Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-lnqnp Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T20:31:36.687Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Kinaesthetic Acuity and Motor Skills of Preschool Children with a Congenital Visual Impairment Preliminary Findings

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 October 2015

Shirley R. Wyver*
Affiliation:
Macquarie University
David J. Livesey
Affiliation:
The University of Sydney
*
Institute of Early Childhood, Macquarie University, NSW 2109, Phone: (02) 9850 9859, Facsmile: (02) 9850 9890, E-mail: swyver@iec.iec.mq.edu.au
Get access

Abstract

This paper presents the preliminary findings of a study examining the impact of congenital visual impairment on the development of motor skills and kinaesthetic acuity. Studies of children with a visual impairment have consistently noted deficits in motor development. It was hypothesised that these deficits may be linked with poor kinaesthetic sensitivity. Six preschool children with a visual impairment (mean age 55.17 months) were compared to nine classmates (mean age 56.67 months) matched for age, gender, and general verbal ability, on modified tasks from the Movement Assessment Battery for Children (M-ABC) and the Kinaesthetic Acuity Test (KAT). Although the difference between groups was significant for one task only (static balance), it was noted that all differences observed were in the hypothesised direction. Further longitudinal or cross-sectional data are required to provide a more comprehensive analysis of the relationship between vision impairment, kinaesthetic development, and motor skills.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Australian Psychological Society 1997

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Adelson, E., & Fraiberg, S. (1974). Gross motor development in infants blind from birth. Child Development, 45, 114126.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cass, H.D., Sonksen, P. M., & McConachie, H. R. (1994). Developmental setback in severe visual impairment. Archives of Disease in Childhood, 70, 192196.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Censullo, M. (1994). Developmental delay in healthy premature infants at age 2 years: Implications for early intervention. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 75, 99104.Google Scholar
Geerdink, J. J., Hopkins, B., & Hoeksma, J. B. (1994). The development of head position preference in preterm infants beyond term age. Developmental Psychobiology, 27,153168.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heermann, J. A., Jones, L. C., & Wikoff, R. L. (1994). Measurement of parent behaviour during interactions with their infants. Infant Behavior and Development, 17, 311321.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henderson, S. E., & Sugden, D. A. (1992). Movement Assessment Battery for Children. UK: The Psychological Corporation, Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.Google Scholar
Kalmar, M. (1996). The course of intellectual development in preterm and fullterm children: An 8-year longitudinal study. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 19, 491516.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kirsten, G. F., Vanzyl, J. I., LeGrange, M., Ancker, E., & Vanzyl, F. (1995). The outcome at 12 months of very-low-birth-weight infants ventilated at Tygerberg Hospital. South African Medical Journal, 85, 649654.Google ScholarPubMed
Laszlo, J. I., & Bairstow, P. J. (1985). Perceptual motor behaviour: Developmental assessment and therapy. New York: Praeger.Google Scholar
Livesey, D., & Coleman, R. (in press). The development of kinaesthesis and its relationship to motor ability in preschool children. In J. P., Piek (Ed.) Motor control and human skill: A multidisciplinary perspective. Champagne, IL: Human Kinetics.Google Scholar
Livesey, D. J., & Intili, D. (1996). A gender difference in visual-spatial ability in four year old children: Effects on performance of a kinaesthetic acuity task. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 63, 436446.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Livesey, D. J., & Parkes, N. (1995). Testing kinaesthetic acuity in preschool children. Australian Journal of Psychology, 47, 160163.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Minium, E. W. (1978). Statistical reasoning in psychology and education (2nd ed.). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Murphy, F. M., & O’Driscoll, M. (1989). Observations on the motor development of visually impaired children: Interpretations from video recordings. Physiotherapy, 75, 505508.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O’Callaghan, M. J., Burn, Y. R., Mohay, H. A., Rogers, Y., & Tudehope, D. I. (1993). The prevalence and origins of left hand preference in high risk infants, and its implications for intellectual, motor, and behavioural performance at 4 and 6 years. Cortex, 29, 617627.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piek, J. P., & Coleman-Carman, R. (1995). Kinaesthetic sensitivity and motor performance in children with Developmental Coordination Disorder. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 37, 976984.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roscoe, J. T. (1975). Fundamental research statistics for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston.Google Scholar
Seelye, W. (1983). Physical fitness of blind and visually impaired Detroit Public School children. Journal of Visual Impairment and Blindness, 77, 117118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Short, F. X., & Winnick, J. P. (1986). The influence of visual impairment on physical fitness test performance. Journal of Visual Impairment and Blindness, 80, 729731.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Short, EX., & Winnick, J.P. (1988). Adolescent physical fitness: A comparative study. Journal of Visual Impairment and Blindness, 82, 237239.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Siegel, S., & Castellan, N. J. (1988). Nonparametric statistics for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Smyth, T. R. (1996). Clumsiness: Kinaesthetic perception and translation. Child: Care, Health, and Development, 22, 19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Teplin, S. W. (1995). Visual impairment in infants and young children. Infants and Young Children, 8, 1851.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Troster, H., & Brambring, M. (1993). Early motor development in blind infants. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 14, 83106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Beek, Y., Hopkins, B., Hoeksma, J. B., & Samsom, J. F. (1994). Prematurity, posture, and the development of looking behaviour during early communication. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 35, 10931107.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Warren, D. H. (1994). Blindness and children: An individual differences approach. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wyver, S. R., & Chamberlain, L. A. (1993). Developmental profiles of partially-sighted 0-5 year olds using the Griffiths Mental Development Scales. Proceedings of the Australian New Zealand Association of Educators of Visually Handicapped Conference (pp. 202216). Melbourne: Royal Victorian Institute of the Blind.Google Scholar