whose parents scored in the moderate range (mean R 2 = 4.30%) or the low range (mean R 2 = 3.75%; Figure 2; Table 2). Black race/ethnicity robustly predicted attention problems only in children whose parents had a strong psychiatric history or who scored in the moderate ADHD symptom range (Tables S4–5). Gestational anemia was a risk factor for attention problems specific to children whose parent had high ADHD scores (Table S5).

Sex

Pre/perinatal variables explained more slightly more variance in attention problems in males (R 2 = 5.28%) compared to females (R 2 = 4.82%; Table 2) however this difference was not significant at the corrected threshold of 0.01 (F(1,198) = 4.07, p = 0.045). More predictors were identified as robust for males versus females (12 vs. 8). Common risk factors among males and females were maternal drug-use and smoking during pregnancy, total number of pregnancy complications, gestational UTIs, severe nausea and younger maternal age. Common protective factors included being Asian and a non-singleton birth (Table 3).

Table 3. Sex-stratified predictors of age 9 ADHD symptoms

a UTI: Urinary Tract Infection.

b Race/ethnicity not defined as White, Black, Hispanic or Asian.

Any variable with a selection frequency ≥95% for any income bracket was included (indicated in bold).

Pre/perinatal predictors of attention problems stronger in males than females (according to selection frequency) included total number of delivery complications, maternal prescription medication-use, gestational anemia and “other” race/ethnicity. There were no female-specific risk factors however gestational diabetes was a protective factor not identified for males (Table 3).

Sensitivity analyses

Additional analyses tested (a) the clinical relevance of a pre/perinatal model, and whether findings were biased by (b) inaccurate retrospective reporting from non-maternal caregivers or (c) by imputation.

A balanced logistic elastic net, predicting high CBCL attention problems (≥9), identified a model with five predictors which had an average test set Area Under the [receiver operating characteristic] Curve (AUC) of 67.4% (95% CI = 65.4%–69.6%; sensitivity = 60%, specificity = 63%). The average positive predictive value of this model was 63% (95% CI = 52%–76%) and the average negative predictive value was 61% (95% CI = 50%–71%). Note that controls were under-sampled to match numbers of cases for this analysis (50:50 prevalence). The 5 robust predictors were: male sex (OR = 1.49), maternal drug-use (OR = 1.30), smoking (OR = 1.28), total pregnancy complications (OR = 1.08) and total delivery complications (OR = 1.07).

Variance in attention problems explained by the model did not change significantly (t(196.67) = 0.19, p = .85) when the data was limited to that reported by the biological mother (85% of cases; N = 8,495). R 2 dropped slightly from 8.13% (95% CI: 5.61%–11.47%) to 8.09% (95% CI: 5.71%–10.47%). The same predictors identified in the full sample were also identified in this biological-mother-only model, with the exception of: Black race/ethnicity and use of non-cannabis drug (Figure S5).

Variance explained in attention problems in the full sample dropped from 8.13% (95% CI: 5.61%–11.47%) to 6.72% (95% CI: 4.19%–9.43%) when limiting the sample to participants with full data (N = 7,429) which was a significant difference in R 2 (t(196.58) = 6.94, p < .001). The 11 robust predictors identified in the restricted analysis were: non-singleton birth (protective), male sex, total pregnancy and delivery complications, gestational nausea, UTI and anemia, younger maternal age, and maternal smoking, drug-use and prescription medication-use (Figure S4). Robust predictors chosen in the imputed data but not the restricted data, and therefore possibly influenced by imputation, were: Asian, Black and other race/ethnicity, maternal use of non-cannabis drug, “other” pregnancy complication and younger paternal age (Figures 1 & S4).

Discussion

Approximately 8% of the variance in age 9 attention problems could be explained by information generally known at birth such as sex, parental age, and prenatal exposures. However, there was considerable heterogeneity in the degree to which the model predicted attention problems across groups in the population. Pre/perinatal variables were more predictive of attention problems in low-income households compared to high- or middle-income households, more predictive in families with a stronger parental psychiatric history, more reliable for White children compared to Black or Hispanic children (Figure 2). Predictive capacity (R 2) did not differ significantly between males and females, however several male-specific risk factors were identified including maternal medication-use in pregnancy and total number of delivery complications (Table 3).

Comparison with other studies

Few other studies have reported R2 statistics from pre/perinatal models predicting continuously measured ADHD symptoms. A study in the ALSPAC cohort (n = 6,969), explained 9.4% of the variance in ADHD symptoms at age 4, and 4.6% of the variance in symptoms at age 15, however they also included genotype and postnatal data such as parenting styles (O'Donnell et al., Reference O'Donnell, Glover, Lahti, Lahti, Edgar, Raikkonen and O'Connor2017). In another study, pre/perinatal factors accounted for 3.7% of the variance in impulsivity at 3–6 years, 1.5% of the variance in hyperactivity and <1% of the variance in inattention (Smidts & Oosterlaan, Reference Smidts and Oosterlaan2007). We explained 8% of the variance in ADHD symptoms using only information typically known at the birth. Additively or multiplicatively combining such a model with postnatal variables such as early life adversity and parenting styles, may improve prediction of childhood ADHD further (Huhdanpaa et al., Reference Huhdanpaa, Morales-Munoz, Aronen, Polkki, Saarenpaa-Heikkila, Kylliainen and Paavonen2021; Willoughby et al., Reference Willoughby, Williams, Mills-Koonce and Blair2020).

We found that a pre/perinatal model could discriminate those with clinically relevant attention problems (scores of 9 or above) better than chance, but with low accuracy (average AUC = 67.4%). Despite many studies using pre/perinatal variables to predict ADHD in childhood (probable or diagnosed), none to our knowledge report accuracy statistics on an unseen data (i.e., internal validation). One exception is a prediction model in very preterm/low birth weight children, which can predict ADHD with good accuracy (AUC = 81%; Franz et al., Reference Franz, Caye, Lacerda, Wagner, Silveira, Procianoy, Moreira-Maia and Rohde2022). This is a useful model for preterm/low birth weight children, however it cannot capture the majority of children who develop ADHD (not born very preterm/low birth weight). The model described in this study is not intended for clinical prediction, as it is based on cross-sectional data and has not been externally validated. However, it does provide an up-to-date estimate of the capacity of pre/perinatal information to predict childhood ADHD symptoms, and an overview of how that capacity may change across demographic groups (Figure 2). There remains a need to improve clinical risk prediction of ADHD in general population samples.

Pre/perinatal predictors of interest

Seventeen out of a possible 40 pre/perinatal variables were identified as robust predictors of CBCL attention problems.

Non-prescription drug-use was associated with a ∼1-point (∼5%), increase on the ADHD symptom scale and smoking was associated with a 0.69 point (3.45%) increase in attention problems controlling for all other variable. The association between maternal smoking and ADHD in the child appears to be strongly confounded by familial factors and may not be causal (Rice et al., Reference Rice, Langley, Woodford, Davey Smith and Thapar2018; Thapar et al., Reference Thapar, Rice, Hay, Boivin, Langley, van den Bree, Rutter and Harold2009). The same concerns may extend to drug-use (Pingault et al., Reference Pingault, Barkhuizen, Wang, Hannigan, Eilertsen, Corfield, Andreassen, Ask, Tesli, Askeland, Davey Smith, Stoltenberg, Davies, Reichborn-Kjennerud, Ystrom and Havdahl2022). However, the issue of causality does not necessarily invalidate the predictiveness of these variables. Unlike another investigation of the ABCD cohort (Paul et al., Reference Paul, Hatoum, Fine, Johnson, Hansen, Karcher, Moreau, Bondy, Qu, Carter, Rogers, Agrawal, Barch and Bogdan2021), our study did not identify maternal cannabis-use as a risk factor for attention problems (Table 1), though cannabis-use did predict attention problems in Hispanic children (Table S3). Our findings suggest non-cannabis drugs drove the association between maternal drug-use and attention problems. While speculative, it may be relevant that the years in which ABCD participants were born (2005–2009) overlaps with a time of substantial increased rates of overdose and mortality due to prescription opioids in the United States (Centers for Disease Control & Prevention, 2011).

The observation that total number of complications in pregnancy, use of any medications, and nausea were robust predictors of attention problems supports the theory that common factors among infections and illnesses (e.g., elevated cytokines, fever) underlie the association between obstetric complications and neurodevelopmental issues (Flinkkilä et al., Reference Flinkkilä, Keski-Rahkonen, Marttunen and Raevuori2016). On the other hand, UTIs and anemia in pregnancy also predicted ADHD risk independently of “total complications,” nausea and medication-use. We found UTI during pregnancy was linked with a 2.8% increase (0.56 points) on the CBCL attention problems scale, and anemia was associated with a 1.75% increase (0.35 points). Another large US study found that maternal genitourinary infection during pregnancy was associated with a 29% increased odds of ADHD diagnosis in the child aged 8–9 (N = 84,721; Mann & McDermott, Reference Mann and McDermott2011). As UTI and anemia are preventable in many cases, results support a reappraisal and improvement of screening for asymptomatic bacteriuria and iron deficiencies during pregnancy. Subgroup analysis shows that anemia during pregnancy was a particularly strong risk factor in children who were male, Black, from low-income homes, and whose parents had high attention problems (Tables S2–6), suggesting strategies to prevent gestational anemia could be further targeted.

Birth weight was not a robust predictor of attention problems when included alongside other pre/perinatal factors. This is noteworthy given the high replicability of this association (Momany et al., Reference Momany, Kamradt and Nikolas2018). The inclusion of pregnancy complications and maternal substance-use may have overshadowed the effects of birth weight on attention problems, though birth weight may still be a useful proxy for prenatal adversity. For instance, low birth weight has been shown to mediate the association between maternal smoking and ADHD (Brannigan et al., Reference Brannigan, Healy, Cannon, Leacy and Clarke2020).

Variance by subgroup

The variance in ADHD symptoms explained in low-income homes was more than three times greater than in the high-income homes (Figure 2). Socioeconomic disparities in adverse birth outcomes and ADHD risk are widely reported (Blumenshine et al., Reference Blumenshine, Egerter, Barclay, Cubbin and Braveman2010; Russell et al., Reference Russell, Ford, Williams and Russell2016). Other have reported that gestational diabetes (Nomura et al., Reference Nomura, Marks, Grossman, Yoon, Loudon, Stone and Halperin2012) and maternal depression (Herba et al., Reference Herba, Glover, Ramchandani and Rondon2016) pose greater risk to the mental health of children from lower socioeconomic strata. We add to these findings, showing a combined pre/perinatal model is more predictive of childhood attention problems in lower income settings. Possible explanations include reduced access to quality healthcare, increased financial stress and the compounded effects of both. The largest jump in prediction accuracy across income groups was from middle to low incomes (Figure 2) suggesting that annual income below $50,000 (“low”) may be a particularly relevant threshold for increased vulnerability to pre/perinatal risks in the United States. Descriptive statistics in Figure S3 shows that UTIs in pregnancy, maternal drug-use, smoking and younger maternal ages at birth were more common in low-income households.

The association between pre/perinatal factors and attention problems also appeared to be moderated by race/ethnicity of the child. A meta-analysis found that the association between birth weight and ADHD symptoms in United States-based studies became stronger as the proportion of Black children in the sample increased (Momany et al., Reference Momany, Kamradt and Nikolas2018). In this study, predictive capacity of the pre/perinatal model did not vary significantly between children of White and Black race/ethnicities (mean R 2 = 6.43 and 7.10 respectively). However, there was large variability in the variance explained in attention problems for Black children (R 2 CI = −2.90% to 14.22%), potentially explained by smaller sample size relative to other racial/ethnic groups (n = 1508; Table 2). The model performed particularly poorly in Hispanic children in terms of both reliability and precision (R 2 = 3.61%; 95% CI = −1.85% to 6.94%) despite reasonable sample size (n = 2,121) and similar average ADHD scores as other groups (Figure S2). Given many Hispanic/LatinX families in the United States have South American roots, it is relevant that South American samples show particularly weak associations between birth weight and ADHD symptoms, compared to samples from North America, Europe and Asia (Momany et al., Reference Momany, Kamradt and Nikolas2018). It is not currently well-understood why this is. Race/ethnicity-linked parental attitudes toward ADHD may influence CBCL scores (Bailey et al., Reference Bailey, Jaquez-Gutierrez and Madhoo2014; Collins & Cleary, Reference Collins and Cleary2016) and observed associations with pre/perinatal variables. Other potential explanations for moderation by race/ethnicity may be unequal access to (or quality of) healthcare, and exposure to race-based discrimination (Bailey et al., Reference Bailey, Jaquez-Gutierrez and Madhoo2014; Rosenthal et al., Reference Rosenthal, Earnshaw, Moore, Ferguson, Lewis, Reid, Lewis, Stasko, Tobin and Ickovics2018).

Prediction of attention problems improved with increasing familial psychopathology (Table 2). Higher levels of parental psychopathology might reflect genetic predisposition and our results may reflect a gene-environment interaction. For instance, Brinksma et al. (Reference Brinksma, Hoekstra, van den Hoofdakker, de Bildt, Buitelaar, Hartman and Dietrich2017) found a gene-environment interaction between 5-HTTLPR genotype (involved in serotonin signaling) and obstetric complications on childhood ADHD symptoms. However, it is also possible that our results reflect an environment-environment interaction or a gene-environment correlation. Parents with mental health issues may have difficulty attending to their child’s needs (Dubber et al., Reference Dubber, Reck, Müller and Gawlik2015), and may be more likely to be substance users pre- and postnatally (Smedberg et al., Reference Smedberg, Lupattelli, Mårdby, Øverland and Nordeng2015). Recent studies have suggested that the effects of environmental factors on ADHD risk may be largely captured by genetic transmission of risk from parents (Agnew-Blais et al., Reference Agnew-Blais, Wertz, Arseneault, Belsky, Danese, Pingault, Polanczyk, Sugden, Williams and Moffitt2022; Pingault et al., Reference Pingault, Barkhuizen, Wang, Hannigan, Eilertsen, Corfield, Andreassen, Ask, Tesli, Askeland, Davey Smith, Stoltenberg, Davies, Reichborn-Kjennerud, Ystrom and Havdahl2022). Future studies should use intergenerational Mendelian randomization to test whether associations between pre/perinatal factors are independent from such genetic processes.

While Figure 2 suggests any psychopathology in parents moderated model accuracy more strongly than parental attention problems, this should be interpreted with caution. Parental psychiatric history was based on the lifetime history of both parents (where available) while parental attention problems was based on the self-reported symptoms of one parent, leading to differences in the variance of each measure.

Being male was the most robust predictor of age 9 attention problems in the full sample and many subgroups (Table S2–5). This is consistent with the well-replicated sex difference in the prevalence of ADHD and many other prediction studies (Getahun et al., Reference Getahun, Rhoads, Demissie, Lu, Quinn, Fassett, Wing and Jacobsen2013; Huhdanpaa et al., Reference Huhdanpaa, Morales-Munoz, Aronen, Polkki, Saarenpaa-Heikkila, Kylliainen and Paavonen2021). The pre/perinatal model was also slightly better at predicting attention problems in males (mean R 2 = 5.28%, SD = 1.51%) compared to females (mean R 2 = 4.82%, SD = 1.72%) supporting male fetal vulnerability theories (DiPietro & Voegtline, Reference DiPietro and Voegtline2017; Inkster et al., Reference Inkster, Fernandez-Boyano and Robinson2021).

Delivery complications, gestational anemia, maternal medication-use and “other” race/ethnicity were more robust predictors of attention problems in males compared to females (Table 3). Consistent with our finding on anemia, Santa-Marina et al. (Reference Santa-Marina, Lertxundi, Andiarena, Irizar, Sunyer, Molinuevo, Llop, Julvez, Beneito, Ibarluzea, Imaz and Ferrin2020) found that the association between maternal iron deficiency in pregnancy and ADHD symptoms was stronger in male compared to female children. This may be explained by sex-specific differences in the effects of perinatal oxygen shortages on dopamine levels in the prefrontal cortex, gene methylation and behavior (Laplante et al., Reference Laplante, Brake, Chehab and Sullivan2012; Wang et al., Reference Wang, Meng, Liu, Fan, Hao, Chen and Du2013). Slow heartbeat at birth specifically had the highest selection frequency of all delivery complications in males (72%; Table S6). Relevantly, females display a more adaptive heart rate increase during labor compared to males (Bernardes et al., Reference Bernardes, Goncalves, Ayres-de-Campos and Rocha2009; DiPietro & Voegtline, Reference DiPietro and Voegtline2017). Medications in pregnancy were also a stronger risk factor for males compared to females. Unfortunately, a standardized labeling procedure was not applied to medications in ABCD, with brand names (e.g., Prozac) and chemical names (e.g., fluoxetine) used interchangeably. Future studies should assess whether specific types of medication increase the risk of ADHD in a sexually dependent manner.

One unexpected finding in sex-stratified models was that gestational diabetes was robustly linked with lower attention problems in females, but not in males (Table 3). It is unknown whether this reflects a sex-moderated effect of diabetes, diabetes treatment, or another unmeasured variable such as maternal weight status. Relevantly, sex differences have been observed in neurodevelopment and cardiometabolic outcomes following gestational diabetes and insulin treatment in experimental animals (Sousa et al., Reference Sousa, Correia, Cruz, Martins, Rodrigues, Gomes, Ambrósio and Baptista2020; Talbot & Dolinsky, Reference Talbot and Dolinsky2019).

Differential vulnerabilities of males and females to certain gestational factors may explain why the effect of birth weight on ADHD symptoms is moderated by sex in some samples (Dooley et al., Reference Dooley, Clarke, Cotter and Cannon2022; Momany et al., Reference Momany, Kamradt, Ullsperger, Elmore, Nigg and Nikolas2017), but not others (Lim et al., Reference Lim, Liu, Schoeler, Cecil, Barker, Viding, Greven and Pingault2018; Pettersson et al., Reference Pettersson, Larsson, D'Onofrio, Almqvist and Lichtenstein2019).

Protective factors

Children of Asian background had lower attention problems compared to all other race/ethnicities (Figure S2; Figure 1). Our results are consistent with other studies suggesting Asian American children are significantly less likely than White American children to receive ADHD diagnosis and treatment (Shi et al., Reference Shi, Guevara, Dykhoff, Sangaralingham, Phelan, Zaccariello and Warner2021; Wong & Landes, Reference Wong and Landes2021). A meta-analysis showed that the association between birth weight and ADHD symptoms was largest in Asia than other continents (North America, South America, Australia & New Zealand, Europe; Momany et al., Reference Momany, Kamradt and Nikolas2018) which, when combined with our results, suggests distinct effects of race/ethnicity and place of residence (e.g., Asian Americans vs. Asians in Asia). Our findings also show that non-singleton birth is a robust protective factor (Figure 2). Twin-ship may offer a more supportive environment due to the close social contact with the co-twin postnatally (Pulkkinen et al., Reference Pulkkinen, Vaalamo, Hietala, Kaprio and Rose2003).

Strengths and limitations

The main strengths of this study are its use of a large data set and the inclusion of wide range of prenatal/perinatal factors. There are few studies in this area which possess both these methodological strengths. Second, use of elastic net regression and 5-fold validation to identify robust predictors of attention problems was novel, and appropriate for this set of correlated pre/perinatal predictors (Figure S6). Third, we used symptom scales as our outcome rather than diagnosis. While diagnosis is clinically informative, it ignores the extent of inter-individual variation and may overlook smaller pre-clinical effects. Finally, we stratified our analyses by social and economic factors which tested generalizability of findings to subgroups of this population.

The primary limitation of this study is the retrospective reporting of pre/perinatal data and the reliance on parental report for exposure, outcome and covariate data. Such reports are subject to recall and rater bias. For instance, studies matching maternal retrospective reports with medical records show mismatch in rates of alcohol use and some complications such as nausea and proteinuria (Liu et al., Reference Liu, Tuvblad, Li, Raine and Baker2013; Rice et al., Reference Rice, Lewis, Harold, van den Bree, Boivin, Hay, Owen and Thapar2007). A second limitation is the sampling bias associated with cohort studies. Missing data patterns were non-random, with those reporting on all variables more likely to have lower CBCL attention problems and fewer prenatal risk factors (Table S10). Third, some important pre/perinatal variables were not available in the data set (e.g., maternal stress). Fourth, CBCL attention problems did not discriminate between the different ADHD symptom domains, which could be differentially impacted by pre/perinatal factors. Finally, despite using machine learning approaches and elastic net feature selection, the model used is relatively simple. For instance, we did not test for interactions between pre/perinatal factors or acknowledge clustering of participants within the 22 research sites. Once the predictive accuracy of an ADHD-prediction model is improved by future studies, the performance of various predictive modeling techniques should be compared (e.g., random forests, classification and regression trees).

Conclusion

We explained roughly 8% of the variance in age 9 ADHD-like symptoms using information available at most births. Almost half of the robust predictors identified were potentially preventable (maternal substance-use, anemia, UTI, young parental age at birth). Future studies will need to validate these results using prospectively collected data and try to improve prediction accuracy by comparing other, more complex, prediction techniques. These findings may inform interventions to prevent and minimize childhood ADHD symptoms, particularly in the United States.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579423000238

Acknowledgements

We gratefully acknowledge the contribution of participants and parents from the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) study, as well as and the researchers that conducted the research. We also thank the funding bodies that enabled this research.

Funding statement

This work was supported by a StAR International PhD Scholarship awarded to ND by the Royal College of Surgeons in Ireland. During this project, DC, CH, and M.Clarke were funded by the Health Research Board of Ireland (DC: HRB ILP POR 2019–005, CH: ILP-PHR-2019-009, MC: HRA-PHR-2015–1130) and an Irish Research Council award (MC: COALESCE/2019/61) and M. Cannon was funded by a Consolidator Award from the European Research Council (iHEAR 724809). The ABCD Study is supported by the National Institutes of Health (NIH) and additional federal partners under award numbers U01DA041022, U01DA041028, U01DA041048, U01DA041089, U01DA041106, U01DA041117, U01DA041120, U01DA041134, U01DA041148, U01DA041156, U01DA041174, U24DA041123, and U24DA041147. A full list of supporters is available at https://abcdstudy.org/nih-collaborators. None of the funding or supportive bodies of ABCD (NIH) take any responsibility for the views expressed or the outputs generated in this study.

Conflicts of interest

None.

References

Achenbach, T., & Rescorla, L. (2001). Manual for the ASEBA school-age forms & profiles: An integrated system of multi-informant assessment. University of Vermont. Research Center for Children, Youth, & Families.Google Scholar
Achenbach, T. M., & Verhulst, F. (2010). Achenbach system of empirically based assessment (ASEBA).Google Scholar
Agnew-Blais, J. C., Polanczyk, G. V., Danese, A., Wertz, J., Moffitt, T. E., & Arseneault, L. (2018). Young adult mental health and functional outcomes among individuals with remitted, persistent and late-onset ADHD. British Journal of Psychiatry, 213(3), 526534.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Agnew-Blais, J. C., Wertz, J., Arseneault, L., Belsky, D. W., Danese, A., Pingault, J. B., Polanczyk, G. V., Sugden, K., Williams, B., & Moffitt, T. E. (2022, October). Mother’s and children’s ADHD genetic risk, household chaos and children’s ADHD symptoms: A gene-environment correlation study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 63(10), 11531163. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.13659 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bailey, R. K., Jaquez-Gutierrez, M. C., & Madhoo, M. (2014). Sociocultural issues in african american and Hispanic minorities seeking care for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Primary Care Companion to CNS Disorders, 16(4). https://doi.org/10.4088/PCC.14r01627 Google ScholarPubMed
Bernardes, J., Goncalves, H., Ayres-de-Campos, D., & Rocha, A. P. (2009). Sex differences in linear and complex fetal heart rate dynamics of normal and acidemic fetuses in the minutes preceding delivery. Journal of Perinatal Medicine, 37(2), 168176.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Biederman, J., Monuteaux, M. C., Mick, E., Spencer, T., Wilens, T. E., Silva, J. M., Snyder, L. E., & Faraone, S. V. (2006). Young adult outcome of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: A controlled 10-year follow-up study. Psychological Medicine, 36(2), 167179.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blumenshine, P., Egerter, S., Barclay, C. J., Cubbin, C., & Braveman, P. A. (2010). Socioeconomic disparities in adverse birth outcomes: A systematic review. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 39(3), 263272. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.amepre.2010.05.012 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boyle, C. A., Boulet, S., Schieve, L. A., Cohen, R. A., Blumberg, S. J., Yeargin-Allsopp, M., Visser, S., & Kogan, M. D. (2011). Trends in the prevalence of developmental disabilities in US children, 1997–2008. Pediatrics, 127(6), 10341042. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2010-2989.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brannigan, R., Healy, C., Cannon, M., Leacy, F. P., & Clarke, M. C. (2020, October). Prenatal tobacco exposure and psychiatric outcomes in adolescence: Is the effect mediated through birth weight? Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 142(4), 284293. https://doi.org/10.1111/acps.13210 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brinksma, D. M., Hoekstra, P. J., van den Hoofdakker, B., de Bildt, A., Buitelaar, J. K., Hartman, C. A., & Dietrich, A. (2017, July). Age-dependent role of pre- and perinatal factors in interaction with genes on ADHD symptoms across adolescence. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 90, 110117. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2017.02.014 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Catov, J. M., Lee, M., Roberts, J. M., Xu, J., & Simhan, H. N. (2015). Race disparities and decreasing birth weight: Are all babies getting smaller? American Journal of Epidemiology, 183(1), 1523. https://doi.org/10.1093/aje/kwv194 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caye, A., Agnew-Blais, J., Arseneault, L., Goncalves, H., Kieling, C., Langley, K., Menezes, A. M. B., Moffitt, T. E., Passos, I. C., Rocha, T. B., Sibley, M. H., Swanson, J. M., Thapar, A., Wehrmeister, F., & Rohde, L. A. (2019, May 15). A risk calculator to predict adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Generation and external validation in three birth cohorts and one clinical sample. Epidemiology and Psychiatric Sciences, 29, e37. https://doi.org/10.1017/S2045796019000283 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Centers for Disease Control & Prevention (2011). Vital signs: Overdoses of prescription opioid pain relievers—United States, 1999–2008. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report, 60(43), 14871492.Google Scholar
Chang, L.-Y., Wang, M.-Y., & Tsai, P.-S. (2016). Diagnostic accuracy of rating scales for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A meta-analysis. Pediatrics, 137(3), e20152749. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2015-2749 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chang, Z., Lichtenstein, P., D’Onofrio, B. M., Almqvist, C., Kuja-Halkola, R., Sjölander, A., & Larsson, H. (2014). Maternal age at childbirth and risk for ADHD in offspring: A population-based cohort study. International Journal of Epidemiology, 43(6), 18151824.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clarke, M. C., Tanskanen, A., Huttunen, M., Whittaker, J. C., & Cannon, M. (2009). Evidence for an interaction between familial liability and prenatal exposure to infection in the causation of schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry, 166(9), 10251030.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cleary-Goldman, J., Malone, F. D., Vidaver, J., Ball, R. H., Nyberg, D. A., Comstock, C. H., Saade, G. R., Eddleman, K. A., Klugman, S., Dugoff, L., Timor-Tritsch, I. E., Craigo, S. D., Carr, S. R., Wolfe, H. M., Bianchi, D. W., D'Alton, M., & FASTER Consortium (2005). Impact of maternal age on obstetric outcome. Obstetrics and Gynecology, 105(5 Part 1), 983990. https://doi.org/10.1097/01.Aog.0000158118.75532.51 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cnattingius, S. (2004). The epidemiology of smoking during pregnancy: Smoking prevalence, maternal characteristics, and pregnancy outcomes. Nicotine & Tobacco Research, 6(Suppl_2), S125S140.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Collins, K. P., & Cleary, S. D. (2016). Racial and ethnic disparities in parent-reported diagnosis of ADHD: National Survey of Children’s Health (2003, 2007, and 2011). The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 77(1), 16572.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DiPietro, J. A., & Voegtline, K. M. (2017). The gestational foundation of sex differences in development and vulnerability. Neuroscience, 342, 420.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dooley, N., Clarke, M., Cotter, D., & Cannon, M. (2022). Birth weight and childhood psychopathology in the ABCD cohort: Association is strongest for attention problems and is moderated by sex. Research on Child and Adolescent Psychopathology, 50(5), 563575. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-021-00859-0 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dubber, S., Reck, C., Müller, M., & Gawlik, S. (2015). Postpartum bonding: The role of perinatal depression, anxiety and maternal-fetal bonding during pregnancy. Archives of Women’s Mental Health, 18(2), 187195. http://doi.org/10.1007/s00737-014-0445-4 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Finch, B. K. (2003). Socioeconomic gradients and low birth-weight: Empirical and policy considerations. Health Services Research, 38(6p2), 18191842.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Flinkkilä, E., Keski-Rahkonen, A., Marttunen, M., & Raevuori, A. (2016). Prenatal inflammation, infections and mental disorders. Psychopathology, 49(5), 317333.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Franz, A. P., Bolat, G. U., Bolat, H., Matijasevich, A., Santos, I. S., Silveira, R. C., Procianoy, R. S., Rohde, L. A., & Moreira-Maia, C. R. (2018). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and very preterm/very low birth weight: A meta-analysis. Pediatrics, 141(1).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Franz, A. P., Caye, A., Lacerda, B. C., Wagner, F., Silveira, R. C., Procianoy, R. S., Moreira-Maia, C. R., & Rohde, L. A. (2022). Development of a risk calculator to predict attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in very preterm/very low birth weight newborns. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 63(8), 929938. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.13546 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friedman, J., Hastie, T., & Tibshirani, R. (2010). Regularization paths for generalized linear models via coordinate descent. Journal of Statistical Software, 33(1), 122. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v033.i01 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fusar-Poli, P., Bauer, M., Borgwardt, S., Bechdolf, A., Correll, C. U., Do, K. Q., Domschke, K., Galderisi, S., Kessing, L. V., Koutsouleris, N., Krebs, M. O., Lennox, B., McGuire, P., Meyer-Lindenberg, A., Millan, M. J., Nieman, D., Pfennig, A., Sand, M., Whenert, A., …Arango, C. (2019). European college of neuropsychopharmacology network on the prevention of mental disorders and mental health promotion (ECNP PMD-MHP). European Neuropsychopharmacology, 29(12), 13011311. https://doi.org/http://doi.org/10.1016/j.euroneuro.2019.09.006 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Getahun, D., Rhoads, G. G., Demissie, K., Lu, S.-E., Quinn, V. P., Fassett, M. J., Wing, D. A., & Jacobsen, S. J. (2013). In utero exposure to ischemic-hypoxic conditions and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Pediatrics, 131(1), e53e61.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Giscombé, C. L., & Lobel, M. (2005). Explaining disproportionately high rates of adverse birth outcomes among African Americans: The impact of stress, racism, and related factors in pregnancy. Psychological Bulletin, 131(5), 662683.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldenberg, R. L., Culhane, J. F., Iams, J. D., & Romero, R. (2008). Epidemiology and causes of preterm birth. The Lancet, 371(9606), 7584. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0140-6736(08)60074-4 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
He, Y., Chen, J., Zhu, L.-H., Hua, L.-L., & Ke, F.-F. (2020). Maternal smoking during pregnancy and ADHD: Results from a systematic review and meta-analysis of prospective cohort studies. Journal of Attention Disorders, 24(12), 16371647.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Herba, C. M., Glover, V., Ramchandani, P. G., & Rondon, M. B. (2016). Maternal depression and mental health in early childhood: An examination of underlying mechanisms in low-income and middle-income countries. The Lancet Psychiatry, 3(10), 983992.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hug, L., Alexander, M., You, D., & Alkema, L. (2019). National, regional, and global levels and trends in neonatal mortality between 1990 and 2017, with scenario-based projections to 2030: A systematic analysis. The Lancet Global Health, 7(6), e710e720. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2214-109X(19)30163-9 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huhdanpaa, H., Morales-Munoz, I., Aronen, E. T., Polkki, P., Saarenpaa-Heikkila, O., Kylliainen, A., & Paavonen, E. J. (2021, October). Prenatal and postnatal predictive factors for children’s inattentive and hyperactive symptoms at 5 years of age: The role of early family-related factors. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 52(5), 783799. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10578-020-01057-7 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Inkster, A. M., Fernandez-Boyano, I., & Robinson, W. P. (2021, July 5). Sex differences are here to stay: Relevance to prenatal care. Journal of Clinical Medicine, 10(13). https://doi.org/http://doi.org/10.3390/jcm10133000 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, D. R., Bale, T. L., & Epperson, C. N. (2015, February). Prenatal programming of mental illness: Current understanding of relationship and mechanisms. Current Psychiatry Reports, 17(2), 5. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11920-014-0546-9 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lampert, T., Polanczyk, G., Tramontina, S., Mardini, V., & Rohde, L. (2004). Diagnostic performance of the CBCL-Attention Problem Scale as a screening measure in a sample of Brazilian children with ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders, 8(2), 6371.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laplante, F., Brake, W. G., Chehab, S. L., & Sullivan, R. M. (2012). Sex differences in the effects of perinatal anoxia on dopamine function in rats. Neuroscience Letters, 506(1), 8993.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lees, B., Mewton, L., Jacobus, J., Valadez, E. A., Stapinski, L. A., Teesson, M., Tapert, S. F., & Squeglia, L. M. (2020, November 1). Association of prenatal alcohol exposure with psychological, behavioral, and neurodevelopmental outcomes in children from the adolescent brain cognitive development study. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 177(11), 10601072. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2020.20010086 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lim, K. X., Liu, C. Y., Schoeler, T., Cecil, C. A., Barker, E. D., Viding, E., Greven, C. U., & Pingault, J. B. (2018). The role of birth weight on the causal pathway to child and adolescent ADHD symptomatology: A population-based twin differences longitudinal design. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 59(10), 10361043.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liu, J., Tuvblad, C., Li, L., Raine, A., & Baker, L. A. (2013). Medical record validation of maternal recall of pregnancy and birth events from a twin cohort. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 16(4), 845860. https://doi.org/10.1017/thg.2013.31 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Madden, D. (2014). The relationship between low birth weight and socioeconomic status in Ireland. Journal of Biosocial Science, 46(2), 248265. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0021932013000187 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mann, J. R., McDermott, S. (2011, November). Are maternal genitourinary infection and pre-eclampsia associated with ADHD in school-aged children? Journal of Attention Disorders, 15(8), 667673. https://doi.org/10.1177/1087054710370566 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martel, M. M., Lucia, V. C., Nigg, J. T., & Breslau, N. (2007). Sex differences in the pathway from low birth weight to inattention/hyperactivity. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 35(1), 8796.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martinson, M. L., & Reichman, N. E. (2016). Socioeconomic inequalities in low birth weight in the United States, the United Kingdom, Canada, and Australia. American Journal of Public Health, 106(4), 748754. https://doi.org/10.2105/ajph.2015.303007 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McNicholas, F., Healy, E., White, M., Sheridan-Pereira, M., O’Connor, N., Coakley, S., & Dooley, B. (2016). Mental health outcomes at age 11 of very low birth weight infants in Ireland. Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine, 33(2), 93104.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Milberger, S., Biederman, J., Faraone, S. V., Guite, J., & Tsuang, M. T. (1997). Pregnancy, delivery and infancy complications and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Issues of gene-environment interaction. Biological Psychiatry, 41(1), 6575.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mohammed, R., Rawashdeh, J., & Abdullah, M. (2020). Machine learning with oversampling and undersampling techniques: overview study and experimental results. In: 2020 11th international conference on information and communication systems (ICICS).Google Scholar
Momany, A. M., Kamradt, J. M., & Nikolas, M. A. (2018, October). A meta-analysis of the association between birth weight and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 46(7), 14091426. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-017-0371-9 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Momany, A. M., Kamradt, J. M., Ullsperger, J. M., Elmore, A. L., Nigg, J. T., & Nikolas, M. A. (2017, February). Sex moderates the impact of birth weight on child externalizing psychopathology. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 126(2), 244256. https://doi.org/10.1037/abn0000238 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murray, A. L., Booth, T., Eisner, M., Auyeung, B., Murray, G., & Ribeaud, D. (2019). Sex differences in ADHD trajectories across childhood and adolescence. Developmental Science, 22(1), e12721. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12721 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nomura, Y., Marks, D. J., Grossman, B., Yoon, M., Loudon, H., Stone, J., & Halperin, J. M. (2012). Exposure to gestational diabetes mellitus and low socioeconomic status: Effects on neurocognitive development and risk of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in offspring. Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine, 166(4), 337343.Google ScholarPubMed
O'Donnell, K. J., Glover, V., Lahti, J., Lahti, M., Edgar, R. D., Raikkonen, K., & O'Connor, T. G. (2017). Maternal prenatal anxiety and child COMT genotype predict working memory and symptoms of ADHD. PloS One, 12(6), e0177506. https://doi.org/http://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0177506 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ornoy, A., & Spivak, A. (2019). Cost effectiveness of optimal treatment of ADHD in Israel: A suggestion for national policy. Health Economics Review, 9(1), 24. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13561-019-0240-z CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paul, S. E., Hatoum, A. S., Fine, J. D., Johnson, E. C., Hansen, I., Karcher, N. R., Moreau, A. L., Bondy, E., Qu, Y., Carter, E. B., Rogers, C. E., Agrawal, A., Barch, D. M., & Bogdan, R. (2021, January 1). Associations between prenatal cannabis exposure and childhood outcomes: Results from the ABCD study. Journal of the American Medical Association: Psychiatry, 78(1), 6476. https://doi.org/http://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2020.2902 Google ScholarPubMed
Pettersson, E., Larsson, H., D'Onofrio, B., Almqvist, C., & Lichtenstein, P. (2019, February 6). Association of fetal growth with general and specific mental health conditions. Journal of the American Medical Association: Psychiatry, 76(5), 536. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2018.4342 Google ScholarPubMed
Pingault, J.-B., Barkhuizen, W., Wang, B., Hannigan, L. J., Eilertsen, E. M., Corfield, E., Andreassen, O. A., Ask, H., Tesli, M., Askeland, R. B., Davey Smith, G., Stoltenberg, C., Davies, N. M., Reichborn-Kjennerud, T., Ystrom, E., & Havdahl, A. (2022). Genetic nurture versus genetic transmission of risk for ADHD traits in the Norwegian Mother, Father and Child Cohort Study. Molecular Psychiatry. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41380-022-01863-6 Google ScholarPubMed
Pulkkinen, L., Vaalamo, I., Hietala, R., Kaprio, J., & Rose, R. J. (2003). Peer reports of adaptive behavior in twins and singletons: Is twinship a risk or an advantage? Twin Research and Human Genetics, 6(2), 106118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ramos, A. M., Marceau, K., Neiderhiser, J. M., De Araujo-Greecher, M., Natsuaki, M. N., & Leve, L. D. (2020). Maternal consistency in recalling prenatal experiences at 6 months and 8 years postnatal. Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics, 41(9), 698705. https://doi.org/10.1097/dbp.0000000000000841 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rice, F., Langley, K., Woodford, C., Davey Smith, G., & Thapar, A. (2018, August). Identifying the contribution of prenatal risk factors to offspring development and psychopathology: What designs to use and a critique of literature on maternal smoking and stress in pregnancy. Development and Psychopathology, 30(3), 11071128. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579418000421 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rice, F., Lewis, A., Harold, G., van den Bree, M., Boivin, J., Hay, D. F., Owen, M. J., & Thapar, A. (2007). Agreement between maternal report and antenatal records for a range of pre and peri-natal factors: The influence of maternal and child characteristics. Early Human Development, 83(8), 497504.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roffman, J. L., Sipahi, E. D., Dowling, K. F., Hughes, D. E., Hopkinson, C. E., Lee, H., Eryilmaz, H., Cohen, L. S., Gilman, J., Doyle, A. E., & Dunn, E. C. (2021). Association of adverse prenatal exposure burden with child psychopathology in the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study. PloS One, 16(4), e0250235. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0250235 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosenthal, L., Earnshaw, V. A., Moore, J. M., Ferguson, D. N., Lewis, T. T., Reid, A. E., Lewis, J. B., Stasko, E. C., Tobin, J. N., & Ickovics, J. R. (2018). Intergenerational consequences: Women’s experiences of discrimination in pregnancy predict infant social-emotional development at 6 months and 1 year. Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics, 39(3). https://journals.lww.com/jrnldbp/Fulltext/2018/04000/Intergenerational_Consequences__Women_s.6.aspx CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Russell, A. E., Ford, T., Williams, R., & Russell, G. (2016). The association between socioeconomic disadvantage and attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): A systematic review. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 47(3), 440458.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sampaio, F., Feldman, I., Lavelle, T. A., & Skokauskas, N. (2021). The cost-effectiveness of treatments for attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder and autism spectrum disorder in children and adolescents: A systematic review. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 31(11), 16551670. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-021-01748-z CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Santa-Marina, L., Lertxundi, N., Andiarena, A., Irizar, A., Sunyer, J., Molinuevo, A., Llop, S., Julvez, J., Beneito, A., Ibarluzea, J., Imaz, L., & Ferrin, M. (2020). Maternal ferritin levels during pregnancy and ADHD symptoms in 4-year-old children: Results from the INMA-INfancia y medio ambiente (environment and childhood) prospective birth cohort study. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 17(21), 7704.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwenke, E., Fasching, P. A., Faschingbauer, F., Pretscher, J., Kehl, S., Peretz, R., Keller, A., Haberle, L., Eichler, A., Irlbauer-Muller, V., Dammer, U., Beckmann, M. W., & Schneider, M. (2018, November). Predicting attention deficit hyperactivity disorder using pregnancy and birth characteristics. Archives of Gynecology and Obstetrics, 298(5), 889895. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00404-018-4888-0 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sciberras, E., Ukoumunne, O. C., & Efron, D. (2011, October). Predictors of parent-reported attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in children aged 6-7 years: A national longitudinal study. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 39(7), 10251034. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-011-9504-8 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sen, B., Borle, N. C., Greiner, R., & Brown, M. R. (2018). A general prediction model for the detection of ADHD and Autism using structural and functional MRI. PloS One, 13(4), e0194856.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shi, Y., Guevara, L. R. H., Dykhoff, H. J., Sangaralingham, L. R., Phelan, S., Zaccariello, M. J., & Warner, D. O. (2021). Racial disparities in diagnosis of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in a US national birth cohort. JAMA Network Open, 4(3), e210321.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shrive, F. M., Stuart, H., Quan, H., & Ghali, W. A. (2006). Dealing with missing data in a multi-question depression scale: A comparison of imputation methods. BMC Medical Research Methodology, 6(1), 57. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2288-6-57 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Silva, D., Colvin, L., Hagemann, E., & Bower, C. (2014, January). Environmental risk factors by gender associated with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Pediatrics, 133(1), e1422. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2013-1434 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smedberg, J., Lupattelli, A., Mårdby, A.-C., Øverland, S., & Nordeng, H. (2015). The relationship between maternal depression and smoking cessation during pregnancy—a cross-sectional study of pregnant women from 15 European countries. Archives of Women’s Mental Health, 18(1), 7384. https://doi.org/http://doi.org/10.1007/s00737-014-0470-3 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smidts, D. P., & Oosterlaan, J. (2007). How common are symptoms of ADHD in typically developing preschoolers? A study on prevalence rates and prenatal/demographic risk factors. Cortex, 43(6), 710717. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70500-8 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, T. F., Schmidt-Kastner, R., McGeary, J. E., Kaczorowski, J. A., & Knopik, V. S. (2016, May). Pre- and perinatal ischemia-hypoxia, the ischemia-hypoxia response pathway, and ADHD risk. Behavior Genetics, 46(3), 467477. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10519-016-9784-4 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sousa, F. J., Correia, R. G., Cruz, A. F., Martins, J. M., Rodrigues, M. S., Gomes, C. A., Ambrósio, A. F., & Baptista, F. I. (2020). Sex differences in offspring neurodevelopment, cognitive performance and microglia morphology associated with maternal diabetes: Putative targets for insulin therapy. Brain, Behavior, & Immunity-Health, 5, 100075.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Talbot, C. P. J., & Dolinsky, V. W. (2019). Sex differences in the developmental origins of cardiometabolic disease following exposure to maternal obesity and gestational diabetes. Applied Physiology, Nutrition, and Metabolism, 44(7), 687695.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thapar, A., Rice, F., Hay, D., Boivin, J., Langley, K., van den Bree, M., Rutter, M., & Harold, G. (2009, October 15). Prenatal smoking might not cause attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Evidence from a novel design. Biological Psychiatry, 66(8), 722727. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.05.032 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Waljee, A. K., Mukherjee, A., Singal, A. G., Zhang, Y., Warren, J., Balis, U., Marrero, J., Zhu, J., & Higgins, P. D. (2013). Comparison of imputation methods for missing laboratory data in medicine. BMJ Open, 3(8), e002847. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmjopen-2013-002847 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, X., Meng, F.-S., Liu, Z.-Y., Fan, J.-M., Hao, K., Chen, X.-Q., & Du, J.-Z. (2013). Gestational hypoxia induces sex-differential methylation of Crhr1 linked to anxiety-like behavior. Molecular Neurobiology, 48(3), 544555.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wiegersma, A. M., Dalman, C., Lee, B. K., Karlsson, H., & Gardner, R. M. (2019). Association of prenatal maternal anemia with neurodevelopmental disorders. Journal of the American Medical Association: Psychiatry, 76(12), 12941304. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2019.2309 Google ScholarPubMed
Wiggs, K., Elmore, A. L., Nigg, J. T., & Nikolas, M. A. (2016, November). Pre- and perinatal risk for attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: Does neuropsychological weakness explain the link? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 44(8), 14731485. https://doi.org/http://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-016-0142-z CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Willcutt, E. G. (2012). The prevalence of DSM-IV attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A meta-analytic review. Neurotherapeutics, 9(3), 490499.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Willoughby, M., Williams, J., Mills-Koonce, W., & Blair, C. (2020, August). Early life predictors of attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptomatology profiles from early through middle childhood. Development and Psychopathology, 32(3), 791802. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579419001135 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wong, A. W., & Landes, S. D. (2021). Expanding understanding of racial-ethnic differences in ADHD prevalence rates among children to include Asians and Alaskan natives/American Indians. Journal of Attention Disorders, 26(5), 747754. https://doi.org/10.1177/10870547211027932 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yoshimasu, K., Barbaresi, W. J., Colligan, R. C., Voigt, R. G., Killian, J. M., Weaver, A. L., & Katusic, S. K. (2012). Childhood ADHD is strongly associated with a broad range of psychiatric disorders during adolescence: A population-based birth cohort study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 53(10), 10361043. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2012.02567.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zou, H., & Hastie, T. (2005). Regularization and variable selection via the elastic net. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series B (Statistical Methodology), 67(2), 301320.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Descriptive statistics of all pre/perinatal variables and selection frequency of each in full-sample prediction of age 9 ADHD symptoms

Figure 1

Figure 1. Robust pre/perinatal predictors of ADHD symptoms, in order of strength (mean B coefficient) in the full sample. Error bars indicate 95% confidence intervals (N = 9,975).

Figure 2

Figure 2. Subgroup variation in capacity to predict age 9 CBCL attention problems from pre/perinatal factors. R-squared averaged over 100 test sets. Error bars indicate ±1 standard deviation.

Figure 3

Table 2. Group-stratified results

Figure 4

Table 3. Sex-stratified predictors of age 9 ADHD symptoms

Supplementary material: File

Dooley et al. supplementary material

Tables S1-S10 and Figures S1-S6

Download Dooley et al. supplementary material(File)
File 1.8 MB