Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-ndw9j Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-10T06:21:15.372Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Adverse childhood experiences leads to perceived negative attitude of others and the effect of adverse childhood experiences on depression in adulthood is mediated via negative attitude of others

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  21 July 2018

Raimo K.R. Salokangas*
Affiliation:
aDepartment of Psychiatry, University of Turku, Finland
Tiina From
Affiliation:
aDepartment of Psychiatry, University of Turku, Finland
Sinikka Luutonen
Affiliation:
aDepartment of Psychiatry, University of Turku, Finland bPsychiatric Clinic, Turku University Central Hospital, Finland
Jarmo Hietala
Affiliation:
aDepartment of Psychiatry, University of Turku, Finland bPsychiatric Clinic, Turku University Central Hospital, Finland
*
*Corresponding author at: University of Turku, Department of Psychiatry, Kunnallissairaalantie 20, FIN-20700 Turku, Finland. E-mail address: Raimo.K.R.Salokangas@utu.fi (R.K.R. Salokangas).

Abstract

Background:

The attachment theory suggests that adverse childhood experiences (ACEs) can have an effect on how individuals perceive other people's attitude towards them. ACEs have also been associated with adult depression. We hypothesised that ACEs associate with perceived negative attitude of others (AoO) and depressive symptoms (DEPS), and that these associations differ between the genders.

Methods:

Altogether, 692 participants drawn from the general population completed the Trauma and Distress Scale, as a measurement of ACE and its domains: emotional abuse (EmoAb), physical abuse (PhyAb), sexual abuse (SexAb), emotional neglect (EmoNeg) and physical neglect (PhyNeg); a visual analog scale with the question: “What kind of attitude do other people take towards you?”, and the self-report scale DEPS on depressive symptoms.

Results:

ACEs, AoO and DEPS correlated strongly with each other. In path analyses, ACE total and all its domains associated directly and indirectly, via DEPS, to negative AoO in the whole sample, and in females separately. ACE total, EmoAb, PhyAb, EmoNeg and PhyNeg associated directly and indirectly, via AoO, to DEPS in the whole sample and in both genders separately. EmoNeg, in all, and EmoAB, in males, had specific associations both with negative AoO and DEPS. Mediation effect via AoO was greater than via DEPS.

Conclusions:

ACEs have a direct and indirect, via depression, negative effect on how adult individuals perceive other people's attitude towards themselves. Additionally, negative AoO mediates the effects of ACEs on depression. Childhood EmoNeg associates specifically with negative AoO and DEPS in adulthood.

Type
Original article
Copyright
Copyright © European Psychiatric Association 2018

1. Introduction

As human beings, we live in social relations from our early childhood onwards. The way we perceive other people and their attitude towards us has a powerful impact on the ways we act at both interpersonal and societal level. According to Bowlby's attachment theory [Reference Bowlby9], the styles, in which we interpret other people's attitude towards us, have their roots in our childhood. Within interaction with attachment figures (most often with parents), children develop representational models that allow them to predict and interpret the behaviour of attachment figures and view themselves in relation to others. Negative representational models of attachment figures, built during childhood and adolescence, tend to persist relatively unchanged into and throughout adult life, and can manifest in neurotic symptoms and personality disorders [Reference Bowlby8]; some people with neurotic or personality problems can produce their own severe life events in response to early negative experiences [Reference Harris15]. According to Rohner [Reference Rohner27], parental emotional rejection as a part of the acceptance-rejection syndrome may specifically lead to psychological maladjustment including negative worldview.

Various adverse childhood experiences (ACEs) can distort development of the representational models resulting in insecure or vulnerable attachment styles, such as low self-esteem, poor support and childhood adversity, emotional disorders, depression in particular, and disruption in psychosocial functioning in adulthood [Reference Alexander3, Reference Bifulco, Moran, Ball and Lillie5, Reference Harris, Bifulco, Murray-Parkes, Stevenson-Hinde and Marris16, Reference McLaughlin, Zeanah, Fox and Nelson22, Reference Pirkola, Isometsä, Aro, Kestilä, Hämäläinen and Veijola25, Reference Repetti, Taylor and Seeman26]. Negative parenting practices, such as high level of criticism, verbal humiliation and lack of emotional warmth, may lead to dysfunctional attitudes and attribution styles with vulnerability for depression [Reference Abramson, Metalsky and Alloy1, Reference Beck4, Reference Brown, Harris and Hepworth10, Reference Harris15]; it is thus possible that insecure attachment styles with dysfunctional attitudes, such as perceived negative attitude of others (AoO), may act as mediators between ACE and depression in adulthood.

It has been also found that family environments, with ACEs, are associated with poor mental health, depression particularly [Reference Lindert, von Ehrenstein, Grashow, Gal, Braehler and Weisskopf19, Reference Mandelli, Petrelli and Serretti20, Reference Pirkola, Isometsä, Aro, Kestilä, Hämäläinen and Veijola25, Reference Repetti, Taylor and Seeman26, Reference Weich, Patterson, Shaw and Stewart-Brown34], and that depression may change the way individuals perceive their environment, including other people and their attitude towards themselves [Reference Bora, Bartholomeusz and Pantelis7, Reference Salokangas, Schultze-Lutter, Patterson, von Reventlow, Heinimaa and From31, Reference Wang, Wang, Chen, Zhu and Wang33]. Thus, it is possible that depression act as a mediator between individuals' ACE and the way they perceive attitude of others (AoO). Depression – more prevalent in females than in males – may moderate the association between ACE and AoO differently in females and males. Therefore, it has been suggested that, e.g. the association between depression and suicidal behaviour should be analysed separately by genders [Reference Miller, Esposito-Smythers, Weismoore and Renshaw23].

In a population sample, we aimed to explore the associations between ACEs and individuals’ perceived AoO and depressive symptoms (DEPS). We hypothesised 1) that ACEs associate with AoO and DEPS, 2a) that AoO mediates the effect of ACEs to DEPS, 2b) that DEPS mediates the effect of ACEs to AoO, and 3) that these associations vary between females and males.

2. Methods

The study protocol was approved by the ethical committee of the University of Turku and the Turku University Central Hospital.

2.1. Sample

A random, age-stratified sample of 2080 citizens aged 18 years or more was drawn from the general population of South-West Finland. The general sampling rate was 1/100, and, because of their low proportion in the population, 2/100 for people over 70 years. An extensive questionnaire battery was mailed in spring 2008 and re-mailed to non-responders in summer 2008. The questionnaire included written consent and the individuals who had filled it in were accepted as study participants. The response rate for females (41.5%) was higher than that for males (25.3%; Fisher exact: p < 0.001). Mean age of responders (42.0 ± 16.95 years) was slightly higher than that of non-responders (39.5 ± 16.37years; p = 0.001).

2.2. Assessments

The questionnaire included items on participants’ socio-demographic background and previous care for mental problems, including treatment visits for mental problems to primary and psychiatric care. The questionnaire also included a visual analog with the question: “What kind of attitude do other people take towards you?” The ends of the visual analog were: 0 = very negative and 10 = very positive. This question was used as an indicator of perceived attitude of others (AoO) and its distribution is shown in Fig. 1. The attitude question was taken from the PROD screen [Reference Heinimaa, Salokangas, Ristkari, Plathin, Huttunen and Ilonen18] developed for screening people with (cognitive) prodromal symptoms of psychosis. In a prospective study, perceived negative AoO predicted onset of psychosis in clinical high-risk to psychosis patients [Reference Salokangas, Patterson, Heinimaa, Svirskis, From and Vaskelainen29].

Depressive symptoms were assessed by the depression screening instrument DEPS [Reference Salokangas, Poutanen and Stengård30]. It consists of 10 questions rated on a Likert scale as: 0=’not at all’, 1=’to some extent’, 2=’rather much’ and 3=’very much’); their sum (range 1–30) indicates number of depressive symptoms during the past month. The DEPS, with Cronbach's α 0.88, was originally developed for the use of general practitioners. In a sample of patients attending primary care at a cut-off of >8, the DEPS revealed a sensitivity of 74% and a specificity of 85% for clinical depression [Reference Salokangas, Poutanen and Stengård30].

ACEs were assessed by the Trauma and Distress Scale (TADS) [Reference Patterson, Skeate, Schultze-Lutter, Graf von Reventlow, Wieneke and Ruhrmann24]. At the time of the present study, the TADS was the only available ACE instrument in Finnish. It includes 43 items on childhood trauma and adversity rated for their frequency in a Likert format: 0=’never’, 1=’rarely’, 2=’sometimes’, 3=’often’ and 4=’almost always’. Five TADS domain scores can be calculated by summing their respective items: 1. emotional abuse (EmoAb; 5 items), 2. physical abuse (PhyAb; 5 items), 3. sexual abuse (SexAb; 5 items), 4. emotional neglect (EmoNeg; 5 items), and 5. physical neglect (PhyNeg; 4 items), as well as the TADS total score (sum of all five domain scores). The TADS, with Cronbach's α 0.92, and its five core domains proved to be a valid, reliable and clinically useful instrument for assessing retrospectively reported childhood traumatisation [Reference Salokangas, Schultze-Lutter, Patterson, von Reventlow, Heinimaa and From31].

2.3. Statistical analyses

First, means of AoO and DEPS scores were calculated by background characteristics, as well as ACE scores by gender, and differences were tested by t-test. ACE domain and DEPS scores were correlated (Spearman's rho) with AoO. Because the dependent variables, DEPS and AoO, were skewed, their logarithmic transformations were used in multivariate path analyses.

In multivariate analyses, a PROCESS macro in SPSS (model template 4) by A. F. Hayes [Reference Hayes17] was used. In cross-sectional samples, this macro tests the direct and indirect effects of an independent variable (X) on a dependent variable (Y), while modelling a process in which X affects a mediator (M), which in turn affects Y. The models tested the effect of X (ACE) on a) Y (AoO) with the mediator (DEPS) and b) on Y (DEPS) with the mediator (AoO) in the total sample, and in females and males separately. In each model, the total effect of X on Y, the direct and the indirect effect via mediator DEPS/AoO, as well as the effect of ACE total on DEPS/AoO (a), the effect of DEPS/AoO on AoO/DEPS (b) and the direct effect of ACE total on AoO/DEPS (c) are reported in Fig. 2. In Table 4 and in Supplementary Table S1, the total effect X (ACE) on Y (AoO/DEPS), the direct effect and the specific effect, when the effects of other ACE domains are controlled, are reported. Five thousand bootstrap samples and 95% confidence intervals were used for all analyses. In these analyses, the effects of gender, age and previous mental care were controlled. The same analyses were also carried out in a subsample of participants without previous mental care. Because in preliminary path analyses age did not significantly mediate the effect of ACE to AoO or DEPS, it was not used as a mediator in final analyses. The data were analyzed using SPSS software (22.0 for Windows). P-values below 0.05 (two-tail) were considered statistically significant.

Fig 1. Distribution of attitude of others scores (1=Extremely negative, 10= Extremely positive).

3. Results

3.1. Univariate analyses

In the study sample, about two thirds were women, lived in an intimate relationship, and were in full-time work. The majority of the subjects were younger than 45 years, about a third had completed education of university level, and a fourth had previously received care for mental problems (Table 1).

Women, the married or cohabitating, the highly educated, the full-time working subjects, and the subjects without previous mental care reported higher AoO scores. There were no gender or age differences in the DEPS scores. The single, those who had completed secondary school, the unemployed (or work situation unknown) and the participants who had received mental care reported higher DEPS scores (Table 1).

There were no gender differences in ACE total scores and both genders reported most EmoNeg. Females reported SexAb more than males and males reported EmoAb more than females (Table 2).

DEPS, ACE and its domains correlated strongly with AoO. There were also high inter-correlations between ACE domains, indicating a great overlap between individual domains (Table 3).

3.2. Path analyses

In path analyses, DEPS and AoO were in turn treated as mediators and, correspondingly, AoO and DEPS as outcomes. Fig. 2 shows the components of direct and indirect effects, as well as ratios between indirect and total effect in the whole sample and females and males separately. Comparison between models revealed that the indirect/total effect ratio was higher when AoO was treated as a mediator than when DEPS was a mediator.

Fig 2. Path analyses for all and females and males separately; the effects gender (in all), age and previous mental care are controlled.

In the first analyses with DEPS as a mediator, when the effects of gender, age and previous mental care were controlled, ACE total and all its domains had significant direct and indirect effects on AoO, via DEPS, in the whole sample and in females and males separately, with one exception; in males SexAb had no significant effect on AoO (Table 4). Additionally, in the whole sample and in both genders EmoNeg had a significant direct and indirect specific effect on AoO when the effects of other ACE domains were taken into account. In males, also EmoAb had a direct and indirect specific effect on AoO (Table 4).

Table 1 Background of the study sample and means (range 1–10) for attitude of others (AoO) and depression symptoms (DEPS).

Table 2 DEPS and ACE scores by gender.

DEPS = depressive symptoms; EmoAb = emotional abuse; PhyAb = physical abuse; SexAb = sexual abuse; EmoNeg = emotional neglect; PhyNeg = physical neglect; ACE total = sum of adverse childhood experiences.Significant associations bolded.

Table 3 Spearman's correlation between DEPS scores, ACE domains and AoO.

All correlations: p < 0.001; AoO = attitude of others; DEPS = depressive symptoms; EmoAb = emotional abuse; PhyAb = physical abuse; SexAb = sexual abuse; EmoNeg = emotional neglect; PhyNeg = physical neglect; ACE total = sum of adverse childhood experiences.

Table 4 Path analyses for AoO (A) and DEPS (B); the effects of gender, age and previous mental care controlled.

a = total effect of ACE on AoO/DEPS; b = direct effect of ACE on AoO/DEPS; c = specific direct effect of ACE on AoO/DEPS; d = ratio of indirect effect to total effect X to Y; AoO = attitude of others; DEPS = depression symptoms; Significant associations bolded

ACE total = sum of adverse childhood experiences; EmoAb = emotional abuse; PhyAb = physical abuse; SexAb = sexual abuse; EmoNeg = emotional neglect; PhyNeg = physical neglect.

In the second analyses with AoO as mediator, ACE total and all its domains, except SexAb, had significant direct and indirect effects on DEPS in the whole sample and in females and males separately (Table 4). In the whole sample, SexAb had a direct, in females, only an indirect, and in males, no effect on DEPS. In the whole sample and in females, EmoNeg had a significant direct and indirect, in males only an indirect specific effect on DEPS. In males, also EmoAb had an indirect specific effect, via AoO, on DEPS (Table 4). The indirect/total effect ratios were for AoO as a mediator 1.5 or higher for ACE total and EmoAb in males, for EmoNeg in all and both genders and for PhyNeg in females, indicating that in these cases the indirect effect of AoO on DEPS was greater than vice versa.

About a fourth of the participants had received care for mental problems. It was probable that many of them had depressive symptoms before the inquiry. We therefore formed a subgroup of the participants who had not received mental care previously. We assumed that in this subgroup reported depressive symptoms would be brief, not chronic. The results of the first sensitivity analyses with DEPS as mediator were very similar to those in the first path analysis for all participants. However, SexAb had a significant direct effect on AoO in males, but no longer in females (Supplement Table 1).

Also the results of the second sensitivity analyses with AoO as mediator were similar to those in the second path analysis for all participants, with two exceptions; SexAb had no effect on DEPS in the whole subsample, and PhyNeg had no significant direct effect on DEPS in females and males (Supplement Table S1). In sensitivity analyses, the indirect/total effect ratios were for AoO as mediator 1.5 or higher for ACE total, EmoAb, EmoNeg and PhyNeg in all; for ACE total, PhyAb and PhyNeg in females; and for ACE total, EmoAb, PhyAb and EmoNeg in males, indicating that in participant without previous mental care, mediating effects of AoO on DEPS were greater than among all.

4. Discussion

The main hypotheses put forward were confirmed. The ACEs associated significantly with negative AoO and DEPS. The ACE and all its domains, except SexAb, had direct and indirect – via depressive symptoms – associations with negative AoO, and direct and indirect – via negative attitude of others – associations with DEPS. Generally, mediation of ACE via AoO to DEPS was stronger than vice versa. Of the five core ACE domains, EmoNeg had specific direct and indirect effects on negative AoO and on DEPS in the whole sample and in females. In males, EmoAb and EmoNeg had direct and indirect specific effects on AoO, but only an indirect specific effect on DEPS.

4.1. Gender

Females' perception of other people’s attitude towards them was more positive than that of males although they reported slightly more ACEs than males. It seems that females take a more positive stand to other people and trust them more than males per se. It is possible that this gender difference has its origin in individuals' childhood: boys are inclined to extravert behavioural problems [e.g. [Reference Bongers, Koot, van der Ende and Verhulst6] [Reference Wichstrøm, Berg-Nielsen, Angold, Egger, Solheim and Sveen36],], and may therefore receive negative attention more often than girls from their parents and other attachment figures, and may consequently develop negative representational models more often than girls, the effect of which is seen as perceived negative AoO yet in adulthood.

In the present study, there were no significant gender difference in depressive symptoms measured by the DEPS, possibly because the DEPS instrument detects depressive symptoms more gender-neutrally than, e.g. the Beck Depression Inventory [Reference Salokangas, Vaahtera, Pakriev, Sohlman and Lehtinen32]. The fact that the total effects of ACE total on AoO and DEPS (Fig. 2) were in males stronger than in females indicates that adverse childhood experiences may in general have stronger effects on negative perceived AoO and depressive symptoms in males than in females.

4.2. Perceived attitude of others and depressive symptoms as mediators

In previous studies, mood disorders and symptoms have been associated with disturbed ability to attribute mental states of others [Reference Bora, Bartholomeusz and Pantelis7, Reference Wang, Wang, Chen, Zhu and Wang33, Reference Weightman, Air and Baune35], and with how individuals have perceived other people and their attitude towards them. Individuals with depressive disorders are inclined to interpret their interpersonal relations negatively and this bias towards negative emotions seems to persist even in the remission phase [Reference Weightman, Air and Baune35]. In line with clinical studies, in the present population study, DEPS associated with perceived negative AoO. And, because ACEs consistently associate with depression in adulthood, treating them as mediator between ACE and AoO was justified.

However, as Bowlby [Reference Bowlby8, Reference Bowlby9] and others [Reference Abramson, Metalsky and Alloy1, Reference Beck4, Reference Harris15] have suggested, childhood adversities, such as repeated critical comments, verbal humiliation and lack of emotional warmth, may lead to dysfunctional attitudes and attribution styles which in turn may act as vulnerability factors for depression; in other words, may mediate the effect of ACE on depression. Indeed, the present path analyses showed that a great deal of the effect of ACE and its domains on DEPS was mediated via AoO. The indirect/total effect rates for AoO as mediator were almost systematically higher than those for DEPS as mediator. In participants without previous mental care, these rates were even greater than in the whole sample, indicating that perceived negative AoO mediates the effects of ACE on current depression symptoms much more strongly than vice versa.

4.3. Adverse childhood experiences

All ACE domains, except SexAb, associated with negative AoO and DEPS, indicating that various adverse childhood experiences have long-term effects on how individuals perceive other people and their attitude towards themselves, as well as depressive symptoms. This is in line with the attachment theory [Reference Bowlby9] stating that the representational models of attachment modes, developed during childhood and adolescence, tend to persist into adult life [Reference Bowlby8], and with previous findings that various ACEs have detrimental effects on these representational models [Reference Bifulco, Moran, Ball and Lillie5Reference Bowlby8, Reference Germine, Dunn, McLaughlin and Smoller13] leading to dysfunctional attitudes and attribution styles and depressive mood [Reference Abramson, Metalsky and Alloy1, Reference Beck4, Reference Harris15]. A new finding was that this association between ACE and perceived negative AoO was found in a general population sample. Our previous finding that 72% of the present sample of the general population reported that they had experienced some abuse or neglect at least sometimes [Reference Salokangas, Poutanen and Stengård30] indicates that ACEs and their possibly negative effects on interpersonal relationships touch a great number of people, and also that the negative interpretation of other people's attitude towards themselves increases their vulnerability to depression.

Of the five core ACE domains [Reference Burgermeister11], only emotional neglect had a specific indirect effect on negative AoO, as well as on DEPS, indicating that emotional neglect was the major specific factor influencing both AoO and DEPS. It is important to note that emotional neglect was also the most frequently reported ACE domain, as has also been found in other cultures [Reference Akyuz, Sar, Kugu and Doğan2, Reference Dubowitz and Bennett12]. The domain of emotional neglect [Reference Patterson, Skeate, Schultze-Lutter, Graf von Reventlow, Wieneke and Ruhrmann24] comprises the following five items which for calculating domain scores are reversed: "When I was young, I felt valued or important", "My family was emotionally warm and loving", "When I was young, my family looked after each other", "I respect myself", "My family was supportive and encouraging when I was young". These items represent nicely the two key facets of secure (as opposed to insecure) attachment: 1) Self is perceived as worthy of love and attention, and 2) others are viewed as warm and responsive, as put forward by Bowlby [Reference Bowlby9]. In Rohner's concept of the parental acceptance-rejection syndrome, emotional rejection comes close to the emotional neglect of ACE [Reference Rohner28]. According to Rohner [Reference Rohner27], children and adults, who have experienced being rejected in their relationship with their parents, report specific forms of psychological maladjustment including impaired self-adequacy and negative worldview.

The greatest gender differences were found in EmoAb, SexAb and EmoNeg. In males, EmoAb and EmoNeg had specific effects on negative AoO directly and via DEPS, while their effects on DEPS were mediated via AoO, indicating that in males, adverse childhood emotional experiences greatly and specifically disturb their trust in other people and also make them vulnerable to depression. In females, SexAb had rather strong indirect effects on DEPS, via AoO, indicating that depression in females is considerably but not specifically mediated via perceived negative attitude of other.

In the present study, we were able confirm the findings of Bowlby and Rohner that were based on individual psychology, on the population level, and show that, although ACEs as a whole may disturb the way in which we perceive our fellow human beings and their attitude towards us, the childhood experiences of being emotionally rejected or neglected have the most harmful specific effect on our ability to trust other people. In addition, these attitudinal disturbances may make us vulnerable to depressive mood and clinical depression.

4.4. Implications

According to the literature, the ACEs are associated with various mental, physical, behavioural and functional problems [Reference Repetti, Taylor and Seeman26]. The results of the present population study indicate that ACEs may have long-standing effects on individuals' social perception, beyond mental problems. Specifically, emotional neglect experienced in childhood seems to lead to the perception that other people take a negative attitude towards us. The emotional neglect, as it was defined in the present study, is common: more than half (51.2%) of the participants drawn from the general population reported that they had experienced emotional neglect at least sometimes in their childhood [Reference Salokangas, Schultze-Lutter, Patterson, von Reventlow, Heinimaa and From31]. Taking into account that the self-reported negative effects of parental rejection, which comes close to the concept of emotional neglect, are universal, regardless of differences in culture, ethnicity, or language [Reference Rohner27], the emotional neglect experienced during childhood may shadow a great number of people's adult social life and affect the whole society and its emotional atmosphere.

From a clinical point of view, there are two possibilities to benefit from the finding that ACEs and childhood emotional neglect specifically predicts negative AoO. In the patients with mental problems, depression particularly, it is useful to study how they perceive other people and their attitude towards themselves and use this finding in therapeutic intervention. On the other hand, if the patients report experiences of childhood emotional neglect, also dealing with current social relationships and their distortions may free emotional resources for recovery from mental illnesses.

4.5. Strength and limitations

Some strengths and limitations of the present study should be acknowledged. The cross-sectional study design and retrospective assessment of ACEs do not allow strong causal conclusions. However, there is some evidence that adverse childhood experiences can be reliably assessed retrospectively [Reference Maughan and Rutter21] and that retrospective recall bias is likely to be conservative, leading to underreporting of childhood adversities [Reference Hardt and Rutter14].

ACEs were assessed by the TADS [Reference Miller, Esposito-Smythers, Weismoore and Renshaw23]. Our earlier study showed that the TADS is a reliable instrument and that there was a good concordance between self-reported and telephone- interviewed TADS domains [Reference Salokangas, Schultze-Lutter, Patterson, von Reventlow, Heinimaa and From31]. It is possible that an interview would be a more reliable and objective method for assessing childhood adversities. However, as Rohner [Reference Rohner27] has argued, individuals’ subjective experience – in this case of emotional neglect and perceived attitude of others – is reality to them, as is their way to act also in relation to other people.

We were not able to define the onset of depressive symptoms. Therefore, we first controlled the effect of previous mental care, and secondly repeated path analyses in the group of participants without previous mental care. These sensitivity analyses reduced the bias due to lack of knowledge about onset of depressive symptoms, though not completely. The low response rate, in males in particular, limits the generability of the results.

Declaration of interest

None.

Funding

The study was funded by Turku University Central Hospital (EVO funding).

Appendix A. Supplementary data

Supplementary material related to this article can be found, in the online version, at doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eurpsy.2018.06.011.

References

Abramson, LYMetalsky, GIAlloy, LBHopelessness depression: a theory based subtype of depression. Psychol Rev 1989; 96:358–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Akyuz, GSar, VKugu, NDoğan, OReported childhood trauma, attempted suicide and self-mutilative behavior among women in the general population. Eur Psychiatry 2005;20(3):268–73.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alexander, PCThe differential effects of abuse characteristics and attachment in the prediction of long-term effects of sexual abuse. J Interpers Violence 1993; 8:346–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beck, ATThe evolution of the cognitive model of depression and its neurobiological correlates. Am J Psychiatry 2008; 165:969–77.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bifulco, AMoran, PMBall, CLillie, AAdult attachment style. II: its relationship to psychosocial depressive-vulnerability. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 2002;37(2):60–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bongers, ILKoot, HMvan der Ende, JVerhulst, FCThe normative development of child and adolescent problem behavior. J Abnorm Psychol 2003;112(2):179–92.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bora, EBartholomeusz, CPantelis, CMeta-analysis of theory of mind (ToM) impairment in bipolar disorder. Psychol Med 2016;46(2):253–64.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bowlby, JThe making and breaking of affectional bonds. I. Aetiology and psychopathology in the light of attachment theory. Br J Psychiatry 1977; 130:201–10.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bowlby, JAttachment and loss: vol 2. Separation: anxiety and anger 1973, Basic books New York.Google Scholar
Brown, GWHarris, TOHepworth, CLoss, humiliation and entrapment among women developing depression: a patient and non-patients comparison. Psychol Med 1995; 25:721.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burgermeister, DChildhood adversity: a review of measurement instruments. J Nurs Meas 2007;15(3):163–76 Review.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dubowitz, HBennett, SPhysical abuse and neglect of children. Lancet 2007;369(9576):1891–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Germine, LDunn, ECMcLaughlin, KASmoller, JWChildhood adversity is associated with adult theory of mind and social affiliation, but not face processing. PLoS One 2015;10(6): e0129612 10.1371/journal.pone.0129612.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hardt, JRutter, MValidity of adult retrospective reports of adverse childhood experiences: review of the evidence. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2004;45(2):260–73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harris, TRecent developments in understanding the psychosocial aspects of depressions. Br Med Bull 2001; 57:1732.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harris, TBifulco, ALoss of parent in childhood, attachment style, and depression in adulthoodMurray-Parkes, CStevenson-Hinde, JMarris, PAttachment across the life-cycle. 1991, Routledge London, New York.Google Scholar
Hayes, AFPROCESS procedure for SPSS release 2.13.2. 2014. [Computer software] Retrieved from http://www.processmacro.org.Google Scholar
Heinimaa, MSalokangas, RKRRistkari, TPlathin, MHuttunen, JIlonen, T et al. PROD-screen – a screen for prodromal symptoms of psychosis. Int J Methods Psychiatr Res 2003; 12:92104.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lindert, Jvon Ehrenstein, OSGrashow, RGal, GBraehler, EWeisskopf, MGSexual and physical abuse in childhood is associated with depression and anxiety over the life course: systematic review and meta-analysis. Int J Public Health 2014; 59:359–72 Review.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mandelli, LPetrelli, CSerretti, AThe role of specific early trauma in adult depression: a meta-analysis of published literature. Childhood trauma and adult depression. Eur Psychiatry 2015; 30:665–80 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.eurpsy.2015.04.007Epub 2015 Jun 13.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maughan, BRutter, MRetrospective reporting of childhood adversity: issues in assessing long-term recall. J Pers Disord 1997;11(1):1933.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McLaughlin, KAZeanah, CHFox, NANelson, CAAttachment security as a mechanism linking foster care placement to improved mental health outcomes in previously institutionalized children. J Child Psychol Psyc 2011; 53:4655.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, ABEsposito-Smythers, CWeismoore, JTRenshaw, KDThe relation between child maltreatment and adolescent suicidal behavior: a systematic review and critical examination of the literature. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev 2013; 16:146–72.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Patterson, PSkeate, ASchultze-Lutter, FGraf von Reventlow, HWieneke, ARuhrmann, S et al. The trauma and distress scale 2002, University of Birmingham UK.Google Scholar
Pirkola, SIsometsä, EAro, HKestilä, LHämäläinen, JVeijola, J et al. Childhood adversities as risk factors for adult mental disorders: results from the health 2000 study. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 2005;40(10):769–77.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Repetti, RLTaylor, SESeeman, TERisky families: family social environments and the mental and physical health of offspring. Psychol Bull 2002;128(2):330–66.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rohner, RPThe parental "acceptance-rejection syndrome": universal correlates of perceived rejection. Am Psychol 2004;59(8):830–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rohner, RPThe warmth dimension: foundations of parental acceptance-rejection theory 1986, Sage Publications, Inc Beverly Hills, CA.Google Scholar
Salokangas, RKRPatterson, PHeinimaa, MSvirskis, TFrom, TVaskelainen, L et al. Perceived negative attitude of others predicts transition to psychosis in patients at risk of psychosis. Eur Psychiatry 2012; 27:264–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salokangas, RKRPoutanen, OStengård, EScreening for depression in primary care. Development and validation of the depression scale, a screening instrument for depression. Acta Psychiatr Scand 1995; 92:10–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salokangas, RKSchultze-Lutter, FPatterson, Pvon Reventlow, HGHeinimaa, MFrom, T et al. Psychometric properties of the trauma and distress scale, TADS, in an adult community sample in Finland. Eur J Psychotraumatol 2016; 7:30062 10.3402/ejpt.v7.30062.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Salokangas, RKRVaahtera, KPakriev, SSohlman, BLehtinen, VGender differences in depressive symptoms. An artefact caused by measurement instruments?. J Aff Disord 2002; 68:215–20.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, YGWang, YQChen, SLZhu, CYWang, KTheory of mind disability in major depression with or without psychotic symptoms: a componential view. Psychiatry Res 2008;161(2):153–61.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weich, SPatterson, JShaw, RStewart-Brown, SFamily relationships in childhood and common psychiatric disorders in later life: systematic review of prospective studies. Br J Psychiatry 2009;194(5):392–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weightman, MJAir, TMBaune, BTA review of the role of social cognition in major depressive disorder. Front Psychiatry 2014; 5:179.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wichstrøm, LBerg-Nielsen, TSAngold, AEgger, HLSolheim, ESveen, THPrevalence of psychiatric disorders in preschoolers. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2012;53(6):695705.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Fig 1. Distribution of attitude of others scores (1=Extremely negative, 10= Extremely positive).

Figure 1

Fig 2. Path analyses for all and females and males separately; the effects gender (in all), age and previous mental care are controlled.

Figure 2

Table 1 Background of the study sample and means (range 1–10) for attitude of others (AoO) and depression symptoms (DEPS).

Figure 3

Table 2 DEPS and ACE scores by gender.

Figure 4

Table 3 Spearman's correlation between DEPS scores, ACE domains and AoO.

Figure 5

Table 4 Path analyses for AoO (A) and DEPS (B); the effects of gender, age and previous mental care controlled.

Supplementary material: File

Salokangas et al. supplementary material

Table S1
Download Salokangas et al. supplementary material(File)
File 152.6 KB
Submit a response

Comments

No Comments have been published for this article.