Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-jn8rn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-27T05:15:55.488Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Special issue: Different perspectives on proper noun modifiers

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 October 2019

TINE BREBAN
Affiliation:
Department of Linguistics and English LanguageThe University of ManchesterOxford Road Manchester M13 9PLUnited Kingdomtine.breban@manchester.ac.uk
JULIA KOLKMANN
Affiliation:
Department of Language and Linguistic ScienceUniversity of YorkYork YO10 5DDUnited Kingdomjulia.kolkmann@york.ac.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Type
Introduction
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2019 

1 Introduction

Proper nouns used as modifiers, e.g. the Watergate scandal, a London theatre, the many Shakespeare biographies, are a common feature of the English language, perhaps most strikingly visible in news headlines. Their usage increased substantially as part of a general rise in usage of premodifying nouns in the nineteenth and twentieth centuries (Biber & Clark Reference Biber, Clark, Fanego, López-Couso and Pérez-Guerra2002; Rosenbach Reference Rosenbach2007; Biber et al. Reference Biber, Grieve, Iberri-Shea, Rohdenburg and Schlüter2009; Biber & Gray Reference Biber and Gray2011, Reference Biber and Gray2016). Though part of general changes to the English noun phrase (see also Günther Reference Günther2019), they stand out from other modifiers because of the special referential status of proper nouns. Where adjectival and nominal modifiers typically add further description, denoting, amongst others, subtypes (red grapes, cat food), properties (black dog, linen curtains) or evaluations (beautiful day), proper nouns in their prototypical usage refer to and identify individual people, places, organisations, etc. Used as modifiers, proper nouns retain their identifying function. In the Watergate scandal, Watergate refers to the Watergate office complex in Washington, DC, and singles out one particular scandal, i.e. the scandal happening in the office complex named Watergate. The function of proper nouns can be contrasted to common nouns in premodifying position (e.g. water bed, stone cottage), which serve to classify or describe rather than single out the entity denoted by the noun phrase.

The apparent incongruence of their occurrence as modifiers sparked interest not only for English, but also for other Germanic languages. The first scholars to single them out as special were Anette Rosenbach and Maria Koptjevskaja-Tamm (Koptjevskaja-Tamm & Rosenbach Reference Koptjevskaja-Tamm and Rosenbach2005). Since then, there has emerged a small but growing body of studies on English (Rosenbach Reference Rosenbach, Houswitschka, Knappe and Müller2006, Reference Rosenbach2007, Reference Rosenbach, Traugott and Trousdale2010; Breban Reference Breban2018), Swedish (Koptjevskaja-Tamm Reference Koptjevskaja-Tamm2009, Reference Koptjevskaja-Tamm, Börjars, Denison and Scott2013) and German (Zifonun Reference Zifonun2010a, Reference Zifonun, Scherer and Holler2010b; Schlücker Reference Schlücker2013, Reference Schlücker, Ackermann, Simon and Zimmer2018), which rather than fully explaining the phenomenon raise a variety of interesting questions and topics to explore. These questions pertain to different areas of linguistics and require different empirical data and methods to answer them. This special issue brings together linguists from a variety of backgrounds to offer different perspectives on proper noun modifiers and to take proper noun modifiers as an empirical starting point to explore questions in their diverse areas of expertise.

We start by introducing the main topics related to proper noun modifiers featuring in the literature to contextualise the questions researched in this issue (section 2). In section 3, we provide an overview of the individual articles, focusing on the different perspectives and methods they use and the larger questions they address. We conclude by drawing together the findings about English proper noun modifiers from the different articles (section 4).

We'd like to thank first and foremost all contributors to this special issue for sharing their work and their insights. We are very grateful to the contributors as well as external reviewers, who provided extensive and constructive feedback which not only helped the authors but also provided us as editors with new insights and potential connections between the articles. Finally, we thank Javier Pérez-Guerra and Elena Seoane, who gave us the opportunity to convene a workshop at BICLCE2017 in Vigo where some of the articles in this issue were first presented, and the editors of English Language and Linguistics for hosting this special issue.

2 State of the art

2.1 Proper noun modifiers: a form–function puzzle

The central focus of several studies (Rosenbach Reference Rosenbach2007, Reference Rosenbach, Traugott and Trousdale2010; Schlücker Reference Schlücker2013; Breban Reference Breban2018) is the puzzle of how to account for proper noun modifiers in models of the noun phrase. Proper noun modifiers can have an identifying function. They then restrict the denotation of the noun phrase to a particular referent, e.g. the London marathon identifies one specific marathon. In theoretical models of the noun phrase, elements with an identifying function are typically found at the left end of the phrase (amongst many others Teyssier's (Reference Teyssier1968) identifying > qualifying > classifying). However, as noun modifiers, proper noun modifiers occur at the right end of the premodifier string, in the position associated with elements with a classifying function.

Rosenbach (Reference Rosenbach2007, Reference Rosenbach, Traugott and Trousdale2010) deals with this mismatch by analysing proper noun modifiers as a particular case of constructional gradience between determiner genitives, with which they share the identifying function, and typical noun modifiers, with which they share the syntactic position. Schlücker's (Reference Schlücker2013) analysis focuses on German, where proper noun and head noun form a compound noun (see section 2.2). Her main goal is to show that the modifying elements in such compounds are not restricted to classifying functions. Within Rijkhoff's (Reference Rijkhoff2002) functional model of the noun phrase, she argues instead that identifying proper noun modifiers have a localising or anchoring function similar to determiner genitives. Breban (Reference Breban2018) questions the functional equivalence between determiner genitives and identifying proper noun modifiers. Instead she argues that modifiers such as London in the London marathon are ‘shorthand’ for a longer description of the referent, ‘the marathon that takes place in London’, and it is the description that leads to restriction of the denotation. As descriptive (or qualifying) elements their more rightward position in the noun phrase is not unexpected. A second set of proper noun modifiers, e.g. Coltrane in a Coltrane fan, have a different function and require a different analysis as complement rather than modifier: they always occur immediately to the left of the head noun and are thematically dependent on it (cf. synthetic modification).

In this issue, Rosenbach revisits the debate and defends her earlier analysis against the more recent proposals.

2.2 Phrasal modification versus compounding

The previous discussion already touched on the question about the morphosyntactic status of proper noun modifiers. For German and Swedish, this is not a question as proper noun modifiers are straightforwardly recognisable as part of compounds (Schlücker Reference Schlücker2013 and Koptjevskaja-Tamm Reference Koptjevskaja-Tamm, Börjars, Denison and Scott2013 respectively). In English, by contrast, how to distinguish phrasal modification and compounding is a longstanding and vexed question (see Giegerich Reference Giegerich2015 for a recent overview). Breban (Reference Breban2018) suggests a dual functionally motivated analysis with some proper noun modifiers being phrasal and other parts of compounds.

For most of the articles in this issue, the question is not a salient one that affects their argumentation. Alexiadou, for example, focuses solely on synthetic patterns, which she refers to as compounds. In other articles, the topic is touched on in passing, e.g. Rosenbach. Breban & De Smet show that the issue is not restricted to current English, but that the precursors of present-day proper noun modifiers are both clearly phrasal modifiers and likely compounds.

2.3 Alternation of proper noun modifiers and determiner genitives

Their relation to determiner genitives has been at the heart of the discussion surrounding proper noun modifiers. Rosenbach and Koptjevskaja-Tamm's seminal ideas were conceived against the background of their previous individual work on genitive and possessive constructions, and Rosenbach (Reference Rosenbach, Houswitschka, Knappe and Müller2006, Reference Rosenbach2007, Reference Rosenbach, Traugott and Trousdale2010) investigates proper noun modifiers as part of a wider research project on the grammatical variation and gradience between genitives and noun modifiers. Central to her argument is that determiner genitives and proper noun modifiers are semantically/functionally equivalent. This idea has been challenged: Breban (Reference Breban2018) argues that they both help identify the noun phrase referent but do this in different ways and are therefore functionally different. Schlücker (Reference Schlücker, Ackermann, Simon and Zimmer2018) argues that the two constructions are semantically different in that, even when they alternate, determiner genitives express relations within the ‘genitive repertoire’ and proper noun modifiers express relations from the ‘noun modifier repertoire’.

In this issue, Breban, Kolkmann & Payne also investigate the semantic relations expressed by determiner genitives and proper noun modifiers. But where Schlücker's argumentation was purely theoretical, they conduct an experiment in which participants are asked to provide paraphrases for the two constructions in actually attested examples. In her contribution, Rosenbach responds to the challenges by Schlücker (Reference Schlücker, Ackermann, Simon and Zimmer2018) and especially Breban (Reference Breban2018), arguing that the very concept of semantic equivalence is not the same in semantic–pragmatic and functional analyses as in variationist studies.

2.4 Languages in contrast

From the start, it was noted that the incongruent use of proper nouns as modifiers was not limited to English but had counterparts in other Germanic languages. Most existing studies have a single language as their primary focus but draw on comparison with other Germanic languages and with findings of studies on these languages. Koptjevskaja-Tamm (Reference Koptjevskaja-Tamm, Börjars, Denison and Scott2013) gives a detailed description of Swedish proper noun compounds but comments on similarities with and differences from English proper noun modifiers. Schlücker (Reference Schlücker2013) deals with German data but builds on Rosenbach's papers and draws comparisons with English. Papers highlight differences between the languages, including the morphosyntactic status of proper noun + noun, but also differences in the types of proper nouns attested. Most notably, English uses proper noun modifiers where German, for example, would use a prepositional construction or a derived adjective, e.g. the Berlin theatre vs das Theater in Berlin or das Berliner Theater.

Despite the comparison of different Germanic languages being a prominent feature throughout the literature, there are no systematic empirical contrastive studies. In this issue, Ström Herold & Levin set out to fill this gap using a new translation corpus LEGS (Linnaeus University English–German–Swedish Corpus). No studies so far have included discussion of the phenomenon beyond Germanic. Alexiadou, in this issue, builds a theoretical analysis of the status of proper nouns on the grammaticality of using them in synthetic compounds in English versus Greek.

2.5 History of proper noun modifiers

Rosenbach (Reference Rosenbach2007, Reference Rosenbach, Traugott and Trousdale2010) includes a diachronic corpus investigation of proper noun modifiers. She uses data from the British news section of the ARCHER corpus (1650–1999) to investigate whether the increase in proper noun modifiers in Late Modern English involved a semantic expansion of the types of proper nouns attested. She sought to provide support for the claim that where determiner genitives became possible with nouns lower on the animacy scale, noun modifiers at the same time gradually featured more animate types of nouns. The restriction to Late Modern English appears primarily motivated by knowledge about the history of the determiner genitive (see, e.g., Rosenbach Reference Rosenbach2002).

In this issue, Breban & De Smet are prompted by the specific restrictions imposed, including the exclusion of examples such as York Minster and Christmas day, which, as fixed expressions, do not alternate with determiner genitives, but which were used well before 1650. They investigate the presence of and changes to proper noun modifiers across the entire history of English. Vartiainen is the first to investigate the development of a related construction, (proper) nouns modifying adjectives, e.g. ice-cold, baby-soft, Einstein-smart.

2.6 Proper noun modifiers and genre

Koptjevskaja-Tamm & Rosenbach (Reference Koptjevskaja-Tamm and Rosenbach2005) identified news as a genre in which proper noun modifiers were particularly prolific, and Rosenbach (Reference Rosenbach2007) subsequently focused her corpus investigation on this genre. Breban (Reference Breban2018) specified that news headlines in particular often made use of proper noun modifiers because of their condensed form, without prepositions or other additional marking. The association of proper noun modifiers and written news text matches up with the more general connection between noun modifiers and written genres (Biber & Gray Reference Biber and Gray2011, Reference Biber and Gray2016). Koptjevskaja-Tamm (Reference Koptjevskaja-Tamm, Börjars, Denison and Scott2013) and Breban (Reference Breban2018) also draw attention to ad hoc proper noun modifiers in informal, everyday speech, e.g. university students talking about Masja courses to denote ‘courses taught by Maria Koptjevskaja-Tamm’.

In this issue, Ström Herold & Levin contribute the corpus-based study of a different written genre, non-fiction, and put forward that different genres may favour proper noun modifiers denoting different types of entities, i.e. organisations in non-fiction versus places in news texts. Breban & De Smet discuss to what extent the focus on formal written genres in the historical study of proper noun modifiers affected recognition of their presence in much earlier stages of English.

3 Different perspectives represented in this issue

3.1 Alternation of proper noun modifiers and genitives: variationist and semantic–pragmatic perspectives

Rosenbach's article ‘On the (non-)equivalence of constructions with determiner genitives and noun modifiers in English’ is situated in a variationist tradition. Her aim is to unpack the differing interpretations of equivalence in theoretical and variationist linguistics. She defines variationist equivalence as involving descriptive meaning (Cruse Reference Cruse2000) in choice contexts at usage level rather than at system level and discusses what this entails in the case of determiner genitives and (proper) noun modifiers. She argues against criticism by Breban (Reference Breban2018), Schlücker (Reference Schlücker, Ackermann, Simon and Zimmer2018) and Breban, Kolkmann & Payne (this issue), while underlining how semantic–pragmatic and variationist approaches can benefit from one another.

In their article ‘The impact of semantic relations on grammatical variation: An experimental study of proper noun modifiers and determiner genitives’, Breban, Kolkmann & Payne report on an experimental investigation into the semantic relations associated with the determiner genitive and proper noun modifier constructions. They use a bottom-up approach deriving semantic relations from paraphrases given by the participants. Their main conclusion is that none of the relations distinguished are categorically associated with one of the constructions, but that different relations are probabilistically associated with the two constructions. They argue that, for their data, semantic relations are more instructive than animacy-based distinctions, but warn against relying solely on researchers’ intuitions when assigning semantic relations to examples.

3.2 Languages in contrast: perspectives from translation and theory

The Obama presidency, the Macintosh keyboard and the Norway fiasco: English proper noun modifiers and their German and Swedish correspondences’ brings in expertise from translation studies. Ström Herold & Levin conduct the first large systematic corpus study assessing the differences and similarities between English, German and Swedish proper noun modifiers. They use a new parallel corpus, LEGS. A main advantage of the corpus is that it contains three versions (original and two translations) of every example. The texts in the corpus are all popular non-fiction texts, diversifying the range of genres investigated. They find that proper noun modifiers are more frequent in English originals than in translations, and in English compared to German and Swedish both in originals and in translations. German and Swedish employ more explicit strategies such as prepositional phrases. The fact that the less explicit and more condensed proper noun modifier construction is used when translating more explicit constructions into English shows that translation direction and type of structure can trump general observations that translations favour explication over implication. Where the literature so far has focused on semantic types of proper nouns, Ström Herold & Levin draw attention to the prominence of acronymic proper nouns in their corpus.

Alexiadou contrasts English (and other Germanic languages) with Greek. Her article ‘Proper name compounds: A comparative perspective’ is a theoretical exploration of the syntactic status of compounds and proper nouns. Her starting point is the observation that where English proper nouns occur in deverbal synthetic root compounds (e.g. Kerry supporter, Luther year), Greek does not allow this, despite the grammaticality of such compounds with common nouns. Instead a genitival construction has to be used. Within a Distributional Morphology framework, Alexiadou argues that proper names in English are referential (have ‘rigid designation’) in the absence of an explicit determiner through D-n merger, in contrast to Greek where an explicit determiner is obligatory to achieve this. Combined with the proposal that compounding involves incorporation in Greek but phrasal movement in English, this explains the different behaviours of proper nouns and common nouns in Greek compounds but not in English.

3.3 Historical change: quantitative and qualitative perspectives

Breban & De Smet set out to find the earliest proper noun modifiers in their article ‘How do new grammatical patterns emerge? The origins and development of the English proper noun modifier construction’. They use a mix of quantitative and qualitative analyses and visualisations to identify precursor constructions and possible paths of development. They conclude that new grammatical patterns develop gradually, combining multiple existing patterns and exploiting ambiguities and areas of overlap. They advocate a data-driven approach combining macro and micro analysis and warn against imposing generalisations based on theorising on historical change.

In the final article in this issue, ‘From twig-skinny to Kate Moss skinny: Expressing degree with common and proper nouns’, Vartiainen proposes a construction grammar analysis for (proper) nouns added to adjectives for intensification conveying very high degree. He argues that it is the construction that coerces the degree meaning of the (proper) noun, but also places pragmatic restrictions on the noun in particular which has to be relevant to the property and, in the case of the proper noun, a paragon. Vartiainen thus provides support for the inclusion of pragmatic information as part of constructional specification (cf. Cappelle Reference Cappelle, Depraetere and Salkie2017). Vartiainen seeks to provide evidence for entrenchment of the construction by looking at increases in token and type frequency and hapax legomena in historical data from the COHA corpus. The occurrence of proper nouns appears to be facilitated by the introduction of human common nouns in the construction. Vartiainen links the developments with changes in word formation patterns in the English noun phrase in general.

4 Perspectives on proper noun modifiers

This special issue brings together new theoretical and empirical studies on proper noun modifiers, showcasing new data, a variety of methods and highlighting how a single phenomenon sparks different but related interests in different subfields. What is noteworthy in one field might not be deemed special or interesting in another, or findings might seem contradictory when talking at cross-purposes. Rosenbach's contribution to this issue deals with such a case, where interpretations of key concepts differ in unobvious but significant ways across different subdisciplines. This special issue was an opportunity to create dialogue beyond subdisciplines, theories and methods. We want to end this introductory article by pointing out some connections and parallel conclusions across the different contributions.

Rosenbach makes a case for cross-fertilisation between variationist and other approaches. Theoretical approaches can help define and operationalise the scope of alternations, as illustrated by Breban, Kolkmann & Payne for the role and operationalisation of semantic relations in the genitive alternation. Translation studies such as Ström Herold & Levin's provide an insight into possible alternation as perceived by highly skilled language users. It is interesting to note that, in both Ström Herold & Levin's translation research and Breban, Kolkmann & Payne's experimental study, unexpected findings, including non-proper noun modifier constructions that were translated as proper noun modifiers in English (Ström Herold & Levin) and incongruent semantic interpretations (Breban, Kolkmann & Payne), provide keen insights into the delimitation of different variants. It is suggested that the relation between proper noun modifiers and determiner genitives is motivated and complicated by the fact that proper noun modifiers appear to have developed out of Old English (morphologically marked) genitive expressions (Breban & De Smet). They are thus historically a part of the different genitival constructions that emerged after the reduction of nominal inflections, providing further support for their inclusion in (historical) variationist studies. The analysis of English proper noun modifiers as referential items, central to Rosenbach's variationist equivalence proposal (2007, this issue), is also an important part of the theoretical analysis of Greek and English proper nouns in Alexiadou.

The two diachronic articles both indicate a historical link with Old English compounds. However, whereas Breban & De Smet propose a direct link between Old English compounds with proper nouns and the present-day English proper noun modifier construction, Vartiainen shows that degree compounds with proper nouns are a later development. The occurrence of proper nouns in the construction is a twentieth-century phenomenon and mainly involves proper nouns denoting persons. Plausible sources for the occurrence are the expansion of types of common nouns to those referring to humans, e.g. baby-soft, as well as the occurrence of human proper nouns modifying nouns. Proper nouns modifying adjectives could be a further ‘syntactic expansion’ (Himmelmann Reference Himmelmann, Bisang, Himmelmann and Wiemer2004) of the proper noun modifier construction. Breban & De Smet include the distinction between onomastic and non-onomastic noun phrases as a variable in their diachronic study. The source constructions of the proper noun modifier constructions are argued to be strongly onomastic. Breban, Kolkmann & Payne's experiment shows a strong association of the proper noun modifier construction and a ‘name relation’, e.g. N is named PN or N is named after PN. This can be taken as evidence of persistence (Hopper Reference Hopper, Traugott and Heine1991) of the source constructions in the present-day English construction.

Finally, several articles touch on the question of proper noun modifiers and genre. Breban & De Smet do not restrict their historical data to a particular genre and find that in early Modern English certain novel types of proper noun modifiers are found in less formal written texts, such as diaries and private letters. Ström Herold & Levin find that proper noun modifiers denoting organisations outnumber those denoting place in their popular non-fiction texts. A systematic investigation of different genres and a detailed investigation of proper noun modifiers in ‘headlinese’ are still outstanding. Another open question (Koptjevskaja-Tamm Reference Koptjevskaja-Tamm, Börjars, Denison and Scott2013; Rosenbach this issue; Breban, Kolkmann & Payne this issue) concerns the role played by the discourse status of the referent in the choice between determiner genitive and proper noun modifier. It is suggested that where noun phrases with determiner genitives often introduce new referents, noun phrases with proper noun modifiers refer to already activated referents. These questions offer opportunities for future perspectives on proper noun modifiers.

Footnotes

Tine Breban gratefully acknowledges the support of the AHRC (Leadership Fellowship AH/N002911/1).

References

Biber, Douglas & Clark, Victoria. 2002. Historical shifts in modification pattern with complex noun phrase structures. In Fanego, Teresa, López-Couso, María-José & Pérez-Guerra, Javier (eds.), English historical syntax and morphology: Selected papers from 11 ICEHL, Santiago de Compostela, 7–11 September 2000, 4366. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Biber, Douglas & Gray, Bethany. 2011. Grammatical change in the noun phrase: The influence of written language use. English Language and Linguistics 15(2), 223–50.Google Scholar
Biber, Douglas & Gray, Bethany. 2016. Grammatical complexity in academic English. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Biber, Douglas, Grieve, Jack & Iberri-Shea, Gina. 2009. Noun phrase modification. In Rohdenburg, Günter & Schlüter, Julia (eds.), One language, two grammars? Differences between British and American English, 182–93. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Breban, Tine. 2018. Proper names used as modifiers: A comprehensive functional analysis. English Language and Linguistics 22(3), 381401.Google Scholar
Cappelle, Bert. 2017. What's pragmatics doing outside constructions? In Depraetere, Ilse & Salkie, Raphael (eds.), Semantics and pragmatics: Drawing a line, 115–51. Amsterdam: Springer.Google Scholar
Cruse, Alan D. 2000. Meaning in language: An introduction to semantics and pragmatics. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Giegerich, Heinz J. 2015. Lexical structures: Compounding and the modules of grammar. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.Google Scholar
Günther, Christine. 2019. A difficult to explain phenomenon: Increasing complexity in the prenominal position. English Language and Linguistics, 23(3), 645–70. [Published online 2018]Google Scholar
Himmelmann, Nikolaus P. 2004. Lexicalization and grammaticization: Opposite or orthogonal? In Bisang, Walter, Himmelmann, Nikolaus P. & Wiemer, Björn (eds.), What makes grammaticalization? A look from its fringes and its components, 2142. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Hopper, Paul J. 1991. On some principles of grammaticization. In Traugott, Elizabeth Closs & Heine, Bernd (eds.), Approaches to grammaticalization, vol. 1: Theoretical and methodological issues, 1735. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Koptjevskaja-Tamm, Maria. 2009. Proper-name compounds in Swedish between syntax and lexicon. Rivista di Linguistica / Italian Journal of Linguistics 21, 119–48.Google Scholar
Koptjevskaja-Tamm, Maria. 2013. A Mozart sonata and the Palme murder: The structure and uses of proper-name compounds in Swedish. In Börjars, Kersti, Denison, David & Scott, Alan (eds.), Morphosyntactic categories and the expression of possession, 253–90. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Koptjevskaja-Tamm, Maria & Rosenbach, Anette. 2005. On the fuzziness of nominal determination. MS, University of Stockholm and University of Düsseldorf. https://freelancehaven.weebly.com/uploads/5/0/1/1/5011326/fuzziness_of_nominal_determination.pdf (accessed January 2019).Google Scholar
Rijkhoff, Jan. 2002. The noun phrase. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Rosenbach, Anette. 2002. Genitive variation in English: Conceptual factors in synchronic and diachronic studies. Berlin and New York: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Rosenbach, Anette. 2006. On the track of noun + noun constructions in Modern English. In Houswitschka, Christoph, Knappe, Gabriele & Müller, Anja (eds.), Anglistentag 2005 Bamberg: Proceedings of the conference of the German Association of University Teachers of English, 543–57. Trier: Wissenschaftlicher Verlag Trier.Google Scholar
Rosenbach, Anette. 2007. Emerging variation: Determiner genitives and noun modifiers in English. English Language and Linguistics 11(1), 143–89.Google Scholar
Rosenbach, Anette. 2010. How synchronic gradience makes sense in the light of language change (and vice versa). In Traugott, Elizabeth C. & Trousdale, Graeme (eds.), Gradience, gradualness and grammaticalization, 149–79. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Schlücker, Barbara. 2013. Non-classifying compounds in German. Folia Linguistica 47(2), 449–80.Google Scholar
Schlücker, Barbara. 2018. Genitives and proper name compounds in German. In Ackermann, Tanja, Simon, Horst & Zimmer, Christian (eds.), Germanic genitives, 275–99. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Teyssier, J. 1968. Notes on the syntax of the adjective in modern English. Lingua 20, 225–49.Google Scholar
Zifonun, Gisela. 2010a. Possessive Attribute im Deutschen. Deutsche Sprache 38, 124–52.Google Scholar
Zifonun, Gisela. 2010b. Von Bush administration zu Kohl-Regierung: Englische Einflüsse auf deutsche Nominalkonstruktionen? In Scherer, Carmen & Holler, Anke (eds.), Strategien der Integration und Isolation nicht-nativer Einheiten und Strukturen, 165–82. Berlin: De Gruyter.Google Scholar