Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-mkpzs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T05:51:47.526Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Gods are watching and so what? Moralistic supernatural punishment across 15 cultures

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  12 May 2023

Theiss Bendixen*
Affiliation:
Aarhus University, Aarhus, Denmark
Aaron D. Lightner
Affiliation:
Aarhus University, Aarhus, Denmark
Coren Apicella
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania
Quentin Atkinson
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand Max Planck Institute for the Science of Human History, Jena, Germany
Alexander Bolyanatz
Affiliation:
College of DuPage, Glen Ellyn, Illinois, USA
Emma Cohen
Affiliation:
Wadham College, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK
Carla Handley
Affiliation:
Arizona State University, Phoenix, Arizona, USA
Joseph Henrich
Affiliation:
Department of Human Evolutionary Biology, Harvard University, Cambridge, Massachusetts, USA
Eva Kundtová Klocová
Affiliation:
LEVYNA, Masaryk University, Brno, Czech Republic
Carolyn Lesorogol
Affiliation:
Washington University in St Louis, St. Louis, Missouri, USA
Sarah Mathew
Affiliation:
Arizona State University, Phoenix, Arizona, USA
Rita A. McNamara
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, Victoria University of Wellington, Wellington, New Zealand
Cristina Moya
Affiliation:
Department of Anthropology, University of California Davis, Davis, California, USA
Ara Norenzayan
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada
Caitlyn Placek
Affiliation:
Department of Anthropology, Ball State University, Muncie, Indiana, USA
Montserrat Soler
Affiliation:
Ob/Gyn and Women's Health Institute Cleveland Clinic, Cleveland, Ohio, USA
Tom Vardy
Affiliation:
Department of International Development, London School of Economics, London, UK
Jonathan Weigel
Affiliation:
Haas School of Business, University of California Berkeley, Berkeley, California, USA
Aiyana K. Willard
Affiliation:
Brunel University, London, UK
Dimitris Xygalatas
Affiliation:
Department of Anthropology, University of Connecticut, Storrs, Connecticut, USA Department of Psychological Sciences, University of Connecticut, Storrs, Connecticut, USA
Martin Lang
Affiliation:
LEVYNA, Masaryk University, Brno, Czech Republic
Benjamin Grant Purzycki*
Affiliation:
Aarhus University, Aarhus, Denmark
*
Corresponding authors: Theiss Bendixen; Email: tb@theissbendixen.com; Benjamin Grant Purzycki; Email: bgpurzycki@cas.au.dk
Corresponding authors: Theiss Bendixen; Email: tb@theissbendixen.com; Benjamin Grant Purzycki; Email: bgpurzycki@cas.au.dk

Abstract

Psychological and cultural evolutionary accounts of human sociality propose that beliefs in punitive and monitoring gods that care about moral norms facilitate cooperation. While there is some evidence to suggest that belief in supernatural punishment and monitoring generally induce cooperative behaviour, the effect of a deity's explicitly postulated moral concerns on cooperation remains unclear. Here, we report a pre-registered set of analyses to assess whether perceiving a locally relevant deity as moralistic predicts cooperative play in two permutations of two economic games using data from up to 15 diverse field sites. Across games, results suggest that gods’ moral concerns do not play a direct, cross-culturally reliable role in motivating cooperative behaviour. The study contributes substantially to the current literature by testing a central hypothesis in the evolutionary and cognitive science of religion with a large and culturally diverse dataset using behavioural and ethnographically rich methods.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

Social media summary: A watchful and punitive god can make people more cooperative, but how does a deity's perceived moral concerns play into this phenomenon?

1. Introduction

Scholars of religion have long contemplated how religious appeals, beliefs and rituals are implicated in human cooperation (Durkheim, Reference Durkheim1912; Ellwood, Reference Ellwood1918; Evans-Pritchard, Reference Evans-Pritchard1965; Lang, Reference Lang1909; Malinowski, Reference Malinowski1936; Rappaport, Reference Rappaport1968; Wallace, Reference Wallace1966; for recent reviews, see Bendixen et al., Reference Bendixen, Lightner, Purzycki, Kendal, Tehrani and Kendal2023b; McKay and Whitehouse, Reference McKay and Whitehouse2014; Purzycki and McKay, Reference Purzycki, McKay, Purzycki and Bendixen2023). Recent psychological and cultural evolutionary accounts of human sociality have proposed that beliefs about ‘moralising’ deities – that is, punitive and monitoring gods and spirits believed to be concerned with violations of inter-personal norms (Purzycki and McNamara, Reference Purzycki, McNamara, De Cruz and Nichols2016) – foster cooperative relationships (Johnson, Reference Johnson2016; Shariff and Norenzayan, Reference Shariff and Norenzayan2007) and help societies increase in size and complexity (Norenzayan et al., Reference Norenzayan, Shariff, Gervais, Willard, McNamara, Slingerland and Henrich2016; Schloss and Murray, Reference Schloss and Murray2011).

While there is some evidence to support a relationship between supernatural punishment beliefs and cooperation, individual-level studies remain inconclusive with regards to establishing a link between the moral content of supernatural punishment beliefs specifically and cooperative behaviour (Bendixen et al., Reference Bendixen, Lightner, Purzycki, Kendal, Tehrani and Kendal2023b). In this study, across 15 diverse field sites and two economic games, we assess the importance of moral content in moralistic supernatural punishment beliefs and its role in motivating cooperative behaviour.

1.1. Supernatural moral punishment and cooperation: A tour of the evidence

A family of theories holds that belief in moralising deities facilitates cooperation through fear that moral norm violations are supernaturally monitored and sanctioned. In turn, by harnessing psychological systems responsible for cooperative behaviour, such beliefs are ostensibly one mechanism that can help resolve problems associated with collective action and cooperation (Schloss and Murray, Reference Schloss and Murray2011; Purzycki and McKay, Reference Purzycki, McKay, Purzycki and Bendixen2023). As such, morally concerned gods in particular are posited to help scale up societies in response to social conflict (Caluori et al., Reference Caluori, Jackson, Gray and Gelfand2020; Skali, Reference Skali2017) and ecological threats (Hayden, Reference Hayden1987). Society-level analyses based on data coded from ethnographic material indeed suggest that moralistic supernatural punishment beliefs are positively associated with a society's size and political stratification (e.g. Roes and Raymond, Reference Roes and Raymond2003; Swanson, Reference Swanson1960; Watts et al., Reference Watts, Greenhill, Atkinson, Currie, Bulbulia and Gray2015) and with environmental stressors (e.g. Botero et al., Reference Botero, Gardner, Kirby, Bulbulia, Gavin and Gray2014; Jackson et al., Reference Jackson, Caluori, Abrams, Beckman, Gelfand and Gray2021; Skoggard et al., Reference Skoggard, Ember, Pitek, Jackson and Carolus2020; Snarey, Reference Snarey1996).

However, database studies of coded ethnographic sources suffer from a variety of biases, including systematic missingness in focal variables (Beheim et al., Reference Beheim, Atkinson, Bulbulia, Gervais, Gray, Henrich and Willard2021; Purzycki et al., Reference Purzycki, Bendixen and Lightner2022a), non-representative sampling of cultural groups and antiquated data coding schemes (e.g. whether a moralising god is also by necessity a high god, that is a creator deity; see Lightner et al., Reference Lightner, Bendixen and Purzycki2022; Purzycki and McKay, Reference Purzycki, McKay, Purzycki and Bendixen2023; Watts et al., Reference Watts, Greenhill, Atkinson, Currie, Bulbulia and Gray2015). Further, surveys of the ethnographic record (e.g. Boehm, Reference Boehm, Bulbulia, Sosis, Harris, Genet and Wyman2008; Bendixen and Purzycki, Reference Bendixen, Purzycki, Purzycki and Bendixen2023a; Purzycki and McNamara, Reference Purzycki, McNamara, De Cruz and Nichols2016; Rossano, Reference Rossano2007; Swanson, Reference Swanson1960) as well as recent individual-level ethnographic inquiries (e.g. Bendixen et al., Reference Bendixen, Apicella, Atkinson, Cohen, Henrich, McNamara and Purzycki2023a; Purzycki, Reference Purzycki2011, Reference Purzycki2013, Reference Purzycki2016; Purzycki et al., Reference Purzycki, Willard, Klocová, Apicella, Atkinson, Bolyanatz and Ross2022c; Singh et al., Reference Singh, Kaptchuk and Henrich2021; Townsend et al., Reference Townsend, Aktipis, Balliet and Cronk2020) strongly indicate that notions of supernatural punishment of moral norm violations are common even in smaller-scale societies, calling into question the reliability of databases that suggest otherwise (Lightner et al., Reference Lightner, Bendixen and Purzycki2022; Purzycki and McKay, Reference Purzycki, McKay, Purzycki and Bendixen2023).

Studies using individual-level data have found mixed evidence of a causal relationship between moralistic supernatural punishment beliefs and increased cooperation. For instance, Ge et al. (Reference Ge, Chen, Wu and Mace2019) failed to find a clear association between belief in supernatural punishment and charitable donations in cross-community economic games with a culturally diverse sample. Conversely, Townsend et al. (Reference Townsend, Aktipis, Balliet and Cronk2020) found that, among the Ik of Uganda, reminding participants about the possibility of supernatural punishment induced higher allocation of money to a needy and anonymous co-community member than in a control condition. Survey (e.g. Atkinson and Bourrat, Reference Atkinson and Bourrat2011; White et al., Reference White, Kelly, Shariff and Norenzayan2019) and experimental (e.g. Shariff et al., Reference Shariff, Willard, Andersen and Norenzayan2016; Yilmaz and Bahçekapili, Reference Yilmaz and Bahçekapili2016) studies conducted primarily in industrialised societies likewise tend to find positive relationships between beliefs in supernatural punishment and/or reward and various indices of cooperation (cf. Bloom, Reference Bloom2012; Galen, Reference Galen2012). Across 15 field sites and two waves of data collection, Lang et al. (Reference Lang, Purzycki, Apicella, Atkinson, Bolyanatz, Cohen and Henrich2019) found a small but robust association between ratings of deities as punitive and monitoring and non-selfish coin allocations in two anonymous economic games, supporting the notion that moralising religions can indeed contribute to an expansion of cooperative circles. Critically, however, in Lang et al. (Reference Lang, Purzycki, Apicella, Atkinson, Bolyanatz, Cohen and Henrich2019) ratings of deities as morally concerned – specifically, a three-item ‘moral interest scale’ on the extent to which a deity cares for punishing theft, lying and murder (see also Purzycki et al., Reference Purzycki, Apicella, Atkinson, Cohen, McNamara, Willard and Henrich2016b) – did not consistently predict cooperative behaviour, casting doubt on the extent to which item scales of this sort reliably reflect culturally relevant models of gods’ concerns (Purzycki et al., Reference Purzycki, Willard, Klocová, Apicella, Atkinson, Bolyanatz and Ross2022c).

Taken together, while supernatural punishment and monitoring generally seem to induce cooperative behaviour, the effect of a deity's explicitly postulated moral concerns on cooperation remains unclear. It is central to theory, however, since many deities across cultures are attributed with punitive concerns that seem to directly correspond to local coordination problems, such as territoriality, ecological management, resource distribution and, crucially, breaches of moral norms (Bendixen and Purzycki, Reference Bendixen, Purzycki, Purzycki and Bendixen2023a; Bendixen et al., Reference Bendixen, Apicella, Atkinson, Cohen, Henrich, McNamara and Purzycki2023a; Purzycki and McNamara, Reference Purzycki, McNamara, De Cruz and Nichols2016; Purzycki et al., Reference Purzycki, Willard, Klocová, Apicella, Atkinson, Bolyanatz and Ross2022c). As such, both measuring these punitive concerns with culturally appropriate methods and determining whether they guide individuals’ cooperative behaviour would help clarify the role that supernatural appeals and beliefs might have played in human societies, past and present (Bendixen et al., Reference Bendixen, Lightner, Purzycki, Kendal, Tehrani and Kendal2023b). Using both behavioural and ethnographically rich methods, the present study investigates whether or not gods’ explicitly postulated moral concerns predict cooperative behaviour across up to 15 diverse field sites.

2. Hypotheses, data and methods

2.1. The present study

Here, we report a set of pre-registered analyses to assess the importance of moral content in moralistic supernatural punishment beliefs and its role in motivating cooperative behaviour. Specifically, we assess whether free-listing a locally relevant moralistic deity as angered by immorality predicts cooperative play in two permutations of each of two economic games. In the supplementary materials (Section S4) we outline all deviations from the pre-registration protocol; deviations were few and minor.

As an ethnographic technique, free-listing reflects cognitive and cultural models of the target topic, to the extent that this topic is relevant in the local context (Quinlan, Reference Quinlan2017). Free-listing might therefore be a more appropriate measure of a deity's degree of moral concern than pre-fabricated item scales used in previous studies. In fact, a recent cross-cultural methodological analysis (Bendixen and Purzycki, Reference Bendixen and Purzycki2023b) failed to find clear evidence of within-subject agreement between the three-item ‘moral interest scale’ from Lang et al. (Reference Lang, Purzycki, Apicella, Atkinson, Bolyanatz, Cohen and Henrich2019) and Purzycki et al. (Reference Purzycki, Apicella, Atkinson, Cohen, McNamara, Willard and Henrich2016b) and a corollary free-list task, hinting at a dissociation between these two instruments. While free-lists are more often used for descriptive or exploratory purposes (e.g. informing item scale construction), here we leverage free-list data as a predictor variable in a series of multilevel regression models. This arguably allows for higher resolution and a more ethnographically rich and relevant assessment of individuals’ cultural beliefs and their behavioural implications, compared with forced item responses.

In addition, the studies reviewed above suggest that we should distinguish between a deity's tendency to punish and monitor inter-human behaviour more broadly from a deity's explicitly postulated moral concerns. Here, we investigated whether attributing both moral concern and broad capabilities for punishment and monitoring to a deity (i.e. interaction effects) facilitates cooperative in economic game play to a greater extent compared with these attributes in isolation (i.e. additive effects). That is, does the perceived scope of a deity's capabilities for monitoring and punitiveness moderate the impact on cooperative behaviour of the moral concerns attributed to that deity? Our key hypotheses are as follows:

H1: The more an individual claims their god cares about morality, the more they will exhibit cooperative behaviour.

H2: The more an individual claims their god knows, punishes and cares about morality, the more they will exhibit cooperative behaviour.

In our analyses, cooperation is measured as coin allocations in two different economic games and two permutations of each game (Section 2.2). We operationalise morality according to a pre-specified category of relevant human behaviours and attributes as they appear in our coded free-list data on a locally relevant god's concerns (Section 2.3). We operationalise supernatural punishment and knowledge breadth according to two item scales and hold relevant covariates constant (Section S3.1). In the context of our study, then, H1 entails that free-listing a locally relevant deity as moralistic should predict increased probability of impartial coin allocations. H2 entails that free-listing a locally relevant deity as moralistic while also rating that same deity as punitive and omniscient (i.e. a three-way interaction) predicts increased probability of impartial coin allocations.

We first lay out our methods and data sources (Section 2; see also Section S3), a summary of the main statistical analyses (Section 2.4) and then report results (Section 3). Finally, we discuss key implications of our analysis (Section 4). In the supplementary materials, Sections S1–S3 provide more detail on our causal assumptions, statistical models and data. Section S4 documents deviations from the pre-registered statistical protocol.

The data come from the Evolution of Religion and Morality Project (Lang et al., Reference Lang, Purzycki, Apicella, Atkinson, Bolyanatz, Cohen and Henrich2019; Purzycki et al., Reference Purzycki, Apicella, Atkinson, Cohen, McNamara, Willard and Henrich2016a). The full dataset consists of two waves of data collection across a total of 15 field sites (Table 1). As such, while the present study is novel, parts of the dataset have already been analysed – for site-specific and omnibus reports, see: Tanna, Vanuatu (Atkinson, Reference Atkinson2018; Vardy and Atkinson, Reference Vardy and Atkinson2022), Lovu, Fiji (Willard, Reference Willard2018), Mauritius (Klocová et al., Reference Klocová, Lang, Manŏ, Kundt and Xygalatas2022; Xygalatas et al., Reference Xygalatas, Kotherová, Manŏ, Kundt, Cigán, Klocová and Lang2018), Pesqueiro, Brazil (Cohen et al., Reference Cohen, Baimel and Purzycki2018), Tyva Republic (Purzycki and Kulundary, Reference Purzycki and Kulundary2018), Yasawa, Fiji (McNamara and Henrich, Reference McNamara and Henrich2018; McNamara et al., Reference McNamara, Senanayake, Willard and Henrich2021), Hadza, Tanzania (Apicella, Reference Apicella2018; Stagnaro et al., Reference Stagnaro, Stibbard-Hawkes and Apicella2022), Sursurunga, Papua New Guinea (Bolyanatz, Reference Bolyanatz2022), Mysore, India (Placek and Lightner, Reference Placek and Lightner2022), Cachoeira, Brazil (Soler et al., Reference Soler, Purzycki and Lang2022), Kananga, D. R. Congo (Kapepula et al., Reference Kapepula, Konshi and Weigel2022) and omnibus (Baimel et al., Reference Baimel, Apicella, Atkinson, Bolyanatz, Cohen, Handley and Purzycki2022; Bendixen et al., Reference Bendixen, Apicella, Atkinson, Cohen, Henrich, McNamara and Purzycki2023a; Lang et al., Reference Lang, Purzycki, Apicella, Atkinson, Bolyanatz, Cohen and Henrich2019; Purzycki and Lang, Reference Purzycki and Lang2019; Purzycki et al., Reference Purzycki, Apicella, Atkinson, Cohen, McNamara, Willard and Henrich2016b, Reference Purzycki, Pisor, Apicella, Atkinson, Cohen, Henrich and Xygalatas2018, 2022c; Vardy et al., Reference Vardy, Moya, Placek, Apicella, Bolyanatz, Cohen and Atkinson2022).

Table 1. Selected moralistic deities, primary economy, and cultural group of the anonymous DISTANT recipient in the two economic games for each field site. Game-specific sample sizes: RAG SELF, N = 1033; RAG LOCAL, N = 1028; DG SELF, N = 1077; DG LOCAL, N = 1066.

RAG, Random Allocation Game; DG, Dictator Game.

2.2. Economic games

Our focal outcome measure of cooperation is coin allocations in two economic games, the Random Allocation Game (RAG) and the Dictator Game (DG). The participants played two permutations of each game: SELF vs. DISTANT (i.e. coins are either allocated to the participant themselves or an anonymous, geographically distant co-religionist) and LOCAL vs. DISTANT (i.e. coins are either allocated to a local co-religionist or a distant co-religionist).

Two further conditions were employed in the full protocol: SELF vs. OUTGROUP (i.e. coins are either allocated to the participant themselves or an outgroup member), and DISTANT vs. OUTGROUP (i.e. coins are either allocated to a distant co-religionist or an outgroup member). For the present study, we focus exclusively on the SELF vs. DISTANT and LOCAL vs. DISTANT conditions, since the OUTGROUP conditions entail distinct theoretical predictions (cf. Lang et al., Reference Lang, Purzycki, Apicella, Atkinson, Bolyanatz, Cohen and Henrich2019) that are outside the scope of our current aims.

The RAG is a simple economic game experiment designed to measure impartial rule-following (Hruschka et al., Reference Hruschka, Efferson, Jiang, Falletta-Cowden, Sigurdsson, McNamara and Henrich2014; Jiang, Reference Jiang2013). In our RAG, participants were endowed with 30 coins, a fair die with two outcomes (e.g. black and white) and two cups each designated as the cup of two different recipients, a cup for SELF or LOCAL and another cup for DISTANT depending on condition (see just above). For each coin, participants were asked to think of the recipient to whom they wish to allocate the coin and then roll the die. If the die lands with a particular outcome (e.g. black), then the participant can allocate the coin to the wished-for recipient; otherwise, the coin goes in the opposite cup. Participants know that we distribute the money according to the descriptions on the cups. However, since the identities of the actual recipients (other than SELF) are never revealed and participants play alone, they are in a position to place the coins in whichever cup they want. Since we expect the coin allocations to be binomially distributed with a fair die, systematic deviance from this assumption is interpreted as increased (or decreased) rule-breaking in favour of players or their in-group.

In the DG, participants are endowed with a stack of coins (10 in this case) and are simply asked to allocate it between two individuals (SELF vs. DISTANT or LOCAL vs. DISTANT, depending on game condition) however they like. As with the RAG, the DG is a measure of self/in-group partiality. Playing two different games with the same sample ensures that any result is not isolated to a particular game set-up. Figure 1 shows the raw data distributions of coins allocated to the DISTANT cup across sites and games. Informally and overall, we would expect that participants allocate less coins to the DISTANT cup in the SELF games (i.e. the blue densities), a pattern that we indeed find in most – but not all – sites.

Figure 1. Raw data distributions of Random Allocation Games (RAGs, top) and Dictator Games (DGs, bottom). Densities are number of coins allocated to the DISTANT cup, the focal outcome variables in our analyses, across games and sites. The dashed lines represent the mid-points of the endowment for each game (15 coins for the RAGs, five coins for the DGs).

Across games and conditions, the combined total stakes were set at a local daily wage with a show-up fee of ~25% of the local daily wage. The economic games were played at the outset of the full protocol and the order in which the games were played was counterbalanced.

2.3. Free-listing

Free-listing involves simply asking people to list their associations on some topic, in this case what angers a locally relevant moralistic deity. The earlier an item is listed, the more likely it is to appear in the lists of participants drawn from the same cultural milieu (e.g. Bendixen et al., Reference Bendixen, Apicella, Atkinson, Cohen, Henrich, McNamara and Purzycki2023a; Purzycki, Reference Purzycki2016). Hence, free-listing measures both cognitive and cultural models.

As part of the main study protocol, participants were asked to freely list what they thought angered a locally important ‘moralistic deity’ (i.e. a deity that was pre-selected locally based on ethnographic background interviews to be concerned with inter-personal behaviour; see Table 1). The free-list data were subsequently thematically coded by two pairs of independent coders, one pair for each wave, according to a top-down, twelve-category coding rubric (our ‘general codes’) drawn from Purzycki and McNamara (Reference Purzycki, McNamara, De Cruz and Nichols2016). According to the general coding rubric, ‘Morality’ was coded as generalised behaviours that have a benefit or cost to other people (e.g. hurting, being generous, sharing, etc.) (Bendixen et al., Reference Bendixen, Apicella, Atkinson, Cohen, Henrich, McNamara and Purzycki2023a).

In our analysis, we use free-list responses to predict game behaviour in a series of Bayesian multilevel regression models. As we are interested in measuring the extent to which individuals ascribe moral concern to their deities, we use the proportion of moral items in each participant's lists as our focal predictor (see Section S3.3). Figure 2 plots the cross-cultural distributions of the proportion of free-listed moral items across sites in our sample.

Figure 2. Cross-cultural distributions of supernatural moral beliefs. Densities are proportions of moral items in free-lists on what angers focal deities, the focal predictor variable in our main analyses. Note that the overlap between the plotted data and the samples used for analyses is imperfect, since some participants responded to the free-list task but did not complete one or more of the economic games.

Free-list data were cleaned (see Section S3.2) and then processed with the AnthroTools package (Jamieson-Lane and Purzycki, Reference Jamieson-Lane and Purzycki2016; Purzycki and Jamieson-Lane, Reference Purzycki and Jamieson-Lane2016) for R (R Core Team, 2021). Note that in the full study protocol, in addition to free-listing what they thought angered the locally important ‘moralistic deity,’ participants were also asked to list the kinds of things that please the target deity. Participants were also prompted to list what angers and pleases a ‘local deity’ (i.e. a deity that is locally relevant but pre-selected to be less moralistic, punitive, and knowledgeable) and the police (for an empirical report of the wave 1 free-list data, see Bendixen et al., Reference Bendixen, Apicella, Atkinson, Cohen, Henrich, McNamara and Purzycki2023a). Both kinds of deities were selected for each site on the basis of preliminary ethnographic interviews. For the present study, we focus on the ‘moralistic deities’, since for each field site the frame of the experimental game was explicitly about the religious traditions of those deities. We focus on what angers these deities, since we're interested in their punitive aspects.

2.4. Statistical analyses

We analysed the RAG with a binomial model (see Section S2.1), the DG with an ordered categorical model (Section S2.2) and modelled the SELF and LOCAL games in separate model sets. All models are Bayesian multilevel regression models with weakly regularising prior settings. In the pre-registration, we ran the models on synthetic data to ensure that the models in fact recover key parameters in an ideal scenario under the assumed data-generating process. While we assume no systematic missingness conditional on our covariates, our statistical models employ full Bayesian imputation of missing covariates (McElreath, Reference McElreath2020) in order not to discard data unnecessarily. Key model diagnostics and posterior predictive checks were generally acceptable and are reported in the online supplementary materials (Section S5).

For H1 the key parameter is that of free-listed morality on coin allocation. To assess H2 for each of the outcomes we fit and compare two different models: (1) the theoretically informed ‘interaction model’ including a three-way interaction term between punitiveness, knowledge breadth and free-listed morality as well as its two-way interaction components and main effects; and (2) an ‘additive model’, which excludes all interaction terms but retains the main effects. The additive model then serves as a ‘null model’ to be contrasted with the theoretically informed interaction model. See Sections S1 and S2 for further detail on our causal assumptions and statistical models.

To assess H1, our estimand across analyses is the marginal contrast in posterior predicted probabilities of allocating coins to the DISTANT cup between free-listingMoralityvs. not free-listingMorality’, obtained using g-computation (for further methodological details, see Section S2.3). We summarise the contrasts by their posterior mean and 95% highest posterior density interval (HPDI), the narrowest region of the posterior distribution containing 95% of the parameter estimates. In the RAGs, if the contrast is positive, there's an on-average higher probability of allocating a coin to the DISTANT cup, when listing only moral content (M = 1) among the focal deity's concerns, compared with not free-listing any moral content (M = 0). Likewise in the DGs, for a given number of coins between 0–10, if the contrast is positive, there's an on-average higher probability of allocating that number of coins to the DISTANT cup, when listing only moral content (M = 1) among the focal deity's concerns, compared with not free-listing any moral content (M = 0). Our estimates have a causal interpretation to the extent that relevant identification assumptions are satisfied (see Section S1).

Model code and data were prepared with the rethinking package (McElreath, Reference McElreath2020) for R and fit with Stan via cmdstanr (Carpenter et al., Reference Carpenter, Gelman, Hoffman, Lee, Goodrich, Betancourt and Riddell2017; Gabry et al., Reference Gabry, Cĕsnovar and Johnson2022). See the online supplementary materials (Section S5) for a complete list of R packages, their dependencies and version numbers used for this study.

3. Results

3.1. Do gods’ moral concerns predict cooperation?

For H1, while we find support for the predicted effects in some sites, overall moral concerns do not play a cross-culturally reliable role in motivating cooperative behaviour. Figure 3 illustrates the contrast in posterior predicted probabilities of allocating a coin to the DISTANT cup in the RAGs (upper panel, SELF game; lower panel, LOCAL game) between perceiving the focal deity as maximally moralistic (i.e. free-listing only moral responses, M = 1) vs. not moralistic at all (i.e. not free-listing any moral responses, M = 0). The printed numbers are site-specific mean probabilities of a coin allocation to DISTANT, when M = 0 (with 95% HPDIs in brackets), whereas the distributions (summarised by their mean and 95% HPDIs in black points and lines) are the contrasts when M = 1. Recall that when the contrast is positive (i.e. the blue parts of the distributions), it implies that, on average, participants were more likely to allocate a coin to the DISTANT cup, if they free-listed the focal deity as moralistic compared with not free-listing moral responses. When the contrast is negative (i.e. the grey parts of the distributions), the opposite relationship is implied on average.

Figure 3. Random Allocation Games, SELF (top) and LOCAL (bottom). Marginal contrasts in posterior predicted probabilities of allocating a coin to the DISTANT cup (in percentage points). If the contrast is positive (blue), there's an on-average higher probability of allocating a coin to the DISTANT cup, when free-listing only moral content (M = 1) among the focal deity's concerns. If the contrast is negative (grey), there is an on-average higher probability of allocating a coin to the DISTANT cup, when not free-listing moral content (M = 0) among the focal deity's concerns. Printed numbers are site-specific posterior predicted probabilities of a coin allocation to DISTANT, when M = 0 with 95% highest posterior density intervals (HPDIs) in brackets. 0% (dashed line) means no difference. Posterior means and 95% HPDIs in black. Colour gradients reflect posterior mass. Distributions are normalised.

For the SELF RAG, several sites trend in the predicted positive direction (particularly Hadza, Huatasani, Mauritius, and Yasawa) such that the bulk of the posterior masses are positive. At these sites, people are on average around 5 percentage points more likely to allocate a coin to the DISTANT cup if they free-list their focal deity as maximally moralistic, although the estimates are also consistent with little-to-no effect, as indicated by the HPDIs. Other sites have posterior means close to zero or slightly below. Overall, these results suggest the absence of a reliable, cross-cultural effect. The same inference can be drawn for the LOCAL RAG, although here a null effect is arguably more evident, in that the posterior mass is in almost all cases centred on or close to zero.

Results are similar for the DGs. Figure 4 plots for the DGs (upper panel, SELF game; lower panel, LOCAL game) and for each site the contrast in posterior predicted probabilities of allocating 0–10 coins (x-axis) to the DISTANT cup when perceiving the focal deity as maximally moralistic (i.e. free-listing only moral responses, M = 1) compared with not moralistic at all (i.e. not free-listing any moral responses, M = 0). Blue points and lines imply, for any given number of coins, a positive posterior mean contrast such that participants were on average more likely to allocate this particular number of coins if they free-listed the focal deity as maximally moralistic.

Figure 4. Dictator SELF (top) and LOCAL (bottom) Games. Marginal contrasts in posterior predicted probabilities of allocating 0–10 coins (x-axis) to the DISTANT cup (in percentage points). For a given number of coins, if the posterior mean contrast is positive (blue), there's an on-average higher probability of allocating that number of coins to the DISTANT cup, when free-listing only moral content (M = 1) among the focal deity's concerns. If the posterior mean contrast is negative (grey), there's an on-average higher probability of allocating that number of coins to the DISTANT cup, when not free-listing moral content (M = 0) among the focal deity's concerns. 0% (dashed line) means no difference. Points are posterior means and lines are 95% HPDIs.

If participants, who perceive the focal deity as moralistic, were more likely to be cooperative towards the DISTANT co-player, we would expect, then, a positive contrast (i.e. blue points and lines) for the higher coin allocations. While we do find this predicted relationship at a few sites (in the SELF game, Huatasani, Mysore, Sursurunga; and in the LOCAL game, Cachoeira, Coastal Tanna), at no sites are the results unequivocal: the posterior means are small in almost all cases. Hadza is the most striking exception; there, participants are predicted to be more likely to allocate no coins to the DISTANT cup on average if they free-list their focal deity as maximally moralistic – although note the wide and uncertain HPDIs.

As for the tight intervals around the higher coin allocations at several sites, these result from the cumulative nature of the models combined with a lower number of participants putting many coins in the DISTANT cup. That is, on the basis of these data, the models are quite confident that the differences in probability between listing only vs. not listing supernatural moral concerns are generally small for the higher coin allocations, because relatively few participants allocated many coins to the DISTANT cup (see also Figure 1).

To recap, then, even if some sites exhibit trends in the predicted direction, we fail to find reliable evidence overall of a clear positive relationship between perceiving a focal deity as moralistic and increased cooperation toward an anonymous, distant co-religionist. This inference did not change across various model specifications, as discussed in Section 4 (see also online supplementary materials, Section S5).

3.2. Interaction effect between gods’ punishment, knowledge and moral concerns

For H2, we assessed the regression coefficients of the interaction terms, their implied predictions on the probability scale as well as their model comparison metrics (see Section S2). While the main effects of punitiveness and, in particular, knowledge breadth were generally positive across model specifications (in line with Lang et al., Reference Lang, Purzycki, Apicella, Atkinson, Bolyanatz, Cohen and Henrich2019; Purzycki et al., Reference Purzycki, Apicella, Atkinson, Cohen, McNamara, Willard and Henrich2016b), in no case did we find robust evidence for any interaction effect on any of the model comparison metrics. This is possibly owing in part to lack of sufficient statistical power (as also noted in the pre-registration), and in part to small, heterogeneous or non-existent effects. We nonetheless report from the interaction models here, since they are theoretically more informed. In any case, the interaction and additive models support qualitatively similar inferences, apart from slightly higher precision in estimates in the additive model.

4. General discussion

In this study, we assessed with individual-level data – across 15 diverse field sites and two permutations of each of two behavioural economic games – the importance of moral content in moralistic supernatural punishment beliefs and its role in motivating cooperative behaviour.

Specifically, we assessed (H1) whether free-listing a locally relevant moralistic deity as angered by breaches of morality predicts increased cooperation towards an anonymous, distant co-religionist. While some field sites exhibited trends in the predicted direction, overall we failed to find reliable evidence in favour of H1 across games and sites. This finding is consistent with previous studies (Lang et al., Reference Lang, Purzycki, Apicella, Atkinson, Bolyanatz, Cohen and Henrich2019; Purzycki et al., Reference Purzycki, Apicella, Atkinson, Cohen, McNamara, Willard and Henrich2016b), which found that rating a relevant god as concerned with punishing theft, lying and murder does not reliably predict cooperation. We also tested (H2) for the presence of interaction effects with the focal deity's perceived degree of punitiveness and knowledge breadth; that is, whether the effect on economic game play of ascribing moral content was modified by these more general supernatural traits (reported in the online supplementary materials, see Section S5). While the main effects of punitiveness and, in particular, knowledge breadth were generally positive across model specifications (in line with Lang et al., Reference Lang, Purzycki, Apicella, Atkinson, Bolyanatz, Cohen and Henrich2019; Purzycki et al., Reference Purzycki, Apicella, Atkinson, Cohen, McNamara, Willard and Henrich2016b), our study was inconclusive regarding their interactions, presumably owing to both insufficient statistical power and small, variable or non-existent effects.

In three sets of supplementary analyses, we explored a few additional questions (reported in the online supplementary materials, see Section S5). First, we assessed whether our main conclusions would change if instead of free-listed moral concern we used an index of moral concern measured with a set of scale items on particular moral offenses from Lang et al. (Reference Lang, Purzycki, Apicella, Atkinson, Bolyanatz, Cohen and Henrich2019) and Purzycki et al. (Reference Purzycki, Apicella, Atkinson, Cohen, McNamara, Willard and Henrich2016b) (see Section S3.5). While neither Lang et al. (Reference Lang, Purzycki, Apicella, Atkinson, Bolyanatz, Cohen and Henrich2019) nor Purzycki et al. (Reference Purzycki, Apicella, Atkinson, Cohen, McNamara, Willard and Henrich2016b) found a robust relationship between this ‘moral interest scale’ and the behavioural economic games, our statistical approach differed from theirs sufficiently to warrant a pre-registered re-analysis. Second, since the general free-list code ‘Morality’ captures responses that might not directly translate to the economic game contexts (e.g. ‘murder’, ‘violence’, etc.), we ran another set of analyses where we used a more narrow set of free-list responses as the main predictor. This set of free-list responses was preregistered to specifically pertain to resource and social exchange and therefore arguably have higher face validity in the context of the present study (see Section S3.4). Thus, the rationale for this analysis was to explore whether perceptions of supernatural punishment that are more relevant to the game contexts, compared with morality in general, more clearly predict cooperation in the games (see further discussion below). Third, we expanded the predictor of the main analysis such that it also included instances of the general free-list coding category ‘Virtue’, which overlaps conceptually with the ‘Morality’ coding category; a free-list response qualified as ‘Virtue’ if it satisfied the following: individual qualities that may or may not have social ramifications (e.g. hard-working, kind, bad conscience, etc.; see Bendixen et al., Reference Bendixen, Apicella, Atkinson, Cohen, Henrich, McNamara and Purzycki2023a). While this latter analysis was not pre-registered, there is precedence in the published literature for lumping these two coding categories (e.g. Bendixen et al., Reference Bendixen, Apicella, Atkinson, Cohen, Henrich, McNamara and Purzycki2023a; Purzycki and McNamara, Reference Purzycki, McNamara, De Cruz and Nichols2016; Purzycki et al., Reference Purzycki, Apicella, Atkinson, Cohen, McNamara, Willard and Henrich2016b). These supplementary analyses did not yield qualitatively different results than reported here.

What do we make of these results in the context of the family of theories on moralistic supernatural punishment beliefs reviewed at the outset? We first discuss a few methodological considerations before offering some more general reflections on this research program.

Note that in the SELF RAG (Figure 3, top), the more prominent positive effects appear when the probability of allocating coins to the DISTANT cup is generally lower (posterior means < 40% when M = 0; Hadza, Huatasani, Yasawa). With the RAG being a measure of fairness and impartial rule-following, it may be that supernatural moralistic beliefs are mostly effective at nudging people closer to playing fairly (e.g. getting closer to ~50% in the RAG) rather than making people overly cooperative (e.g. getting above 50% in the RAG). As for the LOCAL RAG (Figure 3, bottom), we see more diffuse effects. We speculate whether this could be due to a view that the morally ‘right’ thing to do is sometimes favouring a local co-religionist over a distant co-religionist – or at least that some participants and sites were guided by such intuition. These considerations emphasise a more general take-home point, namely that these kinds of data often call for site-specific contextualisation (see references to site-specific reports above). This inference is in line with the DG results, where we failed to capture a clear omnibus pattern across sites and game type (Figure 4). It's possible that unmodelled site-specific variation is more important when there are little-to-no game constraints, as is the case in the DG compared with the RAG.

Another possibility is that the artificial game set-ups do not map onto salient mental models of the deities in their relevant cultural contexts and that the games therefore fail to reflect relevant behavioural cues (cf. Cronk, Reference Cronk2007; Lightner et al., Reference Lightner, Barclay and Hagen2017). Recent work (Bendixen et al., Reference Bendixen, Apicella, Atkinson, Cohen, Henrich, McNamara and Purzycki2023a; McNamara and Purzycki, Reference McNamara and Purzycki2020; Purzycki et al., Reference Purzycki, Bendixen, Lightner and Sosis2022b) has emphasised that a fit between perceived supernatural concerns and particular socioecological dilemmas might be required for religious appeals and beliefs to have corresponding behavioural consequences. On that perspective, general moral infringements, as captured by our ‘Morality’ free-list code and by the ‘moral interest scale’ from Lang et al. (Reference Lang, Purzycki, Apicella, Atkinson, Bolyanatz, Cohen and Henrich2019) and Purzycki et al. (Reference Purzycki, Apicella, Atkinson, Cohen, McNamara, Willard and Henrich2016b), might not be directly relevant to the experimental settings of the RAGs and DGs. To study religious systems in their culturally relevant context, rather than relying exclusively on standardised economic games, future empirical studies could evaluate this socioecological view by catering data collection to relevant local settings (Bendixen and Purzycki, Reference Bendixen and Purzycki2023c), for instance through natural experiments (e.g. Atran et al., Reference Atran, Medin, Ross, Lynch, Vapnarsky, Ek’ and Baran2002), ethnographically salient games (e.g. Townsend et al., Reference Townsend, Aktipis, Balliet and Cronk2020) and vignette studies (e.g. Purzycki and Arakchaa, Reference Purzycki and Arakchaa2013).

To the extent that our methods do capture relevant mental and behavioural processes of moralistic supernatural punishment beliefs, our study – in line with and building on Lang et al. (Reference Lang, Purzycki, Apicella, Atkinson, Bolyanatz, Cohen and Henrich2019) and Purzycki et al. (Reference Purzycki, Apicella, Atkinson, Cohen, McNamara, Willard and Henrich2016b) – suggests that supernatural moral concerns do not play a direct, cross-culturally reliable role in motivating cooperative actions, above and beyond a deity's perceived capabilities for punishment and monitoring more generally. Rather than driving behaviour directly, postulated supernatural concerns may instead make salient certain classes of collective action problems that religious systems adapt to – and perhaps address – through more indirect pathways (Bendixen and Purzycki, Reference Bendixen, Purzycki, Purzycki and Bendixen2023a; Bendixen et al., Reference Bendixen, Apicella, Atkinson, Cohen, Henrich, McNamara and Purzycki2023a; Purzycki et al., Reference Purzycki, Bendixen, Lightner and Sosis2022b).

Indeed, many lines of evidence suggest that appeals to, beliefs about, and rituals directed towards the supernatural support social and ecological life-ways in diverse human societies (e.g. Bulbulia and Sosis, Reference Bulbulia and Sosis2011; Leeson and Suarez, Reference Leeson and Suarez2015; Rossano, Reference Rossano2007). However, above and beyond a few particularly focused studies (e.g. Ge et al., Reference Ge, Chen, Wu and Mace2019; Lang et al., Reference Lang, Purzycki, Apicella, Atkinson, Bolyanatz, Cohen and Henrich2019; Power, Reference Power2017; Purzycki et al., Reference Purzycki, Apicella, Atkinson, Cohen, McNamara, Willard and Henrich2016b; Townsend et al., Reference Townsend, Aktipis, Balliet and Cronk2020; Xygalatas et al., Reference Xygalatas, Mitkidis, Fischer, Reddish, Skewes, Geertz, Roepstorff and Bulbulia2013), many ethnographically informed, individual-level findings remain primarily circumstantial (e.g. Atran et al., Reference Atran, Medin, Ross, Lynch, Vapnarsky, Ek’ and Baran2002; Bendixen et al., Reference Bendixen, Apicella, Atkinson, Cohen, Henrich, McNamara and Purzycki2023a; Purzycki and Arakchaa, Reference Purzycki and Arakchaa2013; Singh et al., Reference Singh, Kaptchuk and Henrich2021). Future studies seeking to quantify the effect of supernatural punishment beliefs could build on these efforts by ensuring that the mental models and implied behavioural patterns under investigation map onto ethnographically salient and ecologically valid contexts (Bendixen and Purzycki, Reference Bendixen and Purzycki2023c). This requires acute ethnographic attention to the constraints and affordances of the local socioecology as well as, through careful study design and statistical analysis, adhering to rigorous principles of contemporary causal inference.

Acknowledgements

TB, ADL and BGP thank Aarhus University Research Foundation for financial support and Cecilie Sandfeld and Andreas Damgaard for coding the free-list data.

Author contributions

BGP and TB conceptualised the study. TB led writing and analysis and ADL, ML and BGP provided extensive feedback. The Evolution of Religion and Morality Project was designed by JH, AN and BGP and managed by ML and BGP. CA, QA, AB, EC, CH, EKK, CL, SM, RAM, CM, CP, MS, TV, JW, AKW, DX, ML and BGP collected data.

Financial support

We acknowledge funding from a research grant, ‘The Emergence of Prosocial Religions’ from the John Templeton Foundation, and the Cultural Evolution of Religion Research Consortium, funded by a generous partnership grant (895-2011-1009) from the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada. CH and SM were funded by the John Templeton Foundation grant no. 48952. TB, ADL and BGP acknowledge funding from Aarhus University Research Foundation.

Conflict of interest

No conflict of interest.

Research transparency and reproducibility

Pre-registration materials, supplementary analyses and plots, as well as data and analysis scripts for this study are openly available at: https://github.com/tbendixen/moral-freelist-econ .

Supplementary material

Supplemental materials are available at https://doi.org/10.1017/ehs.2023.15.

References

Apicella, C. L. (2018). High levels of rule-bending in a minimally religious and largely egalitarian forager population. Religion, Brain & Behavior, 8(2), 133148.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Atkinson, Q. D. (2018). Religion and expanding the cooperative sphere in Kastom and Christian villages on Tanna, Vanuatu. Religion, Brain & Behavior, 8(2), 149167.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Atkinson, Q. D., & Bourrat, P. (2011). Beliefs about God, the afterlife and morality support the role of supernatural policing in human cooperation. Evolution and Human Behavior, 32(1), 4149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Atran, S., Medin, D., Ross, N., Lynch, E., Vapnarsky, V., Ek’, E. U., … Baran, M. (2002). Folkecology, cultural epidemiology, and the spirit of the commons: A garden experiment in the Maya Lowlands, 1991–2001. Current Anthropology, 43(3), 421450.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baimel, A., Apicella, C., Atkinson, Q., Bolyanatz, A., Cohen, E., Handley, C., … Purzycki, B. (2022). Material insecurity predicts greater commitment to moralistic and less commitment to local deities: A cross-cultural investigation. Religion, Brain & Behavior, 12(1–2), 417.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beheim, B., Atkinson, Q. D., Bulbulia, J., Gervais, W., Gray, R. D., Henrich, J., … Willard, A. K. (2021). Treatment of missing data determined conclusions regarding moralizing gods. Nature, 595(7866), E29E34.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bendixen, T., Apicella, C., Atkinson, Q., Cohen, E., Henrich, J., McNamara, R. A., … Purzycki, B. G. (2023a). Appealing to the minds of gods: Religious beliefs and appeals correspond to features of local social ecologies. Religion, Brain & behavior.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bendixen, T., Lightner, A. D., & Purzycki, B. G. (2023b). The cultural evolution of religion and cooperation. In Kendal, R., Tehrani, J., & Kendal, J. (Eds.), Oxford handbook of cultural evolution. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bendixen, T., & Purzycki, B. G. (2023a). Accounting for cross-cultural variation in the minds of gods. In Purzycki, B. G., & Bendixen, T. (Eds.), The minds of gods: New horizons in the naturalistic study of religion (pp. 149158). Bloomsbury.Google Scholar
Bendixen, T., & Purzycki, B. G. (2023b). Cognitive and cultural models in psychological science: A tutorial on modeling free-list data as a dependent variable in Bayesian regression. Psychological Methods.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bendixen, T., & Purzycki, B. G. (2023c). Cultural evolution of gods minds: Response to commentators. Religion, Brain & Behavior.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bloom, P. (2012). Religion, morality, evolution. Annual Review of Psychology, 63(1), 179199.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boehm, C. (2008). A biocultural evolutionary exploration of supernatural sanctioning. In Bulbulia, J., Sosis, R., Harris, E., Genet, R., and Wyman, K. (Eds.), Evolution of religion: Studies, theories, and critiques (pp. 143152). Collins Foundation Press.Google Scholar
Bolyanatz, A. H. (2022). When god is watching: Dictator game results from the Sursurunga of New Ireland, Papua New Guinea. Religion, Brain & Behavior, 12(1–2), 6178.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Botero, C. A., Gardner, B., Kirby, K. R., Bulbulia, J., Gavin, M. C., & Gray, R. D. (2014). The ecology of religious beliefs. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111(47), 1678416789.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bulbulia, J., & Sosis, R. (2011). Signalling theory and the evolution of religious cooperation. Religion, 41(3), 363388.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caluori, N., Jackson, J. C., Gray, K., & Gelfand, M. (2020). Conflict changes how people view god. Psychological Science, 31(3), 280292.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carpenter, B., Gelman, A., Hoffman, M., Lee, D., Goodrich, B., Betancourt, M., … Riddell, A. (2017). Stan: A probabilistic programming language. Journal of Statistical Software, 76(1), 132.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, E., Baimel, A., & Purzycki, B. G. (2018). Religiosity and resource allocation in Marajó, Brazil. Religion, Brain Behavior, 8(2), 168184.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cronk, L. (2007). The influence of cultural framing on play in the trust game: A Maasai example. Evolution and Human Behavior, 28(5), 352358.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Durkheim, E. (2001 [1912]). The elementary forms of religious life/Emile Durkheim, transl. C. Cosman. Oxford World's Classics. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Ellwood, C. A. (1918). Religion and social control. The Scientific Monthly, 7(4), 335348.Google Scholar
Evans-Pritchard, E. E. (1965). Theories of primitive religion. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gabry, J., Cĕsnovar, R., & Johnson, A. (2022). cmdstanr: R interface to ‘CmdStan’. https://mc-stan.org/cmdstanr/, https://discourse.mc-stan.org.Google Scholar
Galen, L. W. (2012). Does religious belief promote prosociality? A critical examination. Psychological Bulletin, 138(5), 876906. American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ge, E., Chen, Y., Wu, J., & Mace, R. (2019). Large-scale cooperation driven by reputation, not fear of divine punishment. Royal Society Open Science, 6(8), 190991.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hayden, B. (1987). Alliances and ritual ecstasy: Human responses to resource stress. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 26(1), 8191.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hruschka, D., Efferson, C., Jiang, T., Falletta-Cowden, A., Sigurdsson, S., McNamara, R., … Henrich, J. (2014). Impartial institutions, pathogen stress and the expanding social network. Human Nature, 25(4), 567579.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jackson, J. C., Caluori, N., Abrams, S., Beckman, E., Gelfand, M., & Gray, K. (2021). Tight cultures and vengeful gods: How culture shapes religious belief. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jamieson-Lane, A., & Purzycki, B. G. (2016). AnthroTools: Some custom tools for anthropology. R package version 0.8.Google Scholar
Jiang, T. (2013). Cheating in mind games: The subtlety of rules matters. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 93, 328336.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, D. D. P. (2016). God is watching you: How the fear of god makes us human. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kapepula, G. T., Konshi, M. M., & Weigel, J. L. (2022). Prosociality and pentecostalism in the D.R. Congo. Religion, Brain & Behavior, 12(1–2), 150170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klocová, E. K., Lang, M., Manŏ, P., Kundt, R., & Xygalatas, D. (2022). Cigarettes for the dead: Effects of sorcery beliefs on parochial prosociality in mauritius. Religion, Brain & Behavior, 12(1–2), 116131.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lang, A. (1909). The making of religion. Longmans.Google Scholar
Lang, M., Purzycki, B. G., Apicella, C. L., Atkinson, Q. D., Bolyanatz, A., Cohen, E., & Henrich, J. (2019). Moralizing gods, impartiality and religious parochialism across 15 societies. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 286(1898), 20190202.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leeson, P. T., & Suarez, P. A. (2015). Superstition and self-governance. New Thinking in Austrian Political Economy, 19, 4766.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lightner, A., Bendixen, T., & Purzycki, B. (2022). Moralistic supernatural punishment is probably not associated with social complexity. Evolution and Human Behavior, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2022.10.006.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lightner, A. D., Barclay, P., & Hagen, E. H. (2017). Radical framing effects in the ultimatum game: The impact of explicit culturally transmitted frames on economic decisionmaking. Royal Society Open Science, 4(12), 170543.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Malinowski, B. (1936). The foundations of faith and morals. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
McElreath, R. (2020). Statistical rethinking: A Bayesian course with examples in R and Stan (2nd ed.). CRC Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McKay, R., & Whitehouse, H. (2014). Religion and morality. Psychological Bulletin.Google ScholarPubMed
McNamara, R. A., & Henrich, J. (2018). Jesus vs. the ancestors: How specific religious beliefs shape prosociality on Yasawa Island, Fiji. Religion, Brain & behavior, 8(2), 185204.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McNamara, R. A., & Purzycki, B. G. (2020). Minds of gods and human cognitive constraints: Socio-ecological context shapes belief. Religion, Brain & Behavior, 10(3), 223238.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McNamara, R. A., Senanayake, R., Willard, A. K., & Henrich, J. (2021). God's mind on morality. Evolutionary Human Sciences, 3, E6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Norenzayan, A., Shariff, A. F., Gervais, W. M., Willard, A. K., McNamara, R. A., Slingerland, E., & Henrich, J. (2016). The cultural evolution of prosocial religions. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 39.Google ScholarPubMed
Placek, C., & Lightner, A. (2022). Moralizing gods, local gods, and complexity in Hindu god concepts: Evidence from South India. Religion, Brain & Behavior, 12(1–2), 97115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Power, E. A. (2017). Social support networks and religiosity in rural South India. Nature Human Behaviour, 1, 0057.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Purzycki, B. G. (2011). Tyvan cher eezi and the socioecological constraints of supernatural agents’ minds. Religion, Brain & Behavior, 1(1), 3145.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Purzycki, B. G. (2013). The minds of gods: A comparative study of supernatural agency. Cognition, 129(1), 163179.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Purzycki, B. G. (2016). The evolution of gods’ minds in the Tyva Republic. Current Anthropology, 57(S13), S88S104.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Purzycki, B. G., Apicella, C., Atkinson, Q. D., Cohen, E., McNamara, R. A., Willard, A. K., … Henrich, J. (2016a). Cross-cultural dataset for the evolution of religion and morality project. Scientific Data, 3, 160099.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Purzycki, B. G., Apicella, C., Atkinson, Q. D., Cohen, E., McNamara, R. A., Willard, A. K., … Henrich, J. (2016b). Moralistic gods, supernatural punishment and the expansion of human sociality. Nature, 530(7590), 327330.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Purzycki, B. G., & Arakchaa, T. (2013). Ritual behavior and trust in the Tyva Republic. Current Anthropology, 54(3), 381388.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Purzycki, B. G., Bendixen, T., & Lightner, A. D. (2022a). Coding, causality, and statistical craft: The emergence and evolutionary drivers of moralistic supernatural punishment remain unresolved. Religion, Brain & Behavior, 13(2), 207214.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Purzycki, B. G., Bendixen, T., Lightner, A. D., & Sosis, R. (2022b). Gods, games, and the socioecological landscape. Current Research in Ecological and Social Psychology, 3, 100057.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Purzycki, B. G., & Jamieson-Lane, A. (2016). Anthrotools: An R package for cross-cultural ethnographic data analysis. Cross-Cultural Research, 51(1), 5174.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Purzycki, B. G., & Kulundary, V. (2018). Buddhism, identity, and class: Fairness and favoritism in the Tyva Republic. Religion, Brain & Behavior, 8(2), 205226.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Purzycki, B. G., & Lang, M. (2019). Identity fusion, outgroup relations, and sacrifice: A cross-cultural test. Cognition, 186, 16.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Purzycki, B. G., & McKay, R. (2023). Moralistic gods and social complexity: A brief history of the problem. In Purzycki, B. G., & Bendixen, T. (Eds.), The minds of gods: New horizons in the naturalistic study of religion (pp. 121132). Bloomsbury.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Purzycki, B. G., & McNamara, R. A. (2016). An ecological theory of gods’ minds. In De Cruz, H., & Nichols, R. (Eds.), Cognitive science of religion and its philosophical implications (pp. 143167). Continuum.Google Scholar
Purzycki, B. G., Pisor, A., Apicella, C., Atkinson, Q. D., Cohen, E., Henrich, J., … Xygalatas, D. (2018). The cognitive and cultural foundations of moral behavior. Evolution and Human Behavior, 39(5), 490501.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Purzycki, B. G., Willard, A. K., Klocová, E. K., Apicella, C., Atkinson, Q., Bolyanatz, A., … Ross, C. T. (2022c). The moralization bias of gods’ minds: A cross-cultural test. Religion, Brain and Behavior, 12(1–2), 3860.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Quinlan, M. B. (2017). The freelisting method. Handbook of research methods in health social sciences (pp. 116). Springer.Google Scholar
Rappaport, R. A. (1968). Pigs for the ancestors: Ritual in the ecology of a New Guinea People. Yale University Press.Google Scholar
R Core Team (2021). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing.Google Scholar
Roes, F. L., & Raymond, M. (2003). Belief in moralizing gods. Evolution and Human Behavior, 24(2), 126135.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rossano, M. J. (2007). Supernaturalizing social life: Religion and the evolution of human cooperation. Human Nature, 18(3), 272294.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schloss, J. P., & Murray, M. J. (2011). Evolutionary accounts of belief in supernatural punishment: a critical review. Religion, Brain & Behavior, 1(1), 4699.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shariff, A. F., & Norenzayan, A. (2007). God is watching you: Priming god concepts increases prosocial behavior in an anonymous economic game. Psychological Science, 18(9), 803809.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shariff, A. F., Willard, A. K., Andersen, T., & Norenzayan, A. (2016). Religious priming: A meta-analysis with a focus on prosociality. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 20(1), 2748.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Singh, M., Kaptchuk, T. J., & Henrich, J. (2021). Small gods, rituals, and cooperation: The Mentawai water spirit Sikameinan. Evolution and Human Behavior, 42(1), 6172.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skali, A. (2017). Moralizing gods and armed conflict. Journal of Economic Psychology, 63:184198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skoggard, I., Ember, C. R., Pitek, E., Jackson, J. C., & Carolus, C. (2020). Resource stress predicts changes in religious belief and increases in sharing behavior. Human Nature, 31(3), 249271.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Snarey, J. (1996). The natural environment's impact upon religious ethics: A cross-cultural study. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 35(2), 8596.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Soler, M., Purzycki, B. G., & Lang, M. (2022). Perceptions of moralizing agents and cooperative behavior in northeastern brazil. Religion, Brain & Behavior, 12(1–2), 132149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stagnaro, M. N., Stibbard-Hawkes, D. N. E., & Apicella, C. L. (2022). Do religious and market-based institutions promote cooperation in hadza hunter-gatherers? Religion, Brain & Behavior, 12(1–2), 171189.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swanson, G. E. (1960). The birth of the gods: The origin of primitive beliefs. University of Michigan Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Townsend, C., Aktipis, A., Balliet, D., & Cronk, L. (2020). Generosity among the Ik of Uganda. Evolutionary Human Sciences, 2, E23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vardy, T., & Atkinson, Q. D. (2022). Moralistic and local god beliefs and the extent of prosocial preferences on Tanna Island, Vanuatu. Religion, Brain & Behavior, 12(1–2), 7996.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vardy, T., Moya, C., Placek, C. D., Apicella, C. L., Bolyanatz, A., Cohen, E., … Atkinson, Q. D. (2022). The religiosity gender gap in 14 diverse societies. Religion, Brain & Behavior, 12(1–2), 1837.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wallace, A. (1966). Religion: An anthropological view. Random House.Google Scholar
Watts, J., Greenhill, S. J., Atkinson, Q. D., Currie, T. E., Bulbulia, J., & Gray, R. D. (2015). Broad supernatural punishment but not moralizing high gods precede the evolution of political complexity in Austronesia. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 282(1804), 20142556.Google Scholar
White, C. J., Kelly, J. M., Shariff, A. F., & Norenzayan, A. (2019). Supernatural norm enforcement: Thinking about karma and god reduces selfishness among believers. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 84, 103797.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Willard, A. K. (2018). Religion and prosocial behavior among the Indo-Fijians. Religion, Brain & Behavior, 8(2), 227242.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Xygalatas, D., Kotherová, S., Manŏ, P., Kundt, R., Cigán, J., Klocová, E. K., & Lang, M. (2018). Big gods in small places: The random allocation game in mauritius. Religion, Brain & Behavior, 8(2), 243261.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Xygalatas, D., Mitkidis, P., Fischer, R., Reddish, P., Skewes, J., Geertz, A. W., Roepstorff, A., & Bulbulia, J. (2013). Extreme rituals promote prosociality. Psychological Science, 24(8), 16021605.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yilmaz, O., & Bahçekapili, H. G. (2016). Supernatural and secular monitors promote human cooperation only if they remind of punishment. Evolution and Human Behavior, 37(1), 7984.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Selected moralistic deities, primary economy, and cultural group of the anonymous DISTANT recipient in the two economic games for each field site. Game-specific sample sizes: RAG SELF, N = 1033; RAG LOCAL, N = 1028; DG SELF, N = 1077; DG LOCAL, N = 1066.

Figure 1

Figure 1. Raw data distributions of Random Allocation Games (RAGs, top) and Dictator Games (DGs, bottom). Densities are number of coins allocated to the DISTANT cup, the focal outcome variables in our analyses, across games and sites. The dashed lines represent the mid-points of the endowment for each game (15 coins for the RAGs, five coins for the DGs).

Figure 2

Figure 2. Cross-cultural distributions of supernatural moral beliefs. Densities are proportions of moral items in free-lists on what angers focal deities, the focal predictor variable in our main analyses. Note that the overlap between the plotted data and the samples used for analyses is imperfect, since some participants responded to the free-list task but did not complete one or more of the economic games.

Figure 3

Figure 3. Random Allocation Games, SELF (top) and LOCAL (bottom). Marginal contrasts in posterior predicted probabilities of allocating a coin to the DISTANT cup (in percentage points). If the contrast is positive (blue), there's an on-average higher probability of allocating a coin to the DISTANT cup, when free-listing only moral content (M = 1) among the focal deity's concerns. If the contrast is negative (grey), there is an on-average higher probability of allocating a coin to the DISTANT cup, when not free-listing moral content (M = 0) among the focal deity's concerns. Printed numbers are site-specific posterior predicted probabilities of a coin allocation to DISTANT, when M = 0 with 95% highest posterior density intervals (HPDIs) in brackets. 0% (dashed line) means no difference. Posterior means and 95% HPDIs in black. Colour gradients reflect posterior mass. Distributions are normalised.

Figure 4

Figure 4. Dictator SELF (top) and LOCAL (bottom) Games. Marginal contrasts in posterior predicted probabilities of allocating 0–10 coins (x-axis) to the DISTANT cup (in percentage points). For a given number of coins, if the posterior mean contrast is positive (blue), there's an on-average higher probability of allocating that number of coins to the DISTANT cup, when free-listing only moral content (M = 1) among the focal deity's concerns. If the posterior mean contrast is negative (grey), there's an on-average higher probability of allocating that number of coins to the DISTANT cup, when not free-listing moral content (M = 0) among the focal deity's concerns. 0% (dashed line) means no difference. Points are posterior means and lines are 95% HPDIs.

Supplementary material: PDF

Bendixen et al. supplementary material

Bendixen et al. supplementary material

Download Bendixen et al. supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 428.1 KB