Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-p9bg8 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T07:54:03.242Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Pubertal changes in emotional information processing: Pupillary, behavioral, and subjective evidence during emotional word identification

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  15 January 2009

Jennifer S. Silk*
Affiliation:
University of Pittsburgh School of Medicine
Greg J. Siegle
Affiliation:
University of Pittsburgh School of Medicine
Diana J. Whalen
Affiliation:
University of Pittsburgh School of Medicine
Laura J. Ostapenko
Affiliation:
University of Pittsburgh School of Medicine
Cecile D. Ladouceur
Affiliation:
University of Pittsburgh School of Medicine
Ronald E. Dahl
Affiliation:
University of Pittsburgh School of Medicine
*
Address correspondence and reprint requests to: Jennifer S. Silk, Department of Psychiatry, University of Pittsburgh School of Medicine, 3811 O'Hara Street, Pittsburgh, PA 15260; E-mail: silkj@upmc.edu.

Abstract

This study investigated pupillary and behavioral responses to an emotional word valence identification paradigm among 32 pre-/early pubertal and 34 mid-/late pubertal typically developing children and adolescents. Participants were asked to identify the valence of positive, negative, and neutral words while pupil dilation was assessed using an eyetracker. Mid-/late pubertal children showed greater peak pupillary reactivity to words presented during the emotional word identification task than pre-/early pubertal children, regardless of word valence. Mid-/late pubertal children also showed smaller sustained pupil dilation than pre-/early pubertal children after the word was no longer on screen. These findings were replicated controlling for participants' age. In addition, mid-/late pubertal children had faster reaction times to all words, and rated themselves as more emotional during their laboratory visit compared to pre-/early pubertal children. Greater recall of emotional words following the task was associated with mid-/late pubertal status, and greater recall of emotional words was also associated with higher peak pupil dilation. These results provide physiological, behavioral, and subjective evidence consistent with a model of puberty-specific changes in neurobehavioral systems underpinning emotional reactivity.

Type
Special Section Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2009

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Achenbach, T. M. (1991). Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist/4–18 and 1991 profile. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Department of Psychiatry.Google Scholar
Alpert, J. E., Maddocks, A., Rosenbaum, J. F., & Fava, M. (1994). Childhood psychopathology retrospectively assessed among adults with early onset major depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 31, 165171.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.Google Scholar
Anderson, J. R., Qin, Y., Jung, K.-J., & Carter, C. S. (2007). Information-processing modules and their relative modality specificity. Cognitive Psychology, 54, 185217.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Angold, A., Costello, E. J., & Worthman, C. M. (1998). Puberty and depression: The roles of age, pubertal status and pubertal timing. Psychological Medicine, 28, 5161.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baird, A., Fugelsang, J., & Bennett, C. (2005, April). What were you thinking: An fMRI study of adolescent decision-making. Poster presented at Cognitive Neuroscience Society MeetingNew York.Google Scholar
Beatty, J. (1982). Task-evoked pupillary responses, processing load, and the structure of processing resources. Psychological Bulletin, 91, 276292.Google Scholar
Beatty, J. (1986). The pupil system. In Coles, M. G. H. (Ed.), Psychophysiology systems processes and applications (pp. 4450). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Beatty, J., & Lucero Wagoner, B. (2000). The pupillary system. In Cacioppo, J. T. (Ed.), Handbook of psychophysiology (2nd ed., pp. 142162). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Boev, A. N., Fountas, K. N., Karampelas, I., Boev, C., Machinis, T. G., Feltes, C., et al. (2005). Quantitative pupillometry: Normative data in healthy pediatric volunteers. Journal of Neurosurgery, 103, 496500.Google ScholarPubMed
Booth, J. R., Burman, D. D., Meyer, J. R., Gitelman, D. R., Parrish, T. B., & Mesulam, M. M. (2004). Development of brain mechanisms for processing orthographic and phonologic representations. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 12341249.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Booth, J. R., Burman, D. D., Van Santen, F. W., Harasaki, Y., Gitelman, D. R., Parrish, T. B., et al. (2001). The development of specialized brain systems in reading and oral-language. Child Neuropsychology, 7, 119141.Google Scholar
Brooks-Gunn, J., Warren, M. P., Rosso, J., & Gargiulo, J. (1987). Validity of self-report measures of girls' pubertal status. Child Development, 58, 829841.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caspi, A., Lynam, D., Moffitt, T. E., & Silva, P. A. (1993). Unraveling girls' delinquency: Biological, dispositional, and contextual contributions to adolescent misbehavior. Developmental Psychology, 29, 1930.Google Scholar
Cauffman, E., & Steinberg, L. (1995). The cognitive and affective influences on adolescent decision-making. Temple Law Review, 68, 17631789.Google Scholar
Chambers, R., Taylor, J. R., & Potenza, M. N. (2003). Developmental neurocircuitry of motivation in adolescence: A critical period of addiction vulnerability. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160, 10411052.Google Scholar
Chung, T., Martin, C. S., & Winters, K. C. (2006). Diagnosis, course and assessment of alcohol abuse and dependence in adolescents. In Galanter, M. (Ed.), Alcohol problems in adolescents and young adults: Epidemiology, neurobiology, prevention, and treatment (pp. 527). New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Cicchetti, D., & Rogosch, F. A. (2002). A developmental psychopathology perspective on adolescence. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 70, 620.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dahl, R. E. (2001). Affect regulation, brain development, and behavioral/emotional health in adolescence. CNS Spectrums, 6, 112.Google Scholar
Dahl, R. E. (2004). Adolescent brain development: A period of vulnerabilities and opportunities. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1021, 122.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dorn, L. D., Dahl, R. E., Woodward, H. R., & Biro, F. (2006). Defining the boundaries of early adolescence: A user's guide to assessing pubertal status and pubertal timing in research with adolescents. Applied Developmental Science, 10, 3056.Google Scholar
Ernst, M., Pine, D. S., & Hardin, M. (2005). Triadic model of the neurobiology of motivated behavior in adolescence. Psychological Medicine, 35, 114.Google Scholar
Gadow, K. D., Devincent, C. J., Pomeroy, J., & Azizian, A. (2005). Comparison of DSM-IV symptoms in elementary school-age children with PDD versus clinic and community samples. Autism, 9, 392415.Google Scholar
Gadow, K. D., & Sprafkin, J. (1998a). Adolescent Symptom Inventory—4: Norms manual. Stony Brook, NY: Checkmate Plus.Google Scholar
Gadow, K. D., & Sprafkin, J. (1998b). Child Symptom Inventory—4: Screening manual. Stony Brook, NY: Checkmate Plus.Google Scholar
Gardner, R. M., Philp, P., & Radacy, S. (1978). Pupillary changes during recall in children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 25, 168172.Google Scholar
Ge, X., Conger, R. D., & Elder, G. H. Jr. (1996). Coming of age too early: Pubertal influences on girls' vulnerability to psychological distress. Child Development, 67, 33863400.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gotlib, I. H., Traill, S. K., Montoya, R. L., Joormann, J., & Chang, K. (2005). Attention and memory biases in the offspring of parents with bipolar disorder: Indications from a pilot study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46, 8493.Google Scholar
Graber, J. A., Lewinsohn, P. M., Seeley, J. R., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (1997). Is psychopathology associated with the timing of pubertal development? Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 36, 17681776.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Granholm, E., Asarnow, R. F., Sarkin, A. J., & Dykes, K. L. (1996). Pupillary responses index cognitive resource limitations. Psychophysiology, 33, 457461.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guthrie, D., & Buchwald, J. S. (1991). Significance testing of difference potentials. Psychophysiology, 28, 240244.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Herman-Giddens, M. E. (2006). Recent data on pubertal milestones in united states children: The secular trend toward earlier development. International Journal of Andrology, 29, 241246.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hess, E. H. (1965). Attitude and pupil size. Scientific American, 212, 1965.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hollingshead, A. B. (1975). Four factor index of social status. Unpublished manuscript, Yale University, Sociology Department.Google Scholar
Janisse, M. P. (1973). Pupil size and affect: A critical review of the literature since 1960. The Canadian Psychologist, 14, 311329.Google Scholar
Janisse, M. P., & Peavler, W. S. (1974). Pupillary response today: Emotion in the eye. Psychology Today, 7, 6063.Google Scholar
Jernigan, T. L., & Sowell, E. R. (1997). Magnetic resonance imaging studies of developing brain. In Keshvan, M. S. & Murray, R. M. (Eds.), Neurodevelopment and adult psychopathology (pp. 6370) New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Juris, M., & Velden, M. (1977). The pupillary response to mental overload. Physiological Psychology, 5, 421424.Google Scholar
Kahneman, D., & Beatty, J. (1966). Pupil diameter and load on memory. Science, 154, 15831585.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kohnen, E. M., Zubcov, A. A., & Kohnen, T. (2004). Scotopic pupil size in a normal pediatric population using infrared pupillometry. Graefes Archive for Clinical & Experimental Ophthalmology, 242, 1823.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koikegami, H., & Yoshida, K. (1953). Pupillary dilation induced by stimulation of amygdaloid nuclei. Folia Pychiatrica Neurologica Japonica, 7, 109125.Google Scholar
Ladouceur, C. D., Dahl, R. E., Williamson, D. E., Birmaher, B., Ryan, N. D., & Casey, B. (2005). Altered emotional processing in pediatric anxiety, depression, and comorbid anxiety–depression. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 33, 165177.Google Scholar
Laurent, J., Catanzaro, S. J., Joiner, T. E. Jr., Rudolph, K. D., Potter, K. I., Lambert, S., et al. (1999). A measure of positive and negative affect for children: Scale development and preliminary validation. Psychological Assessment, 11, 326338.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewis, D. A. (1997). Development of the prefrontal cortex during adolescence: Insights into vulnerable neural circuits in schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology, 16, 385398.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luna, B., Thulborn, K. R., Munoz, D. P., Merriam, E. P., Garver, K. E., Minshew, N. J., et al. (2001). Maturation of widely distributed brain function subserves cognitive development. NeuroImage, 13, 786793.Google Scholar
MacLachlan, C., & Howland, H. C. (2002). Normal values and standard deviations for pupil diameter and interpupillary distance in subjects aged 1 month to 19 years. Ophthalmic & Physiological Optics, 22, 175182.Google Scholar
Martin, C. A., Kelly, T. H., Rayens, M. K., Brogli, B. R., Brenzel, A., Smith, W. J., et al. (2002). Sensation seeking, puberty and nicotine, alcohol and marijuana use in adolescence. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 41, 14951502.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Matthews, G., & Southall, A. (1991). Depression and the processing of emotional stimuli: A study of semantic priming. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 15, 283302.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Monk, C. S., McClure, E. B., Nelson, E. E., Zarahn, E., Bilder, R. M., Leibenluft, E., et al. (2003). Adolescent immaturity in attention-related brain engagement to emotional facial expressions. Neuroimage, 20, 420428.Google Scholar
Nelson, C. A., Bloom, F. E., Cameron, J. L., Amaral, D., Dahl, R. E., & Pine, D. (2002). An integrative, multidisciplinary approach to the study of brain–behavior relations in the context of typical and atypical development. Development and Psychopathology, 14, 499–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, E. E., Leibenluft, E., McClure, E., & Pine, D. S. (2005). The social re-orientation of adolescence: A neuroscience perspective on the process and its relation to psychopathology. Psychological Medicine, 35, 163174.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neshat-Doost, H. T., Moradi, A. R., Taghavi, M. R., Yule, W., & Dalgleish, T. (1999). The development of a corpus of emotional words produced by children and adolescents. Personality and Individual Differences, 27, 433451.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neshat-Doost, H. T., Taghavi, M. R., Moradi, A. R., Yule, W., & Dalgleish, T. (1998). Memory for emotional trait adjectives in clinically depressed youth.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Orr, D. P., & Ingersoll, G. M. (1995). The contribution of level of cognitive complexity and pubertal timing to behavioral risk in young adolescents. Pediatrics, 95, 528533.Google Scholar
Ozer, E. M., Macdonald, T., & Irwin, C. E. Jr. (2002). Adolescent health care in the united states: Implications and projections for the new millennium. In Mortimer, J. T. & Larson, R. W. (Eds.), The changing adolescent experience: Societal trends and the transition to adulthood (pp. 129174). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Partala, T., & Surakka, V. (2003). Pupil size variation as an indication of affective processing. International Journal of Human–Computer Studies, 59, 185198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Patton, G. C., & Viner, R. (2007). Pubertal transitions in health. The Lancet, 369, 11301139.Google Scholar
Perez Edgar, K., & Fox, N. A. (2003). Individual differences in children's performance during an emotional stroop task: A behavioral and electrophysiological study. Brain and Cognition, 52, 3351.Google Scholar
Petersen, A. C., Crockett, L., Richards, M., & Boxer, A. (1988). A self-report measure of pubertal status: Reliability, validity, and initial norms. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 17, 117133.Google Scholar
Pine, D. S., Cohen, P., Gurley, D., Brook, J., & Ma, Y. (1998). The risk for early-adulthood anxiety and depressive disorders in adolescents with anxiety and depressive disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry, 55, 5664.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Proverbio, A. M., & Zani, A. (2005). Developmental changes in the linguistic brain after puberty. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 164167.Google Scholar
Quevedo, K. M., Benning, S. D., Gunnar, M. R., & Dahl, R. E. (2009). The onset of puberty: Effects on the psychophysiology of defensive and appetitive motivation. Development and Psychopathology, 21, 2745.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Resnick, M. D., Bearman, P. S., Blum, R. W., Bauman, K. E., Harris, K. M., Jones, J., et al. (1997). Protecting adolescents from harm. Findings from the national longitudinal study on adolescent health. Journal of the American Medical Association, 278, 823832.Google Scholar
Reyna, V. F., & Farley, F. (2006). Risk and rationality in adolescent decision making: Implications for theory, practice, and public policy. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 7, 144.Google Scholar
Russell, J. A., & Fehr, B. (1987). Relativity in the perception of emotion in facial expressions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 116, 223237.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwartz, C. E., Wright, C. I., Shin, L. M., Kagan, J., Whalen, P. J., McMullin, K. G., et al. (2003). Differential amygdalar response to novel versus newly familiar neutral faces: A functional mri probe developed for studying inhibited temperament. Biological Psychiatry, 53, 854862.Google Scholar
Shirtcliff, E. A., Heiligenstein, M., Hoornstra, L., Squires, K., & Pollak, S. D. (2007). Raging hormones? Stages of pubertal development largely capture underlying hormonal processes in early adolescence. Paper presented at the Society for Research on Child DevelopmentBoston.Google Scholar
Siegle, G. J., Granholm, E., Ingram, R. E., & Matt, G. E. (2001). Pupillary and reaction time measures of sustained processing of negative information in depression. Biological Psychiatry, 49, 624636.Google Scholar
Siegle, G. J., Steinhauer, S. R., Carter, C. S., Ramel, W., & Thase, M. E. (2003). Do the seconds turn into hours? Relationships between sustained pupil dilation in response to emotional information and self-reported rumination. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 27, 365382.Google Scholar
Siegle, G. J., Steinhauer, S., Carter, C. S., & Thase, M. E. (2008). Is sustained processing specific to emotional information in depression? Evidence from pupil dilation. Manuscript submitted for publication.Google Scholar
Siegle, G. J., Steinhauer, S. R., Stenger, V. A., Konecky, R., & Carter, C. S. (2003). Use of concurrent pupil dilation assessment to inform interpretation and analysis of fMRI data. NeuroImage, 20, 114124.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Siegle, G. J., Steinhauer, S. R., & Thase, M. E. (2004). Pupillary assessment and computational modeling of the stroop task in depression. International Journal Psychophysiology, 52, 6376.Google Scholar
Silk, J. S., Dahl, R. E., Ryan, N. D., Forbes, E. E., Axelson, D. A., Birmaher, B., et al. (2007). Pupillary reactivity to emotional information in child and adolescent depression: Links to clinical and ecological measures. American Journal of Psychiatry, 164, 18731880.Google Scholar
Silk, J. S., Steinberg, L., & Morris, A. S. (2003). Adolescents' emotion regulation in daily life: Links to depressive symptoms and problem behavior. Child Development, 74, 18691880.Google Scholar
Slovic, P. (1987). Perception of risk. Science, 236, 280285.Google Scholar
Slovic, P. (2000). What does it mean to know a cumulative risk? Adolescent's perceptions of short-term and long-term consequences of smoking. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 13, 259266.Google Scholar
Sowell, E. R., & Jernigan, T. L. (1998). Further MRI evidence of late brain maturation: Limbic volume increases and changing asymmetries during childhood and adolescence. Developmental Neurophysiology, 14, 599617.Google Scholar
Spear, L. P. (2000). Neurobehavioral changes in adolescence. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 9, 111114.Google Scholar
Steinberg, L. (1987). Impact of puberty on family relations: Effects of pubertal status and pubertal timing. Developmental Psychology, 27, 451460.Google Scholar
Steinberg, L. (2005). Cognitive and affective development in adolescence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 6974.Google Scholar
Steinberg, L. (2007). Risk-taking in adolescence: New perspectives from brain and behavioral science. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 5559.Google Scholar
Steinberg, L., Dahl, R., Keating, D., Kupfer, D. J., Masten, A. S., & Pine, D. S. (2006). The study of developmental psychopathology in adolescence: Integrating affective neuroscience with the study of context. In Cicchetti, D. & Cohen, D. J. (Eds.), Developmental Psychopathology: Vol 2. Developmental neuroscience (2nd ed., pp. xvii, 876). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.Google Scholar
Steinberg, L., & Morris, A. (2000). Adolescent development. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 83110.Google Scholar
Steinberg, L., & Silk, J. S. (2002). Parenting adolescents. In Bornstein, M. H. (Ed.), Handbook of parenting: Vol. 1. Children and parenting (2nd ed., pp. 103133). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Steinhauer, S. R., Siegle, G. J., Condray, R., & Pless, M. (2004). Sympathetic and parasympathetic innervation of pupillary dilation during sustained processing. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 52, 7786.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stelmack, R. M., & Mandelzys, N. (1975). Extraversion and pupillary response to affective and taboo words. Psychophysiology, 12, 536540.Google Scholar
Szabadi, E., & Bradshaw, C. M. (1996). Autonomic pharmacology of 2-adrenoceptors. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 10, S.3, 618.Google Scholar
Taghavi, M. R., Dalgleish, T., Moradi, A. R., Neshat Doost, H. T., & Yule, W. (2003). Selective processing of negative emotional information in children and adolescents with generalized anxiety disorder. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 42, 221230.Google Scholar
Thomas, K. M., Drevets, W. C., Whalen, P. J., Eccard, C. H., Dahl, R. E., Ryan, N. D., et al. (2001). Amygdala response to facial expressions in children and adults. Biological Psychiatry, 49, 309316.Google Scholar
Urry, H. L., van Reekum, C. M., Johnstone, T., Kalin, N. H., Thurow, M. E., Schaefer, H. S., et al. (2006). Amygdala and ventromedial prefrontal cortex are inversely coupled during regulation of negative affect and predict the diurnal pattern of cortisol secretion among older adults. Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 44154425.Google Scholar
US Preventive Services Task Force. (1996). Guide to clinical preventive services (2nd ed.). Alexandria, VA: International Medical Publishing.Google Scholar
Vasey, M. W., El-Hag, N., & Daleiden, E. L. (1996). Anxiety and the processing of emotionally threatening stimuli: Distinctive patterns of selective attention among high- and low-test-anxious children. Child Development, 67, 11731185.Google Scholar
Whalen, P. J. (1998). Fear, vigilance, and ambiguity: Initial neuroimaging studies of the human amygdala. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 7, 177188.Google Scholar
Worthman, C. M. (1999). Epidemiology of human development. In Panter-Brick, C. & Worthman, C. M. (Eds.), Hormones, health, and behavior: A socioecological and lifespan perspective (pp. 47104). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar