Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-fbnjt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-10T08:40:03.446Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Do people like to discuss politics? A study of citizens’ political talk culture

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 January 2023

Rüdiger Schmitt-Beck*
Affiliation:
Department of Political Science, University of Mannheim, Mannheim, Germany
Manuel Neumann
Affiliation:
Department of Political Science, University of Mannheim, Mannheim, Germany
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

As deliberative democracy is gaining practical momentum, the question arises whether citizens’ attitudes toward everyday political talk are congruent with this ‘talk-centric’ vision of democratic governance. Drawing on a unique survey we examine how German citizens view the practice of discussing politics in everyday life, and what determines these attitudes. We find that only a minority appreciates talking about politics. To explain these views, we combine Fishbein and Ajzen’s Expectancy-Value Model of attitudes toward behaviors with perspectives from research on interpersonal communication. Individuals’ interest in politics emerges as the only relevant political disposition for attitudes toward everyday political talk. Its impact is surpassed and conditioned by conflict orientations and other enduring psychological dispositions, as well as contextual circumstances like the closeness of social ties and the amount of disagreement experienced during conversations. The beneficial effect of political interest dwindles under adverse interpersonal conditions. The social dimension of everyday political talk thus appears to outweigh its political dimension.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of European Consortium for Political Research

Introduction

In recent years, the normative vision of ‘talk-centric’ deliberative democracy has gained considerable practical momentum. Slowly but steadily, democratic will-formation seems to acquire a ‘deliberative timbre’ (Bächtiger and Parkinson, Reference Bächtiger and Parkinson2019: 5). As emphasized in rather general terms by early advocates (Barber, Reference Barber1984; Manin, Reference Manin1987; Habermas, Reference Habermas1996), and fleshed out more precisely by recent systemic theorizing (Mansbridge, Reference Mansbridge and Macedo1999; Chambers, Reference Chambers, Parkinson and Mansbridge2012; Neblo, Reference Neblo2015: 17–25; Tanasoca, Reference Tanasoca2020), deliberative democratic decision-making ought to be anchored in citizens’ discussions with one another. Ultimately, it is ordinary people’s political talk in their day-to-day lifeworld – the informal conversations about political themes, casually held in homes, at workplaces, or at neighborhood parties (Conover and Miller, Reference Conover, Miller, Bächtiger, Dryzek, Mansbridge and Warren2018) – that should serve as the mainspring of deliberative politics and touchstone of its democratic character (Schmitt-Beck, Reference Schmitt-Beck2022).

Almond and Verba’s classic congruence thesis suggests that in order to work such a model of democracy presupposes an ‘allegiant’ political culture (Almond and Verba, Reference Almond and Verba1963, 21–22). Deliberative democracy needs a talk culture (Steiner et al., Reference Steiner, Bächtiger, Spörndli and Steenbergen2004: 4) that is favorable to the pivotal role assigned to political discussions in general, and political talk between ordinary citizens in particular. The members of a society that advances in the direction of a deliberative democracy should hold positive attitudes toward political talk as the procedural, institutional and normative centerpiece of this project (Chambers, Reference Chambers2003). Otherwise, they can be expected to opt out or resist more deliberative processes and may become apathetic or even alienated. Ultimately, incongruence of citizens’ attitudes with structures of will-formation and decision-making designed along deliberative democratic maxims may hollow out the practice of this model of democracy and undermine its legitimacy.

Despite this crucial role as the cultural underpinning of deliberative democracy, citizens’ attitudes toward everyday political talk have only recently begun to attract interest among scholars of this model of democracy (Scudder, Reference Scudder2020: 113–132; Jennstål et al., Reference Jennstål, Uba and Öberg2021). Our analysis ties in with this novel strand of research by asking a very simple but fundamental question: How favorable or unfavorable are citizens’ attitudes toward the practice of everyday political talk, and what determines these attitudes?

We start with a brief discussion of the key role ascribed to everyday political talk in deliberative democracy, and its implications from the perspective of political culture research. Drawing on the Conversations of Democracy study, a unique face-to-face survey specially designed to examine German citizens’ everyday political talk (Grill et al., Reference Grill, Schmitt-Beck and Metz2018), we describe citizens’ attitudes toward this category of behavior. We find only a minority expressing positive attitudes. To understand these views we draw on Fishbein and Ajzen’s Expectancy-Value Model of attitudes toward behaviors (Fishbein and Ajzen, Reference Fishbein and Ajzen1975). To flesh out this generic perspective for everyday political talk, we conceive attitudes on this specific category of behavior as resulting from ‘the push-pull of relational goals and outcomes versus political goals and outcomes’ (Pennington and Winfrey, Reference Pennington and Winfrey2021: 102; italics by authors).

Normative theorizing about deliberative democracy typically adopts a purely political understanding of everyday political talk as an activity whose essence consists in the expression, exchange, and mutual consideration of views about public policy. Drawing on scholarship on interpersonal communication we transcend this narrow perspective. We acknowledge that it may also have a ‘phatic’ dimension (Thornbury and Slade, Reference Thornbury and Slade2006: 19–20; Senft, Reference Senft, Senft, Östman and Verschueren2009) that concerns its function of managing the social bonds between those communicating with one another (Rosenberg, Reference Rosenberg1954; Eliasoph, Reference Eliasoph1998; Conover et al., Reference Conover, Searing and Crewe2002; Cramer, Reference Cramer2004; Mutz, Reference Mutz2006; Watzlawick et al., Reference Watzlawick, Beavin and Jackson2011: 29–52). We accordingly examine the interplay of individual political dispositions, on the one hand, and two kinds of social factors, on the other: interpersonally relevant psychological dispositions and interpersonal contexts.

The talk culture of deliberative democracy

Deliberative democrats embrace a ‘rhetorical’ notion of citizenship (Kock and Villadsen, Reference Kock and Villadsen2017) that conceives citizens as free and equal contributors to an inclusive and encompassing process of interconnected discussions about political problems, goals, and solutions, that permeate society and feed into formal procedures of political decision-making (Habermas, Reference Habermas1996; Parkinson and Mansbridge, Reference Parkinson and Mansbridge2012). Deliberative politics thus presupposes substantial involvement of the citizenry at large in order to qualify as truly democratic (Schmitt-Beck, Reference Schmitt-Beck2022).

In their seminal study of political culture, Almond and Verba (Reference Almond and Verba1963: 21–22) developed the notion that the proper functioning and stability of a system of governance depends on a ‘congruent’ political culture. Congruence implies favorable feelings and positive evaluations of the basic make-up of the polity on the part of its citizenry. When this condition is fulfilled, the political culture is ‘allegiant’ to its structure and expected to lend it support and thereby stabilize it. Indifference gives rise to apathy, whereas negative views are assumed to alienate citizens and bear the risk of resistance against the political system and its concomitant destabilization. Recent research supports this claim (Welzel and Inglehart, Reference Welzel, Inglehart, Haerpfer, Bernhagen, Inglehart and Welzel2009; Claassen, Reference Claassen2020).

This suggests that advocates of deliberative democracy should be concerned about the congruence between culture and structure. Transforming democracy toward a greater centrality of political discussion presupposes an ‘allegiant’ political culture in order to succeed. This concerns in particular what has been labeled citizens’ ‘talk culture’ (Steiner et al., Reference Steiner, Bächtiger, Spörndli and Steenbergen2004: 4), that is, people’s beliefs, attitudes, and values with regard to this particular mode of political engagement. Congruence of citizens’ talk culture with the institutional, normative, and practical features of deliberative democracy requires largely positive attitudes toward engagement in everyday political talk. For the case of Germany, our study examines to what extent this crucial cultural prerequisite of deliberative democracy is fulfilled. It addresses two research questions:

  • RQ1: How do citizens evaluate the practice of everyday political talk?

  • RQ2: What determines these attitudes?

Explaining citizens’ attitudes toward everyday political talk

Attitudes toward everyday political talk are a special case of attitudes toward behavior, an orientation defined as ‘the degree to which a person has a favorable or unfavorable evaluation or appraisal of the behavior in question’ (Ajzen, Reference Ajzen1991: 188). According to Fishbein and Ajzen’s Expectancy-Value Model (Fishbein and Ajzen, Reference Fishbein and Ajzen1975), such attitudes are derived from persons’ ‘readily accessible or salient beliefs about the likely consequences of a contemplated course of action’ (Ajzen and Albarracín, Reference Ajzen, Albarracín, Ajzen, Albarracín and Hornik2007: 5). More precisely, an individual’s attitude toward a behavior reflects the positive or negative valences of outcomes and experiences associated with this behavior in her mind, weighted with the subjective likelihood of these outcomes’ occurrence when the behavior is performed, and totaled across all outcomes that are salient for her when forming this attitude. If cherished outcomes dominate and are expected to occur with a high likelihood, the resulting attitude toward the behavior in question will be positive. But it will be rather negative if adverse outcomes appear more likely than valued ones.

This model offers a generic account of the mechanics that lead to attitudes toward certain behaviors. It assumes that personality traits, values, or socio-demographic characteristics can serve as dispositions that influence attitudes toward behaviors by biasing perceptions of their implications and outcomes. It also acknowledges that these beliefs may be responsive to the contexts within which activities are performed (Fishbein and Ajzen, Reference Fishbein and Ajzen1980: 34–35). The model is agnostic, however, with regard to the specific background factors that give rise to the outcome beliefs that are relevant for any particular form of behavior. It is therefore necessary to flesh out this perspective with domain-specific hypotheses that spell out in detail which dispositional and contextual factors can be expected to be of relevance for the behavior of interest (Ajzen and Albarracín, Reference Ajzen, Albarracín, Ajzen, Albarracín and Hornik2007).

Regarding citizens’ attitudes toward everyday political talk, three sets of factors appear potentially relevant: (i) individual dispositions toward the domain of politics, (ii) psychological dispositions pertaining to social interaction, and (iii) characteristics of the interpersonal contexts within which conversations take place. We assume that they affect attitudes toward everyday political talk directly, but also indirectly in a complex layered web of conditioning relationships (cf. Figure 1).

Figure 1. Hypothesized interplay between political dispositions, interpersonally relevant psychological dispositions, and interpersonal contexts as backgrounds of attitudes toward everyday political talk.

Note: Angular boxes symbolize political factors and rounded boxes social factors.

Political dispositions

The first of these categories follows naturally from deliberative democrats’ preoccupation with everyday political talk as an activity whose essence consists in the expression, exchange, and mutual consideration of views about politics. In this perspective, its meaning for those performing it is believed to emanate exclusively from its topical content. Accordingly, dispositions acquired during individuals’ political socialization can be expected to affect their views concerning the prospect of discussing politics (Beck and Jennings, Reference Beck and Jennings1982). Most prominent among potentially relevant political dispositions is interest in politics which can be understood as a stable ‘expectation that engaging with political content […] in the future will turn out to be rewarding’ (Prior, Reference Prior2019: 4). It should give rise to the belief that political conversations are gratifying experiences because they offer the opportunity to occupy oneself intensely with a subject matter deemed intrinsically attractive. Accordingly, it should lead to favorable attitudes toward everyday political talk.

Passionate stances with regard to certain political worldviews, ideological camps or parties could also allow political discussions to appear as gratifying. Persons with strong attitudes concerning directional alignments like partisanship and ideology may see them as welcome opportunities to affirm their political identity, express it to others, and persuade them of the superiority of their views. As a generalized self-confidence in political matters, internal political efficacy (Craig and Maggiotto, Reference Craig and Maggiotto1982) might also nurture the expectation that political conversations offer rewards. We accordingly hypothesize:

H1a: Favorable political dispositions (high interest in politics, strong partisanship, strong ideological leaning, high internal political efficacy) lead to more positive attitudes toward everyday political talk.

The social dimension of everyday political talk

The other two categories acknowledge that political conversations are a specific mode of interpersonal communication and presuppose the participation of fellow citizens as communication partners. For a more complete understanding of people’s views about everyday political talk it is crucial to take into account that it implies interactions with other people. When discussing politics, individuals encounter each other not only in the political role of citizens but also as social beings. Alongside the cognitive dimension of the themes, issues, and questions that constitute conversations’ substance, they are inevitably also concerned with the affective dimension of interlocutors’ linkages to each other (Watzlawick et al., Reference Watzlawick, Beavin and Jackson2011: 29–52). Scholarship of interpersonal communication has long recognized that its meaning cannot be fully understood by focusing exclusively on its topical content. Drawing on seminal work by anthropologist Bronisław Malinowski (Reference Malinowski, Ogden and Richards1923) and linguist Roman Jakobson (Reference Jakobson and Sebeok1960) it has adopted the term ‘phatic’ to denote conversations’ distinctly social function of managing the bonds between their participants (Thornbury and Slade, Reference Thornbury and Slade2006: 19–20; Senft, Reference Senft, Senft, Östman and Verschueren2009).

When evaluating the prospect of engaging in everyday political talk, individuals can accordingly be expected to envisage this kind of behavior not only, and perhaps not even primarily, as a political activity, but also in terms of the social interactions that it necessarily entails (Rosenberg, Reference Rosenberg1954: 351). Studies of everyday political talk have not paid much attention to this dimension. What little research there is, however, suggests that certain social features of political talk might indeed be at odds with the instrumental political function assigned to it by deliberative democratic theory. In his seminal study of political apathy, Rosenberg already argued that many people may ‘be blocked from talking […] in behalf of their political beliefs out of fear of losing friends, alienating neighbors, endangering marriages, jeopardizing their positions in groups, losing business, jeopardizing their jobs, endangering production in their plants, facing community pressures, or exposing their feelings of self-esteem to threat’ (Rosenberg, Reference Rosenberg1954: 354).

Over the years, other (mostly qualitative) studies voiced similar concerns. From varying vantage points they converged on the diagnosis that discussing politics with one’s peers can be a quite stressful and adverse experience that many people find uncomfortable and prefer to avoid (Scheuch, Reference Scheuch, Scheuch and Wildenmann1965; Schudson, Reference Schudson1997; Eliasoph, Reference Eliasoph1998; Conover et al., Reference Conover, Searing and Crewe2002; Cramer, Reference Cramer2004; Mutz, Reference Mutz2006). Two aspects, in particular, have been highlighted by this literature: that political views may be considered too personal and private to be shared with others, and that discussing such matters may lead to unpleasant or even painful controversies. This suggests that attitudes toward everyday political talk can be expected to be affected by psychological dispositions of relevance for individuals’ interactions with other people. The fact that this behavior is an inherently social activity also implies that it inevitably takes place in specific social settings. The circumstances of these interpersonal contexts might likewise impinge on people’s attitudes toward this activity.

Interpersonally relevant psychological dispositions

Political psychology has in recent years made significant progress in revealing how political behavior is affected by features of citizens’ intra-individually highly stable, but inter-individually variable personalities (Caprara and Vecchione, Reference Caprara, Vecchione, Huddy, Sears and Levy2013). These personalities are conceived as multi-dimensional internal structures of an enduring nature that are to a large extent genetically rooted and thus heritable. They influence individuals’ behavior in all walks of life and therefore can be expected to leave an imprint also in the realm of politics (Mondak, Reference Mondak2010; Cawvey et al., Reference Cawvey, Hayes, Canache and Mondak2017). Research on political talk has thus far mainly referred to the comprehensive ‘Big Five’ framework of personality traits (Hibbing et al., Reference Hibbing, Ritchie and Anderson2011; Gerber et al., Reference Gerber, Huber, Doherty and Dowling2012). For the purpose of our study, we need a more fine-grained approach that specifically refers to personality facets of direct relevance for individuals’ relationships to other persons. Without claiming to be exhaustive, such interpersonally relevant psychological dispositions entail individuals’ need to belong, conflict orientations, subjective discussion competence, and social trust.

Need to belong is ‘an inherent, primary human need to form and maintain stable interpersonal bonds’ (Morey and Yamamoto, Reference Morey and Yamamoto2020: 83). It is a powerful human desire to experience enduring interactions of mutual concern with other persons (Baumeister, Reference Baumeister, Van Lange, Kruglanski and Higgins2011). For individuals with a high need to belong, the potentially controversial character of discussions about politics might appear threatening. A strong need to belong can therefore be expected to be associated with rather negative attitudes toward everyday political talk.

Individuals’ conflict orientation is ‘a specific type of approach/avoidance motivation; it is a stable personality trait centered around how people experience and react to conflict – whether they are excited by arguments, uncomfortable when others fight in public, or happy to handle a disagreement face to face’ (Sydnor, Reference Sydnor2019: 29). It is defined by the opposing poles of conflict aversion (dislike of political confrontation and argument) and conflict seeking (feelings of excitement and enjoyment about dispute and contention). Since everyday political talk may lead to conflictive experiences, conflict aversion can be expected to lead to unfavorable attitudes toward this behavior, whereas conflict seeking should stimulate positive views (Mutz, Reference Mutz2006; Neblo, Reference Neblo2015: 129–144).

Topically focused discussions in general, and political discussions in particular, demand certain skills, among them a basic understanding of the thematized subject matters and some measure of articulateness and eloquence. People differ in these skills, and in their confidence to command them. People endowed with high subjective competence to discuss politics (Rubin et al., Reference Rubin, Martin, Bruning and Powers1993) should consider everyday political talk an activity in which they can excel and that is accordingly rewarding. Those with little self-confidence, by contrast, face the ‘[t]hreat of ego-deflation’, associated with ‘the prospect of revealing factual ignorance or committing gross logical errors’, and consequently a desire ‘to avoid the feeling of defeat, abashment, humiliation, or other discomfiture’ (Rosenberg, Reference Rosenberg1954: 353). Accordingly, they should view political discussions rather negatively.

Trusting an actor involves one’s confidence that this actor will deliberately refrain from behaving in ways that violate one’s interests, even if the actor could do so without fearing sanctions. In the case of social trust, this actor is the anonymous aggregate of people outside one’s intimate core networks. This orientation has often been described as a lubricant that enables smooth cooperation between the members of societies (Nannestad, Reference Nannestad2008) – a beneficial function that can be assumed to extend to everyday political talk as well. If people expect fellow citizens to exploit opportunities to cause them harm, they will associate a high risk of humiliating or otherwise painful experiences with interactions that involve revealing personal opinions, such as one’s views about politics. Trusting persons, by contrast, should feel unhampered by such worries and accordingly develop more positive attitudes (Wyatt et al., Reference Wyatt, Katz and Kim2000).

The following formal hypothesis summarizes these expectations:

H1b: Favorable psychological dispositions (low need to belong, conflict seeking, high discussion competence, strong social trust) lead to more positive attitudes toward everyday political talk.

Interpersonal contexts

Ordinary citizens’ talk about public affairs typically takes place between the members of social networks (Huckfeldt and Sprague, Reference Huckfeldt and Sprague1995; Tanasoca, Reference Tanasoca2020). Since these networks define contextual circumstances for political conversations, their attributes should play an important role for attitudes toward this behavior. Two characteristics appear particularly important: the relationships between network members (Morey et al., Reference Morey, Eveland and Hutchens2012; Schmitt-Beck and Grill, Reference Schmitt-Beck and Grill2020), and the amount of disagreement individuals encounter when discussing politics with these persons (Nir, Reference Nir, Kenski and Jamieson2017). With regard to both, our expectations run counter to normative claims emphasized by theorists of deliberative democracy.

Concerning relationships between interlocutors, the distinction between strong ties and weak ties appears crucial. Strong ties are characterized by intimacy, mutual regard, and positive affect (Straits, Reference Straits1991). They circumscribe political talk of a private nature, involving close associates in protected spaces, such as people’s homes, and encompass two types of relationships: kinship bonds with spouses and other family members (Zuckerman et al., Reference Zuckerman, Dasovic and Fitzgerald2007), and friendships (Fischer, Reference Fischer1982). Weak ties connect people who know each other, although not necessarily well, and are outside of each other’s circles of intimates, such as co-workers, neighbors, and other acquaintances (Huckfeldt and Sprague, Reference Huckfeldt and Sprague1995). Since they involve interactions in more public settings like workplaces, bars or over the proverbial garden fence, they are often considered more valuable from a deliberative democratic point of view (Huckfeldt et al., Reference Huckfeldt, Johnson and Sprague2004; Tanasoca, Reference Tanasoca2020). However, in view of the challenging character of discussions about politics, strong ties can be expected to offer a more comfortable context (Schudson, Reference Schudson1997; Morey et al., Reference Morey, Eveland and Hutchens2012) which, consequently, should be associated with more favorable attitudes toward this activity.

Advocates of deliberative democracy praise discussions between those holding opposing views as the most constructive mode to address the pluralism of societal interests and value orientations (Thompson, Reference Thompson2008: 502; Sunstein, Reference Sunstein2003). Yet, deliberative democrats’ appreciation of disagreeable encounters seems to go against the grain of how ordinary people want to structure their social life. Many find disagreement a source of tension, with a potential to disrupt highly valued social relationships, and accordingly uncomfortable (Mutz, Reference Mutz2006). Studies indicate that citizens try to avoid political conversations when they anticipate controversies (Gerber et al., Reference Gerber, Huber, Doherty and Dowling2012; Settle and Carlson, Reference Settle and Carlson2019). This suggests that attitudes toward everyday political talk should be more negative when these conversations are expected to entail experiences of political disagreement. Concern about the ‘tyranny’ of majority opinion voiced by classics like de Tocqueville (Reference De Tocqueville1990: 254–286) and Mill (Reference Mill2015: 5–54), but also modern-day spiral-of-silence theory, suggests that individuals find encounters with disagreeable views particularly unpleasant when they are held by a majority (Noelle-Neumann, Reference Noelle-Neumann1974; Hayes et al., Reference Hayes, Shanahan and Glynn2001). Envisaging such experiences should, accordingly, lead to even more negative attitudes toward this behavior.

We accordingly hypothesize:

H1c: Favorable interpersonal contexts (strong ties, no or limited political disagreement) lead to more positive attitudes toward everyday political talk.

A web of conditioning relationships

Everyday political talk needs to be ‘safely integrated into the fabric of everyday lives and relationships’ (Mutz, Reference Mutz2006: 137). It therefore requires balancing political motives with the presumably more profound desire to maintain harmonious relationships with one’s associates (Pennington and Winfrey, Reference Pennington and Winfrey2021: 100). Interpersonal motives are closely associated with individuals’ fundamental longing for affiliation, social identification, self-esteem and well-being as accepted members of their groups and communities (Zayas and Sakman, Reference Zayas and Sakman2020). They are therefore constantly activated as people lead their daily lives. Political dispositions, by contrast, pertain only to one specific domain of life that is, moreover, fairly remote from people’s lifeworld, accordingly more abstract, and often not very salient (van Deth, Reference Van Deth2000). Individuals can therefore be assumed to function as social beings first, and as zooi politikoi only second. As noted by Mutz, ‘[p]eople tend to care more about social harmony in their immediate face-to-face personal relationships than about the larger political world’ (Mutz, Reference Mutz2006: 106). This general perspective bears two implications for our research. First of all, it suggests that social factors exert an overall stronger impact on persons’ attitudes toward everyday political talk than their political dispositions. We accordingly hypothesize (not visualized in Figure 1):

H1d: Psychological dispositions and interpersonal contexts are more relevant for attitudes toward everyday political talk than political dispositions.

In addition, over and above this merely additive specification in terms of competing direct effects, the general assumption that interpersonal factors are more powerful than political ones leads us to expect that they affect attitudes toward everyday political talk also indirectly by conditioning the influence of political dispositions. Mondak et al. (Reference Mondak, Hibbing, Canache, Seligson and Anderson2010), among others, have argued that the impact of distal personality features on political orientations should indeed be seen as primarily interactive. Supporting this claim, a small body of research on various modes of political communication has demonstrated such moderating effects of conflict orientations (Mutz, Reference Mutz2006; Dalisay, Reference Dalisay2012; Testa et al., Reference Testa, Hibbing and Ritchie2014; Sydnor, Reference Sydnor2019; Wolak, Reference Wolak2022), and the need to belong (Eck et al., Reference Eck, Schoel and Greifeneder2017). We accordingly hypothesize that individuals’ psychological dispositions condition the effects of political dispositions on attitudes toward everyday political talk. If persons value social rewards higher than political gratifications, then strong political interest, intense directional passions, and marked internal efficacy should most clearly nurture the expectation that everyday political talk is associated with rewarding experiences when their need to belong is weak, when they enjoy rather than abhor conflict, when they feel competent to stand their ground in discussions, and when they trust their fellow citizens strongly. By contrast, under conditions of a strong need to belong, marked conflict avoidance, little subjective discussion competence, and weak social trust, the dreaded social costs of such conversations should suppress the beneficial effects of political dispositions. Put formally:

H2: Under favorable psychological dispositions political dispositions are more relevant for attitudes toward everyday political talk than under unfavorable psychological dispositions.

The same line of thought leads us to expect that the impact of dispositions is moderated by the contextual circumstances of everyday political talk as ultimate exogeneous factors. Mondak et al. (Reference Mondak, Hibbing, Canache, Seligson and Anderson2010; see also Mondak, Reference Mondak2010; Cawvey et al., Reference Cawvey, Hayes, Canache and Mondak2017) have made a strong case that effects of psychological dispositions should be conceived as conditional on the situational settings of behaviors. To the extent that features of contexts resonate with personality facets, individuals differently endowed with these features can be expected to respond in dissimilar ways to identical social situations. In other words, variable contextual conditions can be expected to moderate and thereby cause heterogeneity in the effects of psychological dispositions. They ‘contribute to patterns of political behavior, but the expression of these effects will often be contingent on the situation’ (Mondak et al., Reference Mondak, Hibbing, Canache, Seligson and Anderson2010: 85). It seems highly plausible to expect similar situational contingencies also for political dispositions.

Importantly, in bearing with our previous hypotheses, contextually induced heterogeneity should work in opposite directions for psychological and political dispositions. We assume that political dispositions affect attitudes toward everyday political talk more strongly when interpersonal settings are favorable (analogous to H2). Conversely, the impact of political dispositions should be muted under more adverse contextual circumstances. The contrary pattern should emerge for psychological dispositions. Their impact should be boosted, especially when they resonate with these contextual features. Conflict orientations, for instance, should become more influential when conversations are envisaged as disagreeable (Mutz, Reference Mutz2006; Sydnor, Reference Sydnor2019). Thinking further along these lines also suggests an even more complex three-way interaction. It implies that the contextual circumstances affect not only the direct effects of psychological and political dispositions, but also the conditioning impact of the former on the effects of the latter.

H3a: In favorable interpersonal contexts political dispositions are more relevant for attitudes toward everyday political talk than in unfavorable contexts.

H3b: In favorable interpersonal contexts psychological dispositions are less relevant for everyday political talk than in unfavorable contexts.

H4: In favorable interpersonal contexts the moderating impact of psychological dispositions on the relevance of political dispositions for attitudes toward everyday political talk is weaker than in unfavorable contexts.

How do citizens evaluate the practice of everyday political talk?

To answer our research questions we draw on data from the Conversations of Democracy (CoDem) study, a high-quality face-to-face survey, based on a random sample of voting-age respondents and specially designed to examine German citizens’ everyday political talk.Footnote 1 Respondents’ attitudes toward everyday political talk were elicited with a complex instrument that started with the question: ‘How much do you like or dislike talking about politics with members of your family?’ Analogous questions were asked for friends, and then ‘acquaintances such as neighbors and co-workers’. Responses were registered on five-point scales ranging from ‘like very much’ to ‘dislike very much’. The same set of questions was repeated twice, introduced by primes that stressed increasing amounts of political disagreement: first, ‘when differences of opinion emerge during conversations,’ and second, ‘when during conversations a clear majority of the other participants rejects your opinion’. Thus, respondents’ views of everyday political talk were recorded for nine different interpersonal contextual circumstances of this behavior, resulting from a cross-classification of three types of relationships between conversation partners with three scenarios of disagreement experiences while talking with them.

Figure 2 displays the joint distribution of responses to these questions across all contextual conditions. The pattern is remarkably symmetric. Somewhat less than four out of ten responses indicate indifference. The rest is divided roughly equally between liking and disliking, with a slight edge on the negative side. This has important implications for the cultural foundations of deliberative democracy. In a congruent political culture, most citizens would hold positive views of the kind of behavior on which this model of democracy crucially depends. Yet, in our data only less than a third of all responses are positive, and less than 10% are very favorable. Supportive views of deliberative democracy’s central mode of citizen engagement are thus a minority phenomenon. Germans’ talk culture seems quite removed from the ideal of allegiance with the political order of deliberative democracy. Instead, the data reveal a considerable potential for apathy or even alienation, arising from indifferent or opposing views toward the basic practice of everyday political talk. Why is this the case?

Figure 2. Attitudes toward everyday political talk (percent).

Note: Data are weighted (by gender, age, and city district) and stacked over all conditional circumstances within respondents (N = 13,880).

What determines citizens’ attitudes toward everyday political talk?

Dependent variable and methodological approach

As indicated above, respondents’ assessments of everyday political talk were recorded for nine different interpersonal contextual circumstances, resulting from a cross-classification of three types of relationships between conversation partners and three kinds of disagreement experiences. To turn these contextual conditions into variables whose impact on attitudes toward everyday political talk can be estimated in conjunction with respondents’ political and psychological dispositions, the survey dataset was reshaped into a long format. It was organized in such a way that the information contained in the original questions was shifted into a generic variable indicating respondents’ like or dislike of political talk and two variables that specify relationships between interlocutors, respectively, the amount of disagreement. The dataset is stacked by disagreement nested within relationships nested within respondents, resulting in nine observations per respondent. In the linear ordinary least squares regression modelsFootnote 2 presented below, the generic like-dislike variable serves as the dependent variable, whereas the two indicators of contextual circumstances are used as independent variables.

Independent and control variables

Political dispositions: Political interest is measured by means of self-reports on a five-point scale. The indicator of partisanship takes the form of a five-point scale ranging from non-partisans to very strong party identifiers. Ideological extremity is measured by means of an 11-point left-right scale folded at the midpoint. Internal political efficacy is measured by an additive scale based on two items.

Psychological dispositions: Subjective discussion competence and need to belong are indicated by additive scales based on two items each. The latter’s coding is reversed because a low need to belong should be associated with positive attitudes toward everyday political talk. Six items were used to construct an additive scale of conflict orientations, with low values indicating conflict aversion and high values conflict seeking. For measuring social trust, an additive scale was used that combined items pertaining to respondents’ trust in acquaintances and strangers.

Interpersonal contexts: To indicate the strength of social ties a dummy variable is used that distinguishes weak ties (1 = acquaintances) from strong ties (0 = family members and friends). The amount of disagreement is indicated by two dummy variables. The first pertains to disagreement as such, the second to majority disagreement. They are coded in such a way that the estimate for the former expresses the contrast with the base condition without disagreement prime, whereas majority disagreement is contrasted with simple disagreement.

All models include the following socio-demographic controls: gender, age, immigration background, education, occupational status and economic well-being. In addition, the models control for the psychological traits need to evaluate, need for cognition, and need for cognitive closure (see Online Appendix for details of instrumentation).

Findings

The findings for model (1) in Table 1 are mostly in line with the basic hypotheses H1a, H1b, and H1c. H1a receives only partial support since political interest is the only political disposition that makes a difference in attitudes toward everyday political talk. This suggests that highly interested individuals find political talk much more rewarding than those with little or no interest, whereas partisan or ideological passions and internal efficacy have no additional impact. Therefore, these orientations will no longer be considered in the following analyses.Footnote 3 The other findings are unequivocally in line with H1b and H1c. Individuals’ conflict orientations are the overall strongest predictor in the model. Conflict seeking is associated with much more positive attitudes toward everyday political talk than conflict avoidance. The effects of social trust, discussion competence and need to belong are substantial, but weaker. The impact of interpersonal contexts is overall somewhat less pronounced, but all effects are likewise in line with our expectations. Political conversations within strong ties are viewed more positively than those within weak ties. The prospect of disagreement renders attitudes toward everyday political talk less favorable, and the most challenging scenario of having to face a disagreeable majority gives an additional push toward more negative attitudes.

Table 1. Direct effects of political dispositions, psychological dispositions, and interpersonal contexts on attitudes toward everyday political talk (unstandardized regression coefficients)

Robust standard errors (clustered by respondents) in parentheses. All variables are normalized to range 0 to 1.

# p < 0.10, *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

Comparing the incremental model fit statistics of models (3) to (6) (relative to the baseline model (2)) allows assessment of H1d. Compared to the purely political model (3), the incremental explanatory power of the complete social model (6), which entails both interpersonally relevant psychological dispositions and interpersonal contexts, is more than twice as large. The interpersonal predictors thus outweigh the political ones by a large margin. Most of this model’s superior fit is due to the psychological dispositions, however (cf. model (4)), whereas the overall impact of contextual circumstances alone (model (5)) is smaller than the explanatory power of the block of political dispositions. H1d thus receives qualified support. The analysis suggests that interpersonally relevant psychological dispositions are more important for attitudes toward everyday political talk than political dispositions, whereas contextual circumstances play a more limited role – at least in a purely additive perspective.

In addition to these direct effects, we expect social factors to affect attitudes toward everyday political talk also conditionally in a complex interplay of moderating relationships with political dispositions. To test these hypotheses for political interest (as the only political disposition of relevance) we add multiplicative interaction terms to the full model (1) displayed in Table 1. We pursue a two-track strategy that combines a more permissive with an extremely conservative approach to modeling. The first approach consists of a series of models in which each interaction effect implied in the generic conditional hypotheses is estimated separately. In the restrictive approach, all proposed interactions are examined simultaneously in a combined model. Since even simple two-way interaction models include the predictors of interest twice in the same equation they are by definition affected with considerable multicollinearity (Brambor et al., Reference Brambor, Clark and Golder2006). This erects high hurdles for achieving conventional levels of statistical significance already in the permissive models. Including key predictors multiple times, as is necessarily the case in the combined models, raises the bar even higher.

According to our first conditional hypothesis (H2), the positive effect of political interest should to some extent depend on favorable psychological dispositions. All interaction effects displayed in Figure 3 are in line with this expectation, but most of them fail to attain statistical significance. In the separate models, conflict seeking and high confidence in one’s ability to discuss politics are associated with a stronger impact of political interest, as expected. High political interest thus appears to induce more favorable attitudes toward everyday political talk primarily among individuals that are confident in their ability to stand their ground in such discussions and do not shy away from political conflicts. But these effects are not robust when subjected to the tough test of the restrictive combined model.

Figure 3. Moderation of effect of political interest by psychological dispositions.

Note: Entries are two-way interaction effects from multiple linear regression models with robust standard errors (tails indicate 95-% and 90-% confidence intervals). Empty symbols are based on permissive models, filled symbols on restrictive models. See Online Appendix for complete models.

The second set of conditional hypotheses claims that the impact of political and psychological dispositions on citizens’ views of everyday political talk is in opposite ways conditioned by the interpersonal contexts within which this activity takes place. The estimates of the interaction terms displayed in Figure 4 show for political interest and the four psychological dispositions how these associations are amplified or mitigated by weak ties in comparison to strong ties, as well as by disagreement in contrast to no disagreement, and by majority disagreement compared to simple disagreement. H3a predicts that weak ties, as well as disagreement of growing intensity, depress the positive effect of political interest. This expectation is largely supported by our data. Attitudes toward everyday political talk are considerably less responsive to differences in citizens’ political interest when the envisaged discussions take place within weak ties or entail disagreement. These patterns emerge not only in the separate models but very clearly also in the combined models. However, it makes no further difference whether disagreeable views are expressed by a majority of one’s interlocutors.

Figure 4. Moderation of effects of political interest and psychological dispositions by interpersonal contexts.

Note: Entries are two-way interaction effects from multiple linear regression models with robust standard errors (tails indicate 95-% and 90-% confidence intervals). Empty symbols are based on permissive models, filled symbols on restrictive models. See Online Appendix for complete models.

H3b posits the opposite pattern for psychological dispositions. Across the board, they should be more relevant under the more challenging conditions of weak ties and disagreeable interlocutors. With some qualifications, this expectation is also sustained. Social trust, for instance, is much more strongly associated with attitudes toward everyday political talk within weak ties. Disagreement, by contrast, does not appear to condition the role of social trust. For the need to belong, we also see interaction effects in the expected direction – less pronounced with regard to the strength of ties, but unlike social trust also pertaining to disagreement (but not majority disagreement). Concerning conflict orientations and discussion competence, the picture is also not uniform. The separate models suggest statistically meaningful conditioning effects of the strength of ties in the wrong direction. But they are not robust to the more restrictive model specification. Moderating effects in the hypothesized direction emerge for conversations that entail disagreement. Majority disagreement appears to boost these effects further, but only under permissive model specifications. Figure 3 thus suggests that the role of social trust is only conditioned by the strength of ties, whereas conflict orientations and discussion competence are primarily important when disagreement is envisaged to occur in conversations. Need to belong has weaker conditioning effects under both contextual conditions. What does not appear to matter much is the difference between the simple presence of disagreement and disagreement expressed by a majority.

The final analysis combines the perspectives of the previous ones in three-way interaction models. This erects even higher hurdles for interaction terms to achieve statistical significance. H4 posits that the deflating role of unfavorable psychological dispositions on the effect of political interest is intensified by adverse contextual circumstances, that is, for conversations within weak ties or in the presence of disagreement, especially majority disagreement. When all hypothesized three-way interactions are modeled simultaneously, none of them is statistically significant (Figure 5). But this finding should not be overrated, given the enormous complexity of these models. The separate models, by contrast, find the effect of political interest on attitudes toward everyday political talk indeed being significantly more sensitive to conflict orientations within weak ties compared to strong ties. The same pattern is detected for discussion competence. Among the conflict averse and those with little confidence in their ability to persist in discussions, strong political interest translates to a lesser extent into a positive view of everyday political talk when it is envisaged to take place outside strong ties. Regarding the moderating role of discussion competence, we also see effects in the expected direction conditional on disagreement as well as majority disagreement, but they do not attain sufficient levels of statistical significance.

Figure 5. Moderation of interactions between political interest and psychological dispositions by interpersonal contexts.

Note: Entries are three-way interaction effects from multiple linear   regression models with robust standard errors (tails indicate 95-% and 90-% confidence intervals). Empty symbols are based on permissive models, filled symbols on restrictive models. See Online Appendix for complete models.

Conclusion

The discussion about the ‘talk-centric’ deliberative model of democracy (Chambers, Reference Chambers2003: 308) has stimulated growing interest in ordinary people‘s everyday political talk (Mansbridge, Reference Mansbridge and Macedo1999; Conover and Miller, Reference Conover, Miller, Bächtiger, Dryzek, Mansbridge and Warren2018). As emphasized by Barber, deliberative democracy requires ‘widespread and ongoing participation in talk by the entire citizenry’ (Barber, Reference Barber1984: 197). Our study addressed a crucial cultural prerequisite of this kind of democracy. According to classic theorizing about political culture, political systems’ institutional and normative architectures need ‘allegiant’ political cultures in order to work and remain intact (Almond and Verba, Reference Almond and Verba1963). Accordingly, deliberative democracy requires a talk culture (Steiner et al., Reference Steiner, Bächtiger, Spörndli and Steenbergen2004: 4) that is congruent with the centrality of citizens’ political talk in its political process. Against this background, we examined how German citizens view the activity of everyday political talk. We found less than a third of the responses being positive. Negative attitudes are slightly more frequent, and the modal category consists of indifferent views. This distribution of attitudes signals a considerable potential for political apathy and even alienation (Almond and Verba, Reference Almond and Verba1963: 22) when political talk among ordinary people becomes more important and consequential in democratic will-formation and decision-making.

Two mechanisms may give rise to these risks. The first is unhappiness and estrangement about the increasing normative valuation of ‘rhetorical’ (Kock and Villadsen, Reference Kock and Villadsen2017) or even ‘deliberative’ citizenship (Doheny, Reference Doheny2007), that is, the growing public appreciation of engagement in political discussions as a desirable mode of engagement or even duty of citizens in deliberative democracy (Mayne and Geissel, Reference Mayne and Geissel2016), among those holding indifferent or even unfavorable attitudes toward this activity. Deliberative democracy may thereby lose its cultural underpinning among citizens that do not cherish the mode of activity central to its conception of how politics should work.

The second mechanism functions via political outcomes. Individuals are more likely to perform a behavior if they hold a positive attitude toward it, and less likely if their attitude is negative (Ajzen, Reference Ajzen1991). Those viewing everyday political talk positively should accordingly have a higher likelihood of actual participation in political discussions than those with indifferent or even negative attitudes. Under deliberative democracy’s talk-centric process of will-formation and decision-making, such behavioral effects may give rise to political inequality. If people’s participation in everyday political talk is correlated with specific political perspectives and viewpoints, unequal engagement may lead to unequal ‘discursive representation’ (Dryzek and Niemeyer, Reference Dryzek and Niemeyer2008), since the themes relevant to those staying silent are then unlikely to enter the agenda and their concerns less likely to find expressionFootnote 4 and influence political outcomes. To the extent that deliberative democratic decision-making ultimately originates from impulses generated in citizens’ discussions with one another (Habermas, Reference Habermas1996; Tanasoca, Reference Tanasoca2020), these perspectives’ prospects of affecting public policies would be impaired. Seeing their interests sidelined may lead to further estrangement among those disliking political discussion.

Broadly, participatory deliberative democracy is advocated as a superior mode of legitimate governance (Manin, Reference Manin1987; Cohen, Reference Cohen, Hamlin and Pettit1989; Habermas, Reference Habermas1996). However, looming behind both mechanisms – indifference or even dislike of political talk as its central modus operandi, and frustration about one’s interests not being effectively represented in its talk-centric decision-making – is the possibility that for significant segments of the mass public it may lead to the opposite outcome: estrangement and feelings of illegitimacy, rather than enhanced support for its regime norms, principles, and output (Easton, Reference Easton1975).

Building on the Expectancy-Value Model as a generic theory of attitudes toward behavior (Fishbein and Ajzen, Reference Fishbein and Ajzen1975) and domain-specific hypotheses inspired by scholarship about interpersonal communication, both generally (Watzlawick et al., Reference Watzlawick, Beavin and Jackson2011: 29–52) and specifically referring to politics (Conover et al., Reference Conover, Searing and Crewe2002; Mutz, Reference Mutz2006), we examined the interplay of political and social backgrounds of attitudes toward everyday political talk. Political interest emerged as the only political disposition of relevance. Our findings suggest that a strong interest in politics leads to the expectation that talking about this topic is a gratifying experience, which in turn affects attitudes toward this activity favorably. However, views of everyday political talk also reflect a broad variety of enduring psychological dispositions and circumstances of interpersonal contexts that refer to the ‘phatic’ function of conversations as a specific form of managing interactions between people (Thornbury and Slade, Reference Thornbury and Slade2006: 19–20; Senft, Reference Senft, Senft, Östman and Verschueren2009).

Overall, these social factors are more important for attitudes toward everyday political talk than political predictors. Specifically, conflict seeking and generalized social trust, but also individuals’ subjective competence to discuss politics and to a lesser extent also a low need to belong, render attitudes toward political talk more positive. By contrast, individuals that are highly averse to conflict, have little confidence in their ability to persist in a political discussion, distrust fellow citizens, or feel a strong desire to stay on good terms with other people seem to associate rather unpleasant experiences with everyday political talk and therefore tend to dislike it. Advocates of deliberative democracy are mainly concerned that its practice could be impaired by adverse effects of social marginalization (Young, Reference Young2000; Knops, Reference Knops2006). Our findings indicate that the egalitarian imperative of deliberative democracy might also be subverted by individuals’ variable endowment with psychological dispositions that entail heritable components (Mondak, Reference Mondak2010; Caprara and Vecchione, Reference Caprara, Vecchione, Huddy, Sears and Levy2013). Conversations about politics are also viewed less favorably if they take place within weak rather than strong ties, and when they entail disagreement, most notably majority disagreement. This suggests that attributes of political discussions that are highly valued by advocates of deliberative democracy – communications within weak ties rather than the secluded private world of strong ties (Tanasoca, Reference Tanasoca2020), and encounters with the political heterogeneity of the social world (Sunstein, Reference Sunstein2003) – are in fact detrimental to the kind of political engagement that is considered key to its vision of better and more legitimate governance.

Importantly, we even found interpersonal factors moderating the impact of interest in politics. Our analyses thus suggest that, concerning attitudes toward everyday political talk, social concerns not only outweigh but may even take precedence over political ones. To some extent, the positive role of political interest appears to presuppose comfortable social circumstances. Especially concerning the varying conditions of interpersonal contexts, its role as a driver of favorable attitudes toward everyday political talk bears features of a ‘fair weather phenomenon’. Under rather unpleasant circumstances, when conversations are envisaged to take place outside the safe haven of kinship and friendship, or when they are expected to include non-like-minded interlocutors, the impact of political interest is deflated. Under such conditions, appreciation of political discussions tends to be less pronounced even among those strongly interested in politics. Under the same conditions, these views are more responsive to psychological dispositions like most notably social trust, conflict orientations and the subjective competence to discuss politics. We also found indications that political interest translates more easily into favorable attitudes toward everyday political talk among people that enjoy rather than abhor dispute and contention, and that this conditioning effect is especially pronounced within weak ties. This observation ties in with a growing literature on the moderating role of conflict orientations in processes of political communication (Mutz, Reference Mutz2006; Dalisay, Reference Dalisay2012; Testa et al., Reference Testa, Hibbing and Ritchie2014; Sydnor, Reference Sydnor2019; Wolak, Reference Wolak2022).

Our findings suggest that in several ways, social concerns interfere with political dispositions as backgrounds of citizens’ views about everyday political talk, the mode of engagement that is crucial for the democratic element in deliberative democracy’s vision of truly legitimate governance. To the extent that attitudes toward everyday political talk lead to a higher intensity of such engagement, people endowed with certain political, and even more so psychological dispositions are advantaged, and those lacking them are disadvantaged. It is difficult to imagine how these obstacles to a more participatory and egalitarian working of deliberative democracy ‘on the ground’ could be overcome. In organized mini-publics smart design choices concerning such events’ institutional make-up can to some extent alleviate problems of this kind (Karpowitz and Mendelberg, Reference Karpowitz and Mendelberg2014). Establishing protected social zones where those burdened with disadvantages can hold ‘enclave deliberations’ restricted to people like themselves is a particularly far-reaching example of these kinds of formalized approaches (Karpowitz et al., Reference Karpowitz, Raphael and Hammond2009). But such strategies appear neither realistic nor appropriate in the unruly world of spontaneous everyday political talk. Attitudes toward this informal practice of citizens’ lifeworld are probably mainly responsive to socialization experiences in childhood and youth (Almond and Verba, Reference Almond and Verba1963: 323–374; Nolas et al., Reference Nolas, Varvantakis and Aruldoss2017). Communication practices within families are protected by norms of privacy and therefore hardly open to outside interventions from public agencies. Conscious training of discursive behavior in schools appears more promising as a strategy to affect deliberative democracy’s cultural underpinnings in favorable ways. But clearly, that can only be a long-term program. And it would affect the less important background of attitudes toward everyday political talk – political interest – more strongly (Koskimaa and Rapeli, Reference Koskimaa and Rapeli2015; Prior, Reference Prior2019: 211–262) than the dominant factors, individuals’, to a significant extent heritable, psychological dispositions.

Our study indicates several fruitful avenues for future research. Our conditional hypotheses can be translated into testable expectations about selectivities in the actual practice of everyday political talk that emerge from the interplay of contextual conditions with political (Huckfeldt and Sprague, Reference Huckfeldt and Sprague1995) and psychological dispositions (Caprara and Vecchione, Reference Caprara, Vecchione, Huddy, Sears and Levy2013: 47–48). From a theoretical point of view, our findings demonstrate the utility of the Expectancy-Value Model of attitudes toward behavior for the study of political talk. This model is a specific element of the Theory of Planned Behavior (Fishbein and Ajzen, Reference Fishbein and Ajzen1980) that aims to provide complete accounts of why individuals display certain behaviors or not. As a general model of human action, this approach has a lot more to offer to the study of citizens’ engagement in political discussions and should be utilized further. Finally, research should attend to the implications of the incongruence between deliberative democracy’s prioritization of political discussion and the rather low esteem in which this activity is held by substantial parts of the citizenry.

Supplementary material

To view supplementary material for this article, please visit https://doi.org/10.1017/S1755773922000625.

Footnotes

The authors are indebted to Simon Ellerbrock, Christian Schnaudt, and two anonymous reviewers for helpful feedback on draft versions of this paper.

1 Based on a register-based one-stage random sample, 1,600 interviews with voting age citizens were completed between 15 May and the German federal election on 24 September, 2017. Following the model of major studies of political communication in citizens’ lifeworld (Lazarsfeld et al., Reference Lazarsfeld, Berelson and Gaudet1968; Huckfeldt and Sprague, Reference Huckfeldt and Sprague1995; Huckfeldt et al., Reference Huckfeldt, Johnson and Sprague2004; Conover et al., Reference Conover, Searing and Crewe2002) the study was conducted locally. Its site was Mannheim, a city in the South of Germany characterized by the variegated social structure, economy, culture and political life of a typical mid-sized German city. The study was conducted under a grant of the German National Science Foundation DFG. For methodological details see Grill et al. (Reference Grill, Schmitt-Beck and Metz2018).

2 Since these models are cross-sectional we can, strictly speaking, not demonstrate causal relationships. Since the phenomena of interest are all of high intra-individual stability over time, panel data spanning long sections of respondents’ life cycles would be needed to identify such patterns.

3 Further analyses show that they are also not relevant in interaction models.

4 Preliminary research found not very strong, but still detectable patterns of overrepresentation and underrepresentation with regard to the political perspectives of those engaging in everyday political talk. They are more pronounced with regard to weak ties than strong ties, and suggest that citizens preferring mainstream center–left or center–right parties are somewhat underrepresented, and adherents of niche parties overrepresented among those that discuss politics. In addition, those holding leftist–liberal positions tend to be overrepresented, and those holding right–wing and populist positions underrepresented (Schmitt–Beck and Schnaudt, 2022).

References

Ajzen, I. (1991), ‘The theory of planned behavior’, Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 50(2): 179211. doi: 10.1016/0749-5978(91)90020-T CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ajzen, I. and Albarracín, D. (2007), ‘Predicting and changing behavior: a reasoned action approach’, in Ajzen, I., Albarracín, D. and Hornik, R. (eds.), Predicting and Change of Health Behavior. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum, pp. 321.10.4324/9780203937082CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Almond, G.A. and Verba, S. (1963), The Civic Culture. Political Attitudes and Democracy in Five Nations, Princeton: Princeton University Press.10.1515/9781400874569CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bächtiger, A. and Parkinson, J. (2019), Mapping and Measuring Deliberation. Towards a New Deliberative Quality, Oxford: Oxford University Press.10.1093/oso/9780199672196.001.0001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barber, B.R. (1984), Strong Democracy. Participatory Politics for a New Age, Berkeley: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Baumeister, R.F. (2011), ‘Need-to-belong theory’, in Van Lange, P.A.M., Kruglanski, A. and Higgins, E.T. (eds.), Handbook of Theories of Social Psychology, vol. 2, Los Angeles: SAGE, pp. 121140.Google Scholar
Beck, P.A. and Jennings, M.K. (1982), ‘Pathways to participation’, The American Political Science Review 76(1): 94108.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brambor, T., Clark, W.R. and Golder, M. (2006), ‘Understanding interaction models: improving empirical analyses’, Political Analysis 14(1): 6382. doi: 10.1093/pan/mpi014 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caprara, G.V. and Vecchione, M. (2013), ‘Personality approaches to political behavior’, in Huddy, L., Sears, D.O. and Levy, J.S. (eds.), Oxford Handbook of Political Psychology, Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 2358.Google Scholar
Cawvey, M., Hayes, M., Canache, D. and Mondak, J.J. (2017), ‘Personality and political behavior’, Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Politics. doi: 10.1093/acrefore/9780190228637.013.221 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chambers, S. (2003), ‘Deliberative democratic theory’, Annual Review of Political Science 6(1): 307326. doi: 10.1146/annurev.polisci.6.121901.085538 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chambers, S. (2012), ‘Deliberation and mass democracy’, in Parkinson, J. and Mansbridge, J. (eds.), Deliberative Systems. Deliberative Democracy at the Large Scale, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 5271.10.1017/CBO9781139178914.004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Claassen, C. (2020), ‘Does public support help democracy survive?’, American Journal of Political Science 64(1): 118134. doi: 10.1111/ajps.12452 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, J. (1989), ‘Deliberation and democratic legitimacy’, in Hamlin, A.P. and Pettit, P.N. (eds.), The Good Polity. Normative Analysis of the State, Oxford: Basil Blackwell, pp. 1834.Google Scholar
Conover, P.J. and Miller, P.R. (2018), ‘Taking everyday political talk seriously’, in Bächtiger, A., Dryzek, J.S., Mansbridge, J. and Warren, M. (eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Deliberative Democracy, Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 378391.Google Scholar
Conover, P.J., Searing, D.D. and Crewe, I.M. (2002), ‘The deliberative potential of political discussion’, British Journal of Political Science 32: 2162. doi: 10.1017/S0007123402000029 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Craig, S.C. and Maggiotto, M.A. (1982), ‘Measuring political efficacy’, Political Methodology 8(3): 85109.Google Scholar
Cramer, K. (2004), Talking about Politics. Informal Groups and Social Identity in American Life, Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Dalisay, F.S. (2012), ‘The spiral of silence and conflict avoidance’, Communication Quarterly 60(4): 481503. doi: 10.1080/01463373.2012.704567 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Tocqueville, A. (1990), Democracy in America, vol. 1, New York: Vintage.Google Scholar
Doheny, S. (2007), ‘Responsibility and the deliberative citizen: theorizing the acceptance of individual and citizenship responsibilities’, Citizenship Studies 11(4): 405420. doi: 10.1080/13621020701476293 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dryzek, J.S. and Niemeyer, S. (2008), ‘Discursive representation’, American Political Science Review 102(4): 481493. doi: 10.1017/s0003055408080325 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Easton, D. (1975), ‘A re-assessment of the concept of political support’, British Journal of Political Science 5: 435457. doi: 10.1017/S0007123400008309 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eck, J., Schoel, C. and Greifeneder, R. (2017), ‘Belonging to a majority reduces the immediate need threat from ostracism in individuals with a high need to belong’, European Journal of Social Psychology 47(3): 273288. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.2233 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eliasoph, N. (1998), Avoiding Politics: How Americans Produce Apathy in Everyday Life, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fischer, C.S. (1982), To Dwell among Friends. Personal Networks in Town and City, Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Fishbein, M. and Ajzen, I. (1975), Belief, Attitude, Intention and Behavior, Reading: Addison-Wesley.Google Scholar
Fishbein, M. and Ajzen, I. (1980), Understanding Attitudes and Predicting Social Behavior, Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Gerber, A.S., Huber, G.A., Doherty, D. and Dowling, C.M. (2012), ‘Disagreement and the avoidance of political discussion: aggregate relationships and differences across personality traits’, American Journal of Political Science 56(4): 849874. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-5907.2011.00571.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grill, C., Schmitt-Beck, R. and Metz, M. (2018), ‘Studying the “Conversations of Democracy”: research design and data collection’, Working Paper 173, Mannheim: MZES. https://www.mzes.uni-mannheim.de/publications/wp/wp-173.pdf Google Scholar
Habermas, J. (1996), Between Facts and Norms. Contributions to a Discourse Theory of Law and Democracy, Cambridge: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hayes, A.F., Shanahan, J. and Glynn, C.J. (2001), ‘Willingness to express one‘s opinion in a realistic situation as a function of perceived support for that opinion’, International Journal of Public Opinion Research 13(1): 4558. doi: 10.1093/ijpor/13.1.45 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hibbing, M., Ritchie, M. and Anderson, M. (2011), ‘Personality and political discussion’, Political Behavior 33(4): 601624.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huckfeldt, R., Johnson, P. and Sprague, J. (2004), Political Disagreement. The Survival of Diverse Opinions within Communication Networks, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huckfeldt, R. and Sprague, J. (1995), Citizens, Politics, and Social Communication. Information and Influence in an Election Campaign, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jakobson, R. (1960). ‘Linguistics and poetics’, in Sebeok, T.A. (ed.), Style in language, Cambridge: MIT Press, pp. 350377.Google Scholar
Jennstål, J., Uba, K. and Öberg, P.O. (2021), ‘Deliberative civic culture: assessing the prevalence of deliberative conversational norms’, Political Studies 69(2): 366389. doi: 10.1177/0032321719899036 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karpowitz, C.F. and Mendelberg, T. (2014), The Silent Sex: Gender, Deliberation, and Institutions, Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Karpowitz, C.F., Raphael, C. and Hammond, A.S. (2009), ‘Deliberative democracy and inequality: two cheers for enclave deliberation among the disempowered’, Politics & Society 37(4): 576615. doi: 10.1177/0032329209349226 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Knops, A. (2006), ‘Delivering deliberation’s emancipatory potential’, Political Theory 34(5): 594623. doi: 10.1177%2F0090591706290780 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kock, C. and Villadsen, L.S. (2017), ‘Rhetorical citizenship: studying the discursive crafting and enactment of citizenship’, Citizenship Studies 21(5): 570586. doi: 10.1080/13621025.2017.1316360 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koskimaa, V. and Rapeli, L. (2015), ‘Political socialization and political interest: the role of school reassessed’, Journal of Political Science Education 11: 141156. doi: 10.1080/15512169.2015.1016033 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lazarsfeld, P.F., Berelson, B., and Gaudet, H. (1968), The People’s Choice. How the Voter Makes Up his Mind in a Presidential Campaign, 3rd edn., New York: Columbia University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Malinowski, B. (1923), ‘The problem of meaning in primitive languages’, in Ogden, C.K. and Richards, I.A. (eds.), The Meaning of Meaning, Supplement 1, London: Kegan Paul, pp. 451519.Google Scholar
Manin, B. (1987), ‘On legitimacy and political deliberation’, Political Theory 15(3): 338368. doi: 10.1177/0090591787015003005 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mansbridge, J. (1999), ‘Everyday talk in the deliberative system’, in Macedo, S. (ed.), Deliberative Politics: Essays on Democracy and Disagreement, Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 211242.Google Scholar
Mayne, Q. and Geissel, B. (2016), ‘Putting the demos back into the concept of democratic quality’, International Political Science Review 37(5): 634644. doi: 10.1177/0192512115616269 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mill, J.S. (2015), On Liberty, Utilitarianism and Other Essays, Oxford: Oxford University Press.10.1093/owc/9780199670802.001.0001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mondak, J.J. (2010). Personality and the Foundations of Political Behavior, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mondak, J.J., Hibbing, M.V., Canache, D., Seligson, M.A. and Anderson, M.R. (2010), ‘Personality and civic engagement: an integrative framework for the study of trait effects on political behavior’, American Political Science Review 104(1): 85110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morey, A.C., Eveland, W.P. and Hutchens, M.J. (2012), ‘The “Who” matters: types of interpersonal relationships and avoidance of political disagreement’, Political Communication 29(1): 86103. doi: 10.1080/10584609.2011.641070 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morey, A.C. and Yamamoto, M. (2020), ‘Exploring political discussion motivations: relationships with different forms of political talk’, Communication Studies 71(1): 7897. doi: 10.1080/10510974.2019.1692885 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mutz, D.C. (2006), Hearing the Other Side. Deliberative versus Participatory Democracy, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nannestad, P. (2008), ‘What have we learned about generalized trust, if anything?’, Annual Review of Political Science 11(1): 413436. doi: 10.1146/annurev.polisci.11.060606.135412 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neblo, M.A. (2015), Deliberative Democracy between Theory and Practice, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nir, L. (2017), ‘Disagreement in political discussion’, in Kenski, K. and Jamieson, K.H. (eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Political Communication, Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 713730.Google Scholar
Noelle-Neumann, E. (1974), ‘The spiral of silence a theory of public opinion’, Journal of Communication 24(2): 4351. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-2466.1974.tb00367.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nolas, S.-M., Varvantakis, C. and Aruldoss, V. (2017), ‘Talking politics in everyday family lives’, Contemporary Social Science 12(1–2): 6883. doi: 10.1080/21582041.2017.1330965 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parkinson, J. and Mansbridge, J. (eds.) (2012), Deliberative Systems. Deliberative Democracy at the Large Scale, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9781139178914CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pennington, N. and Winfrey, K.L. (2021), ‘Engaging in political talk on Facebook: investigating the role of interpersonal goals and cognitive engagement’, Communication Studies 72(1): 100114. doi: 10.1080/10510974.2020.1819844 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Prior, M. (2019), Hooked. How Politics Captures People’s Interest, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Rosenberg, M. (1954), ‘Some determininants of political apathy’, Public Opinion Quarterly 18(4): 349366.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rubin, R.B., Martin, M.M., Bruning, S.S. and Powers, D.E. (1993), ‘Test of a self-efficacy model of interpersonal communication competence’, Communication Quarterly 41(2): 210220. doi: 10.1080/01463379309369880 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scheuch, E.K. (1965), ‘Die Sichtbarkeit politischer Einstellungen im alltäglichen Verhalten’, in Scheuch, E.K., and Wildenmann, R. (eds.), Zur Soziologie der Wahl, Köln: Westdeutscher Verlag, pp. 169214.10.1007/978-3-663-20250-9_6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmitt-Beck, R. (2022). ‘Prerequisites of deliberative democracy: inclusivity, publicity, and heterogeneity of German citizens’ everyday political talk’, Studies in Communication and Media 11(1): 772. doi: 10.5771/2192-4007-2022-1-7 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmitt-Beck, R. and Grill, C. (2020), ‘From the living room to the meeting hall? Citizens’ political talk in the deliberative system’, Political Communication 37(6): 832851. doi: 10.1080/10584609.2020.1760974 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmitt-Beck, R. and Schnaudt, C. (2022), ‘The representativity of deliberative democracy, contribution to the lecture series ’40 years German General Social Survey ALLBUS’, GESIS Mannheim, 19 January, 2022.Google Scholar
Schudson, M. (1997), ‘Why conversation is not the soul of democracy’, Critical Studies of Mass Communication 14: 297309. doi: 10.1080/15295039709367020 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scudder, M.F. (2020), Beyond Empathy and Inclusion: The Challenge of Listening in Democratic Deliberation, Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Senft, G. (2009), ‘Phatic communion’, in Senft, G., Östman, J.-O. and Verschueren, J. (eds.), Culture and Language Use. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 226233.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Settle, J.E. and Carlson, T.N. (2019), ‘Opting out of political discussions’, Political Communication 36(3): 476496. doi: 10.1080/10584609.2018.1561563 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Steiner, J., Bächtiger, A., Spörndli, M. and Steenbergen, M.R. (2004), Deliberative Politics in Action, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Straits, B.C. (1991), ‘Bringing strong ties back in. Interpersonal gateways to political information and influence’, Public Opinion Quarterly 55: 432448.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sunstein, C.R. (2003), Why Societies Need Dissent, Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Sydnor, E. (2019), Disrespectful Democracy. The Psychology of Political Incivility, New York: Columbia University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tanasoca, A. (2020), Deliberation Naturalized: Improving Real Existing Deliberative Democracy, Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Testa, P.F., Hibbing, M.V. and Ritchie, M., (2014), ‘Orientations toward conflict and the conditional effects of political disagreement’, The Journal of Politics 76(3): 770785. doi: 10.1017/S0022381614000255 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thompson, D.F. (2008), ‘Deliberative democratic theory and empirical political science’, Annual Review of Political Science 11: 497520. doi: 10.1146/annurev.polisci.11.081306.070555 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thornbury, S. and Slade, D. (2006), Conversation: From Description to Pedagogy, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Deth, J.W. (2000). ‘Interesting but irrelevant: social capital and the saliency of politics in Western Europe’, European Journal of Political Research 37(2): 115147.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watzlawick, P., Beavin, J.H. and Jackson, D.D. (2011), Pragmatics of Human Communication, New York: W.W. Norton.Google Scholar
Welzel, C. and Inglehart, R.F. (2009), ‘Political culture, mass beliefs, and value change’, in Haerpfer, C., Bernhagen, P., Inglehart, R.F. and Welzel, C. (eds.), Democratization, Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 127144.Google Scholar
Wolak, J. (2022), ‘Conflict avoidance and gender gaps in political engagement’, Political Behavior 44: 133156. doi: 10.1007/s11109-020-09614-5 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wyatt, R.O., Katz, E. and Kim, J. (2000), ‘Bridging the spheres: political and personal conversation in public and private spaces’, Journal of Communication 50: 7192. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-2466.2000.tb02834.x CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Young, I.M. (2000), Inclusion and Democracy, Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Zayas, V. and Sakman, E. (2020). ‘Human attachment and affiliation’, Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Psychology. doi: 10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.276 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zuckerman, A.S., Dasovic, J. and Fitzgerald, J. (2007), Partisan Families. The Social Logic of Bounded Partisanship in Germany and Britain, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. Hypothesized interplay between political dispositions, interpersonally relevant psychological dispositions, and interpersonal contexts as backgrounds of attitudes toward everyday political talk.Note: Angular boxes symbolize political factors and rounded boxes social factors.

Figure 1

Figure 2. Attitudes toward everyday political talk (percent).Note: Data are weighted (by gender, age, and city district) and stacked over all conditional circumstances within respondents (N = 13,880).

Figure 2

Table 1. Direct effects of political dispositions, psychological dispositions, and interpersonal contexts on attitudes toward everyday political talk (unstandardized regression coefficients)

Figure 3

Figure 3. Moderation of effect of political interest by psychological dispositions.Note: Entries are two-way interaction effects from multiple linear regression models with robust standard errors (tails indicate 95-% and 90-% confidence intervals). Empty symbols are based on permissive models, filled symbols on restrictive models. See Online Appendix for complete models.

Figure 4

Figure 4. Moderation of effects of political interest and psychological dispositions by interpersonal contexts.Note: Entries are two-way interaction effects from multiple linear regression models with robust standard errors (tails indicate 95-% and 90-% confidence intervals). Empty symbols are based on permissive models, filled symbols on restrictive models. See Online Appendix for complete models.

Figure 5

Figure 5. Moderation of interactions between political interest and psychological dispositions by interpersonal contexts.Note: Entries are three-way interaction effects from multiple linear   regression models with robust standard errors (tails indicate 95-% and 90-% confidence intervals). Empty symbols are based on permissive models, filled symbols on restrictive models. See Online Appendix for complete models.

Supplementary material: File

Schmitt-Beck and Neumann supplementary material

Online Appendix

Download Schmitt-Beck and Neumann supplementary material(File)
File 72.6 KB