Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-lj6df Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-10T17:12:10.695Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Identifying transmission routes of Streptococcus pneumoniae and sources of acquisitions in high transmission communities

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 August 2017

B. M. ALTHOUSE*
Affiliation:
Institute for Disease Modeling, Bellevue, WA, USA University of Washington, Seattle, WA, USA New Mexico State University, Las Cruces, NM, USA
L. L. HAMMITT
Affiliation:
Center for American Indian Health and International Vaccine Access Center, Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health, Baltimore, MD, USA
L. GRANT
Affiliation:
Center for American Indian Health and International Vaccine Access Center, Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health, Baltimore, MD, USA
B. G. WAGNER
Affiliation:
Institute for Disease Modeling, Bellevue, WA, USA
R. REID
Affiliation:
Center for American Indian Health and International Vaccine Access Center, Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health, Baltimore, MD, USA
F. LARZELERE-HINTON
Affiliation:
Center for American Indian Health and International Vaccine Access Center, Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health, Baltimore, MD, USA
R. WEATHERHOLTZ
Affiliation:
Center for American Indian Health and International Vaccine Access Center, Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health, Baltimore, MD, USA
K. P. KLUGMAN
Affiliation:
Bill and Melinda Gates Foundation, Seattle, WA, USA
G. L. RODGERS
Affiliation:
Bill and Melinda Gates Foundation, Seattle, WA, USA
K. L. O'BRIEN
Affiliation:
Center for American Indian Health and International Vaccine Access Center, Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health, Baltimore, MD, USA
H. HU
Affiliation:
Institute for Disease Modeling, Bellevue, WA, USA
*
*Author for correspondence: B. M. Althouse, Institute for Disease Modeling, Bellevue, WA, USA; University of Washington, Seattle, WA, USA; New Mexico State University, Las Cruces, NM, USA. (Email: balthouse@idmod.org)
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Identifying the transmission sources and reservoirs of Streptococcus pneumoniae (SP) is a long-standing question for pneumococcal epidemiology, transmission dynamics, and vaccine policy. Here we use serotype to identify SP transmission and examine acquisitions (in the same household, local community, and county, or of unidentified origin) in a longitudinal cohort of children and adults from the Navajo Nation and the White Mountain Apache American Indian Tribes. We found that adults acquire SP relatively more in the household than other age groups, and children 2–8 years old typically acquire in their own or surrounding communities. Age-specific transmission probability matrices show that transmissions within household were mostly seen from older to younger siblings. Outside the household, children most often transmit to other children in the same age group, showing age-assortative mixing behavior. We find toddlers and older children to be most involved in SP transmission and acquisition, indicating their role as key drivers of SP epidemiology. Although infants have high carriage prevalence, they do not play a central role in transmission of SP compared with toddlers and older children. Our results are relevant to inform alternative pneumococcal conjugate vaccine dosing strategies and analytic efforts to inform optimization of vaccine programs, as well as assessing the transmission dynamics of pathogens transmitted by close contact in general.

Type
Original Papers
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BYCreative Common License - NCCreative Common License - SA
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/), which permits non-commercial re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the same Creative Commons licence is included and the original work is properly cited. The written permission of Cambridge University Press must be obtained for commercial re-use.
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2017

INTRODUCTION

Identifying the transmission routes of Streptococcus pneumoniae (SP) among individuals of differing ages is a long-standing question in pneumococcal epidemiology and is relevant to vaccine disease prevention efforts [Reference Flasche1]. Previous studies have hypothesized that toddlers are the infectious reservoir for the pneumococcus, typically acquiring SP at daycare and subsequently transmitting it to their siblings and parents [Reference Sá-Leão2]. However, recent work has suggested that infants may be reservoirs of infection themselves due to the long duration of nasopharyngeal (NP) SP colonization [Reference Bogaert, de Groot and Hermans3, Reference Lipsitch4]. Duration of SP carriage and contact patterns are factors that affect transmission and thus are important in mathematical modeling of pneumococcal transmission in populations and for designing optimal vaccine programs.

Here we analyze pneumococcal carriage data at the household and community level, collected in a series of detailed studies on the Navajo Nation and White Mountain Apache Tribal lands, after a decade of pneumococcal conjugate vaccine (PCV) use [Reference Scott5, Reference Mosser6]. We identify the potential routes of transmission between infants, toddlers, children, and their parents both inside and outside households, and their key drivers.

METHODS

NP colonization and prevalence

Data used in the current analysis have been described and analyzed elsewhere [Reference Scott5, Reference Mosser6]. Briefly, the data come from a prospective, longitudinal, observational cohort study of Navajo and White Mountain Apache families living on reservations in the southwest USA. Data were collected from March 2006 to March 2008. PCV was first used in these communities from 1997 to 2000 during a phase III, community randomized trial, and then was incorporated into the routine infant immunization schedule in late 2000. Parents were recruited at well-child or ill visits at Indian Health Service (IHS) clinics. Families were included if at least one parent was a member of the Navajo Nation or White Mountain Apache tribe, the family's home was on or near the Navajo Nation or White Mountain Apache Tribal lands, at least one child in the household was under 9 years old, and at least two persons in the household were willing to participate in the study for the 6-month time period. Enrolled families were visited monthly for a 6-month period (i.e. seven visits). NP swab samples and carriage risk factor questionnaires were collected at each visit. A total of 1074 individuals in 300 households were recruited in the study in five service units: Chinle, Fort Defiance, Gallup, Shiprock, and Whiteriver. Service units are regions across the Navajo Nation defined by the US Department of Health and Human Services. The service units span the Navajo Nation in North East Arizona and North West New Mexico. Each service unit is further divided into multiple communities. Therefore, household, community, and service units form the geographical hierarchy of this study, and we use this information to explore transmission at each level.

NP specimens were obtained with Dacron swabs, using methods described elsewhere, and stored in 1 ml of skim milk, tryptone, glucose, and glycerin (STGG) transport medium [Reference Millar7]. A 100 µl aliquot of each NP specimen in STGG was inoculated onto trypticase soy agar with 5% sheep blood and gentamicin (Becton Dickinson); pneumococci were isolated and identified in the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) Respiratory Diseases Branch Streptococcus Laboratory, using methods described elsewhere [Reference Millar8]. Serotypes were identified from a single colony using the Quellung reaction. Polymerase chain reaction [Reference Millar8] or a Quellung reaction with specific factor sera [Reference Melnick, Thompson and Beall9] was used to distinguish 6A from 6C among carriage isolates originally identified as 6A.

Definitions and statistical methods

An acquisition event was defined as NP colonization in an individual who was not colonized at the previous visit, was colonized with other serotypes at the preceding visits, or was colonized with the same serotype at visits before the immediately preceding one. We attempted to find the source of transmission by searching NP colonization of other individuals carrying the same serotype at the previous visit at expanding radii: within household, outside household but within the same community, outside community but within the same service unit, then across all service units. Serotypes not seen in any of the four levels of hierarchy were classified as new importation events. If multiple individuals in the same area were identified as transmission sources, we consider them as equal contributions and count them as sources. For each new acquisition, the household, community, service unit, and exact ages of both transmitter and transmitee were available.

We calculated the percent of pneumococcus acquisition (transmission events) at each geographic level, and estimate the source of the serotypes of different age groups: 0–10 months (infants), 10 months to 2 years (toddlers), yearly until 5 years, 5–8 years, and >15 years (Fig. 1). The lower two age strata were chosen to address the specific roles of infants with limited mobility vs. young children (see Supplementary Fig. S1) and older children and adults were combined for parsimony. As each child was followed with monthly visits within a 6-month period, when normalizing across multiple visits, we estimated a child's age to be approximately the age at third visit.

Fig. 1. Percentage of subjects with positive NP carriage of SP by age in all seven visits, with 95% confidence intervals.

Transmission events were defined by identification of identical serotypes present in two individuals over two consecutive study visits. We begin looking for identical serotypes in other members of the household; if none are found, we look in the same community, then in the same service unit, then in a different service unit. If no potential sources are found, we consider it an importation. To compare transmission across age groups, we calculated age-specific transmission probability matrices at each geographic level. We wished to estimate the per-pair probability of transmission, and thus calculated the number of transmission events by the number of potential contacts between two age groups. Assuming a given geographic level has n divisions and the population of each division k is N k (with individuals of age i in community k denoted as N k,i ), the age-specific transmission probability r ij between age group i and j was calculated based on total number of acquisitions between the two age groups A ij (where A i,j is the total number of transmissions from age i to age j, with A i,j  ≠ A j,i ) divided by the maximal possible number of pairs of individuals between the two age groups C ij , i.e. r ij  = (A ij /C ij ). The pairs of individuals at each geographic level C ij is calculated as follows:

$$C_{ij}^{{\rm household}} = \left\{ {\matrix{ {\Sigma _{k = 1}^{n^{{\rm household}}} N_{k,i}N_{k,j}} \hfill & {{\rm if}\;i \ne j} \hfill \cr {\Sigma _{k = 1}^{n^{{\rm household}}} N_{k,i}(N_{k,j} - 1)/2} \hfill & {{\rm if}\;i = j} \hfill \cr}} \right.$$
$$C_{ij}^{{\rm community}} = \left\{ {\matrix{ {\Sigma _{k = 1}^{n^{{\rm community}}} N_{k,i}N_{k,j} - C_{ij}^{{\rm household}}} \hfill & {{\rm if}\;i \ne j} \hfill \cr {\Sigma _{k = 1}^{n^{{\rm community}}} N_{k,i}(N_{k,j} - 1)/2 - C_{ij}^{{\rm household}}} \hfill& {{\rm if}\;i = j} \hfill \cr}} \right.$$
$${C_{ij}^{{\rm site}} = \left\{ {\matrix{ {\Sigma _{k = 1}^{n^{{\rm site}}} N_{k,i}N_{k,j} - C_{ij}^{{\rm household}} - C_{ij}^{{\rm community}}} &{{\rm if}\;i \ne j} \hfill \cr {\Sigma _{k = 1}^{n^{{\rm site}}} N_{k,i}(N_{k,j} - 1)/2 - C_{ij}^{{\rm household}} - C_{ij}^{{\rm community}}} \hfill & {{\rm if}\;i = j} \hfill}} \right..}$$

Note C ij  = C ji , and that A i,j can be considered the total unique serotypes shared from age i to age j. Proportion tests were used to compare differences in proportion of acquisition events between age groups across households or sites, and binomial proportion confidence intervals were calculated to assess uncertainty in the estimated transmission probabilities and to test statistical significance of the estimates. The number of ‘successful trials’ was the number of potential acquisition events between age groups (A ij ) and the ‘number of trials’ was the total potential number of contacts by age group (C ij ). We note the confidence intervals were calculated under the assumption that observations of carriage in the same individual and across individuals were independent.

RESULTS

Over the study period, 6399 NP specimens were collected from 999 individuals (Table 1 shows the age distribution of study subjects) who completed the study; SP was isolated from 2294 NP specimens. Figure 1 shows the percentage of subjects with NP carriage of SP by age strata. As reported in previous findings [Reference Scott5], children under 5 years of age have a high prevalence (over 50%) of pneumococcal carriage. Infants under 10 months of age have similar prevalence as in other age categories, in accordance with other high pneumococcal burden settings [Reference Hill10Reference Ben-Shimol12]. The overall carriage prevalence is similar to that of the pre- and early PCV7 eras in these communities [Reference O'Brien13].

Table 1. Summary of pneumococcal acquisition

Table shows the breakdown of SP acquisition by geography, within age strata. Binomial confidence intervals are presented. We note that we are omitting 21 8–15 year olds who contributed 35 transmission events from the detailed table.

A total of 1235 SP acquisition events were identified – an average of 1·24 per person (Table 1). The highest probability of acquisition was seen in those between 2 and 8 years of age, with an average of two acquisitions per person over the 6-month period. The probability of acquisition was similar for all child age groups, ranging from 1·47 per person in those under 10 months of age to 2·04 per person for those 2–3 years of age. Both the carriage prevalence (10·8%) and new acquisition events in adults (0·42 per person) were low. Previous analyses of these data explored the role of children for introducing, transmitting, and sustaining pneumococcal strains in the household. We now extend those analyses to address in more detail the age-specific roles of individuals, and the geographic level of strain transmission and introduction in the community.

Figure 2 shows the proportion of the acquisitions by source for each age group. Adult acquisitions originated from within the household more commonly than they did from the study population as a whole (34·5% vs. 24%, P = 0·02) or than from the 5 to 8 years old group (18·6 percent units greater, P < 0·0001). Children in the 5–8 years age group were significantly more likely to acquire SP in their own service unit than the other age groups (9 percent units greater, P = 0·001). In general, importation events (events with no identified source) were rare, ranging from 1·7% of novel acquisitions in those above 15 years of age to 5·2% in those 4–5 years of age.

Fig. 2. Acquisition sources by age group. Figure shows the percentage of SP acquired within an individual's household, their community, their service unit (SU), another SU, or with no discernible source. Binomial confidence intervals are plotted as whiskers.

The patterns of potential transmission routes varied greatly by age group. Figure 3 shows the numbers of potential contacts (C ij ) within households, community and site by age group. Few households have more than one child in a defined age strata (the diagonal of the matrix), and there are relatively higher numbers of contacts between toddlers and infants (3–4 years old to 2–3 and 2–3 to 10 months to 2 years). Similarly, within the community, 2–3 year olds contact 10 months to 2 years frequently and 10 months to 2 years contact 0–10 months frequently.

Fig. 3. Number of contacts across age groups. Figure shows the number of potential contacts between age groups within the household, community, and service unit.

Figure 4 shows the proportion of potential contacts in an age-pair (i.e., 3–4 to 2–3 year olds) that resulted in SP transmission. Examining these proportions reveals several patterns: first, those community members >15 years old are involved in much less transmission than those in younger age groups, both within household and community. Second, within household, transmission is dominated by older to younger siblings with the highest probabilities of infection from 4–5 to 3–4 years. Third, children under 8, especially older children, preferentially transmit to people of the same age within the same community and site. This assortative mixing behavior with age is consistent with findings from contact pattern studies [Reference Mossong14], and is likely due to children interacting within childcare or classroom settings.

Fig. 4. Proportion of contacts that result in SP transmission between ages. Figure shows the transmission probabilities of SP by age groups from individuals on the Y-axis to individuals on the X-axis within the household, community, or service unit (multiple communities). Transmission probabilities are calculated as the proportion of potential contacts in an age pair (i.e. 3–4 to 2–3 year olds) that resulted in transmission. Transmission events that were not statistically significant are plotted in small italics.

Transmission of SP from colonized mother to infant is a likely source of early colonization. In this study, the mother–infant transmissions are rarely seen for both inside and outside households (Fig. 4), similar to other studies [Reference Darboe15, Reference Gratten16]. Within the household also, transmission from infant to mother is more frequent than mother to infant, also consistent with other studies [Reference Shiri17].

DISCUSSION

Our study infers sources of SP transmission across ages and local geographies. We find markedly different transmission routes and sources of SP by age group. For infants under 10 months, despite having a high prevalence of SP colonization, the direction of transmission is most likely from older siblings rather than other infants or parents. In general, we were confident in the possible sources of acquisition, especially at the household or community level, because in most acquisition events, a single source was identified (see Supplementary Fig. S2).

Previous analyses using the same data echo this, suggesting that older children are more likely to introduce SP into the household and transmit to younger children and adults [Reference Mosser6]. Based on the above findings, we further breakdown the age groups and indirectly identify the transmission events and sources of SP colonization. We find that the sources of infection vary greatly across age and local geography (i.e. within household, community, or service unit). These results have important implications for understanding potential reservoirs of SP, revising PCV vaccination schedules [Reference Flasche1], mathematical modeling of SP transmission dynamics, the potential interaction of SP and other co-infecting pathogens [Reference Weinberger18, Reference Hébert-Dufresne and Althouse19], and perhaps most importantly, for drawing conclusions about the critical community size necessary for SP persistence [Reference Black20, Reference Althouse and Scarpino21], which is a critical feature for understanding what vaccine programs need to achieve to accrue the greatest impact from these life-saving vaccines.

While contact transmission studies are difficult to conduct due to a myriad of logistical and ethical reasons [Reference Mossong14], previous studies have not indicated toddlers as important in respiratory transmission [Reference Kiti22Reference Béraud24], nor have identified specific per-pair transmission probabilities by 1 year age groups. The canonical contact tracing study – the Polymod study – suggests older children attending daycare and preschool have higher numbers of contacts (as compared with those under 4 years old) and thus are primary drivers of transmission [Reference Mossong14]. If toddlers are one of the primary drivers of transmission, as suggested here or as shown in other studies of acquisition in high carriage prevalence areas [Reference Abdullahi25, Reference Tigoi26], future studies and vaccination efforts should target these groups to eliminate reservoirs of SP.

Of specific importance is the question of whether PCV schedules providing a reduced number of doses would be effective in maintaining suppression of carriage and disease achieved through PCV schedules with a greater number of doses [Reference Flasche1]. Consideration has been given to a ‘1 + 1’ schedule in which one dose of PCV is administered early in infancy and a booster is administered at 9–12 months of age. Depending on the effectiveness of a single priming PCV dose for preventing carriage of SP – an open question in pneumococcal biology – high transmission in children who fail to receive a booster or receive it at a delayed age could drive ongoing transmission of vaccine-type SP, resulting in persistence of disease. Future work should evaluate the effects of one dose of PCV on carriage in the first year of life, as well as assessing the timing of the booster dose and its potential ramifications on transmission in young age groups.

A useful tool for investigating novel PCV schedules is mathematical modeling of SP transmission. Biologically realistic models can be constructed with various age groups and used to test potential long-term outcomes of reduced-dose schedules. Importantly, these models can be used to inform the design of field-based studies, allowing them to focus firmly on the variables that are most likely to result in a change in vaccine performance. In this way, the vaccine development process gains efficiency and confidence that we have answered the critical questions and can act on the results for program design. Key to the accurate characterization of these models is the pathogen transmission rates between age groups. The results presented here will be useful to modelers wishing to capture SP, and potentially other respiratory pathogen transmission dynamics. Perhaps most importantly, they will provide an additional benchmark for sensitivity analyses when examining the underlying assumptions present in a mathematical model.

There are several potential drivers for the differences observed in transmission probability across age groups. Differences in the prevalence of carriage across age groups (though as seen in Fig. 1 the prevalence is nearly identical across ages), differences in infectiousness (in terms of transmitting carriage to others) across the age groups, and differences in susceptibility across age groups could all explain between age variability and should be explored in further detail.

We found that over 80% of SP transmissions occurred within the same service unit, suggesting SP is likely self-persisting within these service units. Importantly, the population of the Navajo Nation and White Mountain Apache reservation totals 173 667 and 13 409 in the US 2010 Census (15 167 and 1693 children under 5; 15 391 and 1336 for children between 5 and 10 years old, respectively), suggesting that the population size for SP to persist –– the so-called critical community size for transmission – might be small, and that vaccine coverage across communities should be not only high but also uniform. Heterogeneous coverage might create pockets of persistence leading to difficulty in clearing the remaining SP vaccine types.

Another open question in pneumococcal epidemiology is the extent of, and mode of action of, interaction between SP and other respiratory pathogens, such as influenza or respiratory syncytial virus (RSV) [Reference Weinberger18, Reference Althouse and Scarpino21]. Our study did not examine transmission of other pathogens, and thus we cannot determine if the younger aged transmission of SP is related to coinfection. On the one hand, perhaps coinfection with influenza or RSV enhances bacterial shedding of SP and thus increases transmission. On the other, reduced influenza or RSV incidence in older children may reduce SP shedding and thus transmission. Quantification of the effects of SP and other respiratory pathogens on transmission remains a relevant avenue for further research.

Our study has several limitations. First, we do not have genetic sequences of SP isolates from this study; a full set of characterizations would have allowed us to confirm directionality of transmission. However, the diversity of pneumococcal serotypes present in the study population as well as the chronological order of the infections strongly suggests the order of infections. Looking at the number of identified sources per subject with new acquisitions at different levels, we could track back to a single source of infection mostly at the household level, which gives a more confident representation of transmission (see Supplementary Fig. S2); but because there are multiple potential transmission sources outside the household and because we consider them contributors with equal probability, the uncertainty in who-infected-whom begins to increase at the community and service unit level. Given as the absolute numbers of potential contacts increases as we move to community and service unit levels, we may be seeing serotype matches by chance, thus overestimating the transmission probabilities in these levels. Nonetheless, it is still indicative of transmission.

Second, the findings from any high pneumococcal transmission setting study, like this one, may not be representative of the transmission dynamics from low prevalence settings. The relatively high prevalence of SP in the study setting may affect the transmission dynamics across age groups. However, we believe our results will be applicable to other settings of similar socio-demographic characteristics (e.g. use of daycare) insofar as those drive pneumococcal transmission dynamics. Third, due to the enrollment criteria (infants, children, and their parents), we do not have colonization data on children between 9 and 15 years of age. Future studies should investigate their role in transmitting SP, which may be considerable in households with large numbers of children. Fourth, false-negative NP swabs or undetected multiple colonizations will obscure the estimates of the probabilities of transmission resulting in both over- or underestimation, depending on in whom (transmitter or receiver) the sample was missing or misclassified.

Despite these limitations, our study demonstrates the differences in location of SP acquisition across age groups and highlights the key role of toddlers and older children in transmission of SP, especially toddlers (10 months to 3 years old) – a group previously considered to be less important in the overall transmission of the pneumococcus. Future work can use these results to investigate reservoirs of SP as well as to estimate herd effects under different PCV dosing schedules.

SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S095026881700125X.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

The authors express their appreciation to the families from the Navajo and White Mountain Apache communities who participated actively in the studies. The authors also gratefully acknowledge the dedicated efforts of the Center for American Indian Health field staff who collected these data over many years and the Indian Health Service, which provided clinic space as well as clinical and laboratory support. The authors also gratefully acknowledge input from Maria Deloria Knoll and the work by Jennifer Scott and Jon Mosser on preceding analyses. BGW, BMA, and HH were supported by Bill and Melinda Gates through the Global Good Fund. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript. The findings and conclusions in this paper are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect the views of the Indian Health Service.

DECLARATION OF INTEREST

No conflicts of interest.

Footnotes

These authors contributed equally to this work.

References

REFERENCES

1. Flasche, S, et al. The potential for reducing the number of pneumococcal conjugate vaccine doses while sustaining herd immunity in high-income countries. PLoS Medicine 2015; 12(6): e1001839.Google Scholar
2. Sá-Leão, R, et al. High rates of transmission of and colonization by Streptococcus pneumoniae and haemophilus influenzae within a day care center revealed in a longitudinal study. Journal of Clinical Microbiology 2008; 46(1): 225234.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
3. Bogaert, D, de Groot, R, Hermans, P. Streptococcus pneumoniae colonisation: the key to pneumococcal disease. The Lancet Infectious Diseases 2004; 4(3): 144154.Google Scholar
4. Lipsitch, M, et al. Estimating rates of carriage acquisition and clearance and competitive ability for pneumococcal serotypes in Kenya with a Markov transition model. Epidemiology 2012; 23(4): 510.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
5. Scott, JR, et al. Impact of more than a decade of pneumococcal conjugate vaccine use on carriage and invasive potential in Native American communities. Journal of Infectious Diseases 2012; 205(2): 280288.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
6. Mosser, JF, et al. Nasopharyngeal carriage and transmission of Streptococcus pneumoniae in American Indian households after a decade of pneumococcal conjugate vaccine use. PLoS ONE 2014; 9(1): e79578.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
7. Millar, EV, et al. Nasopharyngeal carriage of Streptococcus pneumoniae in Navajo and White Mountain Apache children before the introduction of pneumococcal conjugate vaccine. The Pediatric Infectious Disease Journal 2009; 28(8): 711716.Google Scholar
8. Millar, EV, et al. Pre-and post-conjugate vaccine epidemiology of pneumococcal serotype 6c invasive disease and carriage within Navajo and White Mountain Apache communities. Clinical Infectious Diseases 2010; 51(11): 12581265.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
9. Melnick, N, Thompson, TA, Beall, BW. Serotype-specific typing antisera for pneumococcal serogroup 6 serotypes 6a, 6b, and 6c. Journal of Clinical Microbiology 2010; 48(6): 23112312.Google Scholar
10. Hill, PC, et al. Nasopharyngeal carriage of Streptococcus pneumoniae in Gambian infants: a longitudinal study. Clinical Infectious Diseases 2008; 46(6): 807814.Google Scholar
11. Turner, P, et al. A longitudinal study of Streptococcus pneumoniae carriage in a cohort of infants and their mothers on the Thailand-Myanmar border. PLoS ONE 2012; 7(5): e38271.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
12. Ben-Shimol, S, et al. Pneumococcal nasopharyngeal carriage in children! 5 years of age visiting the pediatric emergency room in relation to pcv7 and pcv13 introduction in southern Israel. Human Vaccines & Immunotherapeutics 2016; 12(2): 268276.Google Scholar
13. O'Brien, KL, et al. Predictors of pneumococcal conjugate vaccine immunogenicity among infants and toddlers in an American Indian pncrm7 efficacy trial. Journal of Infectious Diseases 2007; 196(1): 104114.Google Scholar
14. Mossong, J, et al. Social contacts and mixing patterns relevant to the spread of infectious diseases. PLoS Medicine 2008; 5(3): e74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
15. Darboe, MK, et al. The dynamics of nasopharyngeal Streptococcus pneumoniae carriage among rural Gambian mother-infant pairs. BMC Infectious Diseases 2010; 10(1): 1.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
16. Gratten, M, et al. Colonisation of haemophilus influenzae and Streptococcus pneumoniae in the upper respiratory tract of neonates in Papua New Guinea: primary acquisition, duration of carriage, and relationship to carriage in mothers. Neonatology 1986; 50(2): 114120.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
17. Shiri, T, et al. Dynamics of pneumococcal transmission in vaccine-naive children and their HIV-infected or HIV-uninfected mothers during the first 2 years of life. American Journal of Epidemiology 2013; 178(11): 16291637.Google Scholar
18. Weinberger, DM, et al. Association between respiratory syncytial virus activity and pneumococcal disease in infants: a time series analysis of us hospitalization data. PLoS Medicine 2015; 12(1), e1001776.Google Scholar
19. Hébert-Dufresne, L, Althouse, BM. Complex dynamics of synergistic coinfections on realistically clustered networks. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 2015; 112(33): 1055110556.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
20. Black, FL. Measles endemicity in insular populations: critical community size and its evolutionary implication. Journal of Theoretical Biology 1966; 11(2): 207211.Google Scholar
21. Althouse, BM, Scarpino, SV. Asymptomatic transmission and the resurgence of Bordetella pertussis . BMC Medicine 2015; 13(1): 146.Google Scholar
22. Kiti, MC, et al. Quantifying age-related rates of social contact using diaries in a rural coastal population of Kenya. PLoS ONE 2014; 9(8): e104786.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
23. Grijalva, CG, et al. A household-based study of contact networks relevant for the spread of infectious diseases in the highlands of Peru. PLoS ONE 2015; 10(3): e0118457.Google Scholar
24. Béraud, G, et al. The French connection: the first large population-based contact survey in France relevant for the spread of infectious diseases. PLoS ONE 2015; 10(7): e0133203.Google Scholar
25. Abdullahi, O, et al. Rates of acquisition and clearance of pneumococcal serotypes in the nasopharynges of children in Kilifi district, Kenya. Journal of Infectious Diseases 2012; 206(7): 10201029.Google Scholar
26. Tigoi, CC, et al. Rates of acquisition of pneumococcal colonization and transmission probabilities, by serotype, among newborn infants in Kilifi district, Kenya. Clinical Infectious Diseases 2012; 55(2): 180188.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Fig. 1. Percentage of subjects with positive NP carriage of SP by age in all seven visits, with 95% confidence intervals.

Figure 1

Table 1. Summary of pneumococcal acquisition

Figure 2

Fig. 2. Acquisition sources by age group. Figure shows the percentage of SP acquired within an individual's household, their community, their service unit (SU), another SU, or with no discernible source. Binomial confidence intervals are plotted as whiskers.

Figure 3

Fig. 3. Number of contacts across age groups. Figure shows the number of potential contacts between age groups within the household, community, and service unit.

Figure 4

Fig. 4. Proportion of contacts that result in SP transmission between ages. Figure shows the transmission probabilities of SP by age groups from individuals on the Y-axis to individuals on the X-axis within the household, community, or service unit (multiple communities). Transmission probabilities are calculated as the proportion of potential contacts in an age pair (i.e. 3–4 to 2–3 year olds) that resulted in transmission. Transmission events that were not statistically significant are plotted in small italics.

Supplementary material: File

Althouse et al supplementary material

Figures S1-S2

Download Althouse et al supplementary material(File)
File 184.9 KB