Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-7cvxr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-30T22:06:59.688Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

A response to Cole

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  07 July 2017

ANS VAN KEMENADE*
Affiliation:
Radboud University Nijmegen, CLS/Dept. of English Language and Culture, Postbus 9103, 6500HD Nijmegen, The NetherlandsA.v.Kemenade@let.ru.nl

Extract

Marcelle Cole presents an interesting study of pronominal reference in Old English, nicely supplementing work available in the literature which shows, in brief, that in contexts with more than one possible referent, clause-initial nominative personal pronouns dominantly continue the topic (subject) of the previous clause, whereas clause-initial se-demonstratives dominantly switch the topic to a new referent of the previous clause. Cole adds to this with a study of the overall use of clause-initial pronouns in five Old English texts, which shows more variation than expected on the basis of this literature. Her conclusion is that se-forms by and large pick up discourse-new referents from the previous context. She further claims that her findings highlight how issues pertaining to style, such as the author–writer relationship, text type, subject matter and the conventionalism propagated by text tradition, influence anaphoric strategies in Old English. In this response article. I wish to counterbalance Cole's argument on two points, and make some suggestions for further research, based on recent psycholinguistic work.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2017 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Allen, Cynthia L. 1980. Topics in English diachonic syntax. New York: Garland.Google Scholar
Bosch, Peter, Katz, Graham & Umbach, Carla. 2007. The non-subject bias of German demonstrative pronouns. In Schwarz-Friesel, Monika, Consten, Manfred & Knees, Mareile (eds.), Anaphors in text: Cognitive, formal and applied approaches to anaphoric reference, 145–64. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dimitrova, Diana & van Kemenade, Ans. 2017. Discourse integration of demonstratives in L1 and L2. Unpublished MS.Google Scholar
Kaiser, Elsi. 2011. Salience and contrast effects in reference resolution: The interpretation of Dutch pronouns and demonstratives. Language and Cognitive Processes 26 (10), 1587–624.Google Scholar
Kehler, Laura Kertz, Rohde, Hannah & Elman, Jeffrey. 2008. Coherence and coreference revisited. Journal of Semantics (special issue on processing meaning) 25, 144.Google Scholar
Kemenade, Ans van. 1987. Syntactic case and morphological case in the history of English. Dordrecht: Foris.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Los, Bettelou & van Kemenade, Ans. 2017. Syntax and the morphology of deixis: the loss of demonstratives and paratactic clause linking. In Coniglio, Marco, Schlachter, Eva & Veenstra, Tonjes (eds.), Demonstratives. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton.Google Scholar
Majid, Asifa, Sanford, Anthony J. & Pickering, Martin J.. 2007. The linguistic description of minimal social scenarios affects the extent of causal inference making. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 43, 918–32.Google Scholar