Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-dzt6s Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-27T11:50:48.821Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The ontogeny of exploratory object manipulation behaviour in wild orangutans

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 July 2021

Caroline Schuppli*
Affiliation:
Development and Evolution of Cognition Research Group, Max Planck Institute for Animal Behavior, Bücklestrasse 5a, 78467 Konstanz, Germany Department of Anthropology, University of Zürich, Winterthurerstrasse 190, 8057 Zürich, Switzerland
Anaïs Van Cauwenberghe
Affiliation:
Development and Evolution of Cognition Research Group, Max Planck Institute for Animal Behavior, Bücklestrasse 5a, 78467 Konstanz, Germany Department of Anthropology, University of Zürich, Winterthurerstrasse 190, 8057 Zürich, Switzerland
Tatang Mitra Setia
Affiliation:
Department of Biology, Graduate School and Faculty of Biology, Universitas Nasional, Jl. Sawo Manila, RT.14/RW.3, Ps. Minggu, DKI Jakarta, Indonesia
Daniel Haun
Affiliation:
Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Deutscher Platz 6, 04103 Leipzig, Germany
*
*Corresponding author. E-mail: caroline.schuppli@aim.uzh.ch

Abstract

In human infants, exploratory object manipulation is a major vehicle for cognitive stimulation as well as an important way to learn about objects and basic physical concepts in general. The development of human infants’ exploratory object manipulation follows distinct developmental patterns. So far, the degree of evolutionary continuity of this developmental process remains unclear. We investigated the development of exploratory object manipulations in wild orangutans. Our data included 3200 exploration events collected on 13 immatures between the ages of 0.5 and 13 years, at the Suaq Balimbing monitoring station in Indonesia. Our results identify several parallels between the development of exploratory behaviour in humans and orangutans: on top of a highly similar overall age trajectory, we found an increase in variability of the actions used, an increase in the number of body parts involved in each event, and an overall decrease of mouthing of the objects. All in all, our results show that orangutans progress through a developmental sequence of different aspects of exploration behaviour. In combination with previous findings from captivity, our results also provide evidence that exploratory object manipulations reflect cognitive development and might function as a means of cognitive stimulation not just in humans but across the great apes.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Social media summary: Young orangutans go through a distinct developmental sequence of exploratory object manipulation behaviour resembling the human pattern.

1. Introduction

Human infants learn about the physical properties of the world around them through exploration, i.e., the deliberate manipulation and visual investigation of objects (Fenson & Schell, Reference Fenson and Schell1985; Muentener et al., Reference Muentener, Herrig and Schulz2018; Power, Reference Power1999; Ruff et al., Reference Ruff, Saltarelli, Capozzoli and Dubiner1992). By examining objects, human infants gain knowledge about their properties as well as about the general principles of the physical world linked to them (Baldwin et al., Reference Baldwin, Markman and Melartin1993; Baumgartner & Oakes, Reference Baumgartner and Oakes2011, Reference Baumgartner and Oakes2013; Bourgeois et al., Reference Bourgeois, Khawar, Neal and Lockman2005; Fenson & Schell, Reference Fenson and Schell1985; Power, Reference Power1999). Exploratory object manipulation is also an important vehicle of cognitive stimulation for human infants (Piaget, Reference Piaget1962). Through the exploration of objects, developing human infants gain a greater knowledge of affordances and increased causal understanding of their properties which can be transferred to other contexts. Ultimately, this latent learning process widens the pool of knowledge which individuals can draw from when confronted with novel problems (Byrne, Reference Byrne2016; Greenberg & Mettke-Hofmann, Reference Greenberg and Mettke-Hofmann2001; Lockman, Reference Lockman2000; Oakes & Baumgartner, Reference Oakes and Baumgartner2012; Piaget, Reference Piaget1962; Power, Reference Power1999). Accordingly, in humans, exploratory tendency is highly predictive of current and future cognitive performance, suggesting that exploratory object manipulation is not just a manifestation of infants’ curiosity about their environment but also of their advancing cognitive development (Belsky & Most, Reference Belsky and Most1981; Caruso, Reference Caruso1993; Piaget, Reference Piaget1962).

Since the 1950s, it has generally been acknowledged that, just like humans, many animal species have an exploratory drive that acts independently of immediate stimulation or biological needs (Harlow et al., Reference Harlow, Harlow and Meyer1950; Montgomery, Reference Montgomery1954; Power, Reference Power1999). Just like in humans, exploration in animals seems to serve to learn about objects (Bard, Reference Bard1995; Bruner, Reference Bruner1972; Chevalier-Skolnikoff, Reference Chevalier-Skolnikoff1989; Greenberg, Reference Greenberg, Reader and Laland2003; Hayashi et al., Reference Hayashi, Takeshita and Matsuzawa2006; Lamon et al., Reference Lamon, Neumann and Zuberbühler2018; McGrew, Reference McGrew, Chevalier-Skolnikoff and Poirier1977; Tayler & Saayman, Reference Tayler, Saayman, Bruner and Jolly1976). Across species, exploratory object manipulation is readily associated with, and certainly the key feature of, all independent learning (Fenson & Schell, Reference Fenson and Schell1985; Heyes, Reference Heyes2012; Muentener et al., Reference Muentener, Herrig and Schulz2018; Piaget, Reference Piaget1962; Power, Reference Power1999; Ruff et al., Reference Ruff, Saltarelli, Capozzoli and Dubiner1992). Most forms of social learning (including many forms of observational social learning) also entail a phase in which the learner is trying out and practising the observed skills (Galef, Reference Galef2015; Heyes, Reference Heyes2012; Piaget, Reference Piaget1952; Reader & Laland, Reference Reader and Laland2002; Schuppli, Meulman, et al., Reference Schuppli, Meulman, Forss, Aprilinayati, Van Noordwijk and Van Schaik2016; Whiten, Reference Whiten2015). Therefore, in immatures across species, exploratory object manipulation is most likely part of virtually all learning about the physical world and thus arguably an important mechanism for the translation of immatures’ cognitive potential into actual skills and knowledge. These skills include the basic handling of ecologically relevant objects, such as food items or nesting substrate and more complex skills such as the ones involved in extractive foraging or tool use. The knowledge gained through exploratory object manipulation includes physical properties of the involved objects and the general physical principles of the world around them (e.g., gravity, motion and weight).

Studies across a wide range of different primate and non-primate species also suggest positive inter- and intraspecies correlations between exploratory tendency, problem-solving ability, and innovation probability (Auersperg et al., Reference Auersperg, Von Bayern, Gajdon, Huber and Kacelnik2011; Benson-Amram et al., Reference Benson-Amram, Dantzer, Stricker, Swanson and Holekamp2016; Griffin & Guez, Reference Griffin and Guez2014; Overington et al., Reference Overington, Cauchard, Côté and Lefebvre2011; Reader & Laland, Reference Reader and Laland2002; Webster & Lefebvre, Reference Webster and Lefebvre2001). The pattern of increasing exploration propensity across those species correlates with the evolution of increasing brain size and advancing cognitive capacity as well as the behavioural expressions of those, such as tool use and other complex foraging behaviours (Boesch & Boesch, Reference Boesch and Boesch1993; Byrne & Byrne, Reference Byrne and Byrne1993; Gibson, Reference Gibson1986; Heldstab et al., Reference Heldstab, Kosonen, Koski, Burkart, van Schaik and Isler2016; Leca et al., Reference Leca, Gunst and Huffman2011; Meulman et al., Reference Meulman, Sanz, Visalberghi and van Schaik2012, Reference Meulman, Seed and Mann2013; Schuppli, Graber, et al., Reference Schuppli, Graber, Isler and van Schaik2016; van Schaik et al., Reference van Schaik, Deaner and Merrill1999).

Because of the apparent significance of exploratory object manipulation for human cognitive development, a considerable amount of research has been carried out on the detailed nature and developmental changes of human infants’ exploration behaviour. Exploratory tendency, in the form of visual, manual and oral manipulation of objects, peaks around the age of 2 years and then decreases (Belsky & Most, Reference Belsky and Most1981; Bock, Reference Bock2005; Power, Reference Power1999; Power et al., Reference Power, Chapieski and McGrath1985). In general, human infants explore less with increasing exposure time to and with increasing familiarity with the object (Ruff et al., Reference Ruff, Saltarelli, Capozzoli and Dubiner1992). Whereas early exploration behaviour is characterized by single actions, with increasing age, the behaviour becomes more complex (i.e., a larger numbers of different types of actions are being combined, at first randomly and ultimately in meaningful sequences; Belsky & Most, Reference Belsky and Most1981; Fenson & Schell, Reference Fenson and Schell1985; McCall, Reference McCall1974; Power, Reference Power1999). Several studies found that the length, diversity, concentration, and efficiency of human infants’ exploratory actions increases with age (Belsky et al., Reference Belsky, Goode and Most1980; McCall, Reference McCall1974; Muentener et al., Reference Muentener, Herrig and Schulz2018; Power, Reference Power1999). However, other studies did not find such effects (Palmer, Reference Palmer1989; Power et al., Reference Power, Chapieski and McGrath1985). At the same time, whereas in early years, exploratory mouthing is the predominant form of exploration, the use of the fingers becomes increasingly prominent with increasing age (Belsky & Most, Reference Belsky and Most1981; Fenson & Schell, Reference Fenson and Schell1985; Power, Reference Power1999; Ruff et al., Reference Ruff, Saltarelli, Capozzoli and Dubiner1992). Furthermore, the exploratory drive of human infants gets significantly shaped by social inputs during development (Bakeman et al., Reference Bakeman, Adamson, Konner and Barr1990; Belsky et al., Reference Belsky, Goode and Most1980; Main, Reference Main1983; Schuetze et al., Reference Schuetze, Lewis and DiMartino1999).

Intriguingly, the developmental sequence through which human infants transition when developing their exploratory behaviour to some degree resembles the phylogenetic scale across species. In primates, an increased and more flexible exploratory tendency seems to be more prevalent than in other taxa (Power, Reference Power1999; Vauclair, Reference Vauclair1984), even though it remains unclear if cognitive or morphological differences (such as the presence or absence of hands) or a combination of both underly this pattern. However, there seems to be a clear trend within the primate lineage: from strepsirrhine Primates to Old- and New World monkeys to apes, exploratory tendency increases (Vauclair, Reference Vauclair1984), manipulations get more varied (Heldstab et al., Reference Heldstab, Kosonen, Koski, Burkart, van Schaik and Isler2016; Vauclair, Reference Vauclair1984), involve more different body parts, and more often involve fingers as opposed to the mouth (Power, Reference Power1999; Ruff et al., Reference Ruff, Saltarelli, Capozzoli and Dubiner1992; Vauclair, Reference Vauclair1984). Humans’ closest relatives, the non-human great apes (henceforward called great apes), show the highest levels of exploratory tendency, including the most flexible, varied and complex manipulations (such as object–object or object–substrate combinations, bimanual manipulations and manipulations with a larger number of body parts involved) of all non-human primates (Byrne et al., Reference Byrne, Corp and Byrne2001; Hayashi & Matsuzawa, Reference Hayashi and Matsuzawa2003; Heldstab et al., Reference Heldstab, Kosonen, Koski, Burkart, van Schaik and Isler2016; Power, Reference Power1999; Torigoe, Reference Torigoe1985), even though they are less complex than manipulations seen in humans (e.g., human infants show a richer and more differentiated repertoire of manipulations, including more object–object combinations and bimanual manipulations as well as more frequently extracting objects from their backgrounds than great ape infants; Redshaw, Reference Redshaw1978; Vauclair & Bard, Reference Vauclair and Bard1983).

A so far unsolved question is how different aspects of exploratory tendency develop in detail in wild non-human primates. The development of an exploratory tendency and its variation at the individual level may help to further elucidate the importance of developmental inputs for great ape exploration. Similarities in the overall development of exploratory behaviour between humans and great apes would also support the hypothesis that our exploratory drive evolved from a common ancestor. Studies that have looked at the exploratory tendency of captive great apes support a strong effect of the individuals’ developmental histories on the exploratory tendency (Bard & Gardner, Reference Bard, Gardner, Russon, Bard and Parker1996; Call & Tomasello, Reference Call and Tomasello1996; Hayashi & Matsuzawa, Reference Hayashi and Matsuzawa2003). Recent studies have also shown that in captive orangutans (Pongo spp.), the variability and persistence of exploration behaviour are highly correlated with problem-solving success (Damerius, Graber, et al., Reference Damerius, Graber, Willems and van Schaik2017) and are best predicted by an individual's rearing history and past experience level of human contact (Damerius, Forss, et al., Reference Damerius, Forss, Kosonen, Willems, Burkart, Call and Van Schaik2017). A handful of studies on the ontogeny of tool use behaviour in non-human primates have looked in detail at how the underlying manipulations develop in individuals. These studies found that the rates of manipulations involved in tool use increase with age and that appropriate sequential combinations of the involved manipulations and objects (i.e., as they are performed during the actual tool use behaviour) occur later than the individual manipulations (De Resende et al., Reference De Resende, Ottoni and Fragaszy2008; Inoue-Nakamura & Matsuzawa, Reference Inoue-Nakamura and Matsuzawa1997; Tan, Reference Tan2017).

Comparison of wild and captive non-human primates shows that captivity significantly increases overall exploratory tendency (Forss et al., Reference Forss, Schuppli, Haiden, Zweifel and Van Schaik2015; Kummer & Goodall, Reference Kummer and Goodall1985). This has been linked to the fact that (mainly because they don't have to find food) captive individuals have increased amounts of time and energy available for exploration compared with their wild conspecifics (Kummer & Goodall, Reference Kummer and Goodall1985). Furthermore, throughout development captive subjects may be provided with different availabilities of objects as well as different means of interacting with those objects than they would experience under natural conditions (e.g., through increased time they spend on the ground and thus with both hands available for manipulating objects).

Because exploratory behaviour is highly dependent on environmental conditions and has a strong age dependency, to understand natural exploratory behaviour and its function in everyday learning, we have to look at the development of exploration behaviour in wild great apes across age. So far, the number of studies that have looked at the development of exploration behaviour in wild great apes remains limited (but see: Bard, Reference Bard1995; Koops, Furuichi, & Hashimoto, Reference Koops, Furuichi and Hashimoto2015; Koops, Furuichi, Hashimoto, et al., Reference Koops, Furuichi, Hashimoto and van Schaik2015; Lamon et al., Reference Lamon, Neumann and Zuberbühler2018; Schuppli et al., Reference Schuppli, Forss, Meulman, Atmoko, van Noordwijk and van Schaik2017). In a previous study on wild orangutans, we found that exploratory object manipulation is heavily socially mediated: on the proximate level, being in association with conspecifics increases rates of exploration behaviour in adults and juveniles (Schuppli et al., Reference Schuppli, Forss, Meulman, Atmoko, van Noordwijk and van Schaik2017). On the developmental level, the amount of past experienced sociability predicts later exploratory tendency in immature orangutans (Schuppli et al., Reference Schuppli, van Noordwijk, Atmoko and van Schaik2020). Interestingly, adults and juveniles in more sociable populations are more exploratory even when on their own compared with individuals in less sociable populations (Schuppli et al., Reference Schuppli, Forss, Meulman, Atmoko, van Noordwijk and van Schaik2017).

The current study aimed at looking into the development of the natural exploration behaviour of wild orangutans in more detail, using a longitudinal and cross-sectional study design. Specifically, we wanted to find out to what extent the patterns that have been established for the development of an exploratory tendency in humans can also be found in immature orangutans. Orangutans are especially suitable for examining immature animals’ exploration behaviour because of their long developmental period and advanced cognitive skills (Damerius et al., Reference Damerius, Burkart, van Noordwijk, Haun, Kosonen, Galdikas and van Schaik2019; van Noordwijk et al., Reference van Noordwijk, Atmoko, Knott, Kuze, Morrogh-Bernard, Oram and Willems2018). In terms of the overall developmental trajectory of the behaviour, we predicted that (a) immature orangutans show the highest rates of exploratory object manipulations during the early dependency period, just as has been found for humans and wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Based on the human data we also hypothesized that, as in humans, with increasing age, immature orangutans will explore more persistently and more diversely. We therefore predicted that, with increasing age, (b) exploration events would become longer and that (c) exploratory object manipulations would become more diverse in terms of the number of different actions performed and the number of body parts involved, with less overall involvement of the mouth.

2. Methods

2.1 Data collection

The data for this study were collected from 2007 to 2019 in wild Sumatran orangutans (Pongo abelii) at the Suaq Balimbing research site located in South Aceh, Indonesia.

Exploration was defined as prolonged, non-repetitive, usually destructive manipulations of objects, whereby the visual and tactile foci of the individual are on the object. The set minimal duration of an exploration event to qualify as such was 5 seconds. The duration of each event was defined as lasting from the beginning of the manipulation activity until the point in time when the individual had stopped manipulating the object for at least 10 seconds. Following Pisula (Reference Pisula2008), we excluded feeding (defined as such by actual ingestion) and object play that did not have any explorative elements (i.e., manipulations where the visual and tactile foci were desynchronized, and were characterized by repetitive movements) from exploration. This approach includes all exploratory object play but not non-exploratory play manipulations which are thought to be separate constructs (Hutt, Reference Hutt1966; Pellegrini & Gustafson, Reference Pellegrini and Gustafson2005).

Focal animals were 13 immature orangutans (see Table S4), aged from 0.5 to 13.1 years. The focal animals were divided into dependent immatures (immatures that are still suckling, 0–8 years of age) and independent immatures (immatures that have stopped suckling but are not sexually active yet, 8.1–13.5 years of age).

Exploration events were collected by experienced observers during nest-to-nest focal animal follows and on an all-occurrence basis. To look at the overall exploratory tendency, we calculated exploration rates as a function of age. For this, we used data collected by CS, AVC and seven additional, highly trained observers. All observers had passed a 90% inter-observer reliability test on occurrence rates with CS for the specific detailed age classes on which their data was used (young dependent immatures, 0–5 years; old dependent immatures, 5.1 to weaning; juveniles, weaning to adulthood). To be able to assess age-specific developmental states of immature individuals and reliable rates of rare behaviours, we aimed to follow immature individuals for a minimum of 4 days within 6 months. This approach divided our data into so-called ‘data blocks’. Each data block spanned over a period of maximally five months (4–156 days, mean = 54 days) and contained at least 20 (20–131, mean = 87) follow hours. Data blocks on the same individual were separated by 223–1250 days (mean = 775 days). Exploration rates were calculated based on data collected on one individual during each data block available on this individual. The total sample for this analysis contained 23 exploration rates by 12 individuals (five males and seven females) based on a total of 3202 exploration events, whereby each individual immature contributed to one to six (mean = 1.9) age–individual data blocks and exploration rates (see Table 3 for details on the focal individuals).

To look at the nature of the exploration events in more detail, we focused on the age period with the highest exploration rates and thus densest data, namely the dependency period (0–8 years; see Table 3 for details on the focal individuals). From 2013 to 2019, exploration events collected by CS and AVC were timed and described in detail in terms of the object involved, body parts used for the manipulation and the different manipulative actions that were performed. The two observers reached an interobserver reliability of 85% on occurrence rates of the body parts and explorative actions involved in the exploration events, assessed during two simultaneous full-day follows at the end of AVC's data collection training period. Furthermore, there was no statistically significant difference in the duration of the exploration events recorded by the two observers. We had detailed information available for 2430 exploration events by seven dependent immatures (five males and two females). For the analyses with this dataset, we used daily averages. However, to avoid issues of pseudo replication and autocorrelation, we also divided this data into data blocks, following the same criteria as above and added the ID of the block as a random factor in the analyses (see below). We only included blocks which contained a minimum of 50 (50–488, mean = 203) exploration events. Each focal animal contributed to one to three (mean = 1.7) of these data blocks (one individual was excluded owing to there being too few follow hours).

Two exploration events had to be separated by at least 10 seconds during which the focal animal engaged in a different activity to be counted as two events. To assess exploratory diversity, we counted the number of actions performed with the object per exploratory event (i.e., exploratory action diversity; see Table S1 for a list of all explorative manipulations observed in this study and Table S4 for their combinations) and the number of body parts that were used for the manipulation (see Table S2 for a list of all body parts used during the explorative manipulations observed in this study). The use of the mouth during and exploration event was considered as exploratory mouthing.

2.2 Statistical analyses

We used the R programming language to analyse and visualize these data (R Development Core Team, 2019). To assess the effects of age on exploration duration, exploration variability (defined as the average number of explorative actions performed by event and number of body parts used for each event), and the share of exploration events that involved mouthing, we computed average daily values. For the exploration duration, we used linear mixed-effects models (implemented in the lme4 package in R; Bates et al., Reference Bates, Maechler, Bolker, Walker, Christensen, Singmann and Grothendieck2011). For the count data (exploration variability and share of mouthing events), we used generalized mixed models with a Poisson family distribution whereby we used the total number of exploration events as an offset. As a first step, to assess the effect of age, we compared our full models (including age as a predictor variable) with a null model (which only included the random effects), using a likelihood ratio test (LRT) via the anova function (Dobson & Barnett, Reference Dobson and Barnett2018; Fox, Reference Fox2015). Because, for some of our response variables, visual inspection of the age effect suggested a flattening after an initial increase over age, we additionally tested if including age as a sigmoid factor (using the sigmoid package in R; Quast, Reference Quast2018) would further increase the model fit, again using likelihood ratio tests. Aside from age, we also tested for sex differences by including sex as an additional factor in each model, using likelihood ratio tests. However, including sex did not significantly improve the fit of any of the models in any of the analyses and it was thus not included in the final models. To account for the fact that several individuals occurred multiple times in the dataset, we included the individual as a random effect in the analyses. To avoid autocorrelation and pseudo replication issues caused by data points that were collected close together in time, the ID of the block in which the data of each data point was collected (see above) was included as a random effect in all analyses. We report the results of the model comparisons and the details on the best fitting models. All model fits were examined visually to assess whether they satisfied model assumptions and to check for the presence of influential observations (Harrell, Reference Harrell2015), and in the case of the Poisson models, overdispersion (Mundry, Reference Mundry2014). For the plots, the mean age of each data block was computed (weighted according to the age of the focal individual at each day of data collection).

3. Results

3.1 Overall developmental trajectory of exploration behaviour

Exploration rates increased steeply during the early dependency period, peaked at the age of around 2 years and subsequently decreased to reach near-zero values by the age of weaning (7–8.5 years, Figure 1).

Figure 1. Development of exploratory tendency: average hourly exploration rates over age for immature females and males, based on the age-individual data blocks. Error bars depict the variation across different observation days and symbol–colour combinations represent different individuals.

3.2 Development of detailed aspects of dependent immatures’ exploration behaviour

Since exploratory tendency is highest during the dependency period, in the following we look at the detailed development of dependent immatures’ exploratory behaviour. In terms of exploration duration, we found that including the factor age did not significantly improve the model fit (likelihood ratio test: χ 2 = 1.07, p = 0.301, Figure 2), suggesting that the average duration of the exploration events did not change over age.

Figure 2. Development of exploration duration: daily average durations of exploration events over age for female and male dependent immatures for each data block with symbol–colour combinations representing different individuals.

When looking at the diversity of exploration behaviour as a function of age, we found that the model with age as a predictor variable was preferred over the null model LRT: χ 2 = 11.73, p = 0.003). Furthermore, the model with age as a sigmoid factor was preferred over the model with age as linear factor (LRT: χ 2 = 4.57, p < 0.001). The preferred model showed a significant positive effect of sigmoid age on exploration diversity (Table 1a, Figure 3).

Figure 3. Development of exploratory manipulation action diversity: daily average number of exploratory actions performed per exploratory event over age for female and male dependent immatures for each data block with symbol–colour combinations representing different individuals.

Table 1. Effects of age on exploration diversity, number of body parts used and exploratory mouthing. Estimates, standard errors and p-values of the preferred full models. Significant effects of predictor variables are indicated in bold. For the model with the Gaussion family distribution, the p-values of the effects were obtained via the cftest function implemented in the multcomp package in R (Hothorn et al., Reference Hothorn, Bretz, Westfall, Heiberger, Schuetzenmeister, Scheibe and Hothorn2016). R 2 refers to conditional pseudo delta R 2 values, obtained via the MuMln package (Bartoń, Reference Bartoń2009; Nakagawa et al., Reference Nakagawa, Johnson and Schielzeth2017).

For the number of body parts used to manipulate the object we found that the model with age as a predictor variable was preferred over the null model (LRT: χ 2 = 6.86, p = 0.009). Furthermore, the model with age as a sigmoid factor was preferred over the model with age as a linear factor (LRT: χ 2 = 0.89, P < 0.001). The preferred model showed a significant positive effect of sigmoid age on the number of body parts involved in the exploration event (Table 1b, Figure 4).

Figure 4. Development of exploratory body part diversity: daily average number of body parts used per exploratory event over age for female and male dependent immatures for each data block with symbol–colour combinations representing different individuals.

For the share of exploration events that included the mouth, we found that the model with age as a predictor variable was preferred over the null model (LRT: χ 2 = 5.55, p = 0.019). The full model indicated that age had a significant negative effect on the share of exploration events that included the mouth (Table 1c, Figure 5). Model selection showed that including age as a sigmoid factor did not further increase the model fit.

Figure 5. Development of exploratory mouthing: daily average shares of exploration events that involved the mouth (as a percentage of total exploration events) over age for female and male dependent immatures for each data block with symbol–colour combinations representing different individuals.

4. Discussion

Our results showed several parallels between the development of exploratory object manipulation behaviour in orangutans and that in humans, suggesting that orangutans go through similar developmental trajectories of several aspects of exploration behaviour to humans. In general, developmental changes in exploration behaviour can be the result of advancing physical or cognitive development and it will be very difficult to tease these effects apart. Furthermore, even though similarities in developmental processes between closely related species suggest common ancestry as the underlying cause, comparisons of two species alone are limited in their conclusions. However, the similarities and differences in the development of orangutan and human exploration behaviour may help us to draw conclusions about the broad mechanisms underlying the development of the different aspects of exploratory behaviour.

In line with our prediction, our results showed that the overall age trajectory of immature orangutans’ exploratory objects manipulation rates is very similar to the human pattern, with a peak during the early dependency period (Belsky & Most, Reference Belsky and Most1981; Bock, Reference Bock2005; Power, Reference Power1999; Power et al., Reference Power, Chapieski and McGrath1985). In wild chimpanzees, with increasing age, rates of object manipulation decrease while the manipulations become more goal-directed (Lamon et al., Reference Lamon, Neumann and Zuberbühler2018). If exploratory object manipulations are an expression of learning processes, one would predict that humans show a higher exploration rate than orangutans and other great apes, owing to their advanced cognitive capacity. In a study in which human, chimpanzee and bonobo (Pan pansicus) infants were observed under a highly comparable free play setting in which they were provided with access to the same objects, human infants showed more frequent and more flexible object manipulations than chimpanzees and bonobos (Vauclair & Bard, Reference Vauclair and Bard1983). Furthermore, chimpanzees were mouthing objects more frequently than bonobos and humans (Vauclair & Bard, Reference Vauclair and Bard1983). Whereas experiments like this provide valuable insights into the propensity of the species to explore objects under identical conditions, data from everyday life in the wild are needed to assess the natural expression of exploration behaviour. Comparisons of levels of naturally occurring exploration would allow us to draw conclusions about the role of exploration in everyday learning. However, to our knowledge, there are only a handful of datasets available on the development of human infants’ rates of exploration behaviour during their everyday life and across contexts (but see: Bakeman et al., Reference Bakeman, Adamson, Konner and Barr1990; Belsky & Most, Reference Belsky and Most1981; Bock, Reference Bock2005; Pellegrini & Gustafson, Reference Pellegrini and Gustafson2005). Furthermore, while investigating species differences in infant exploration, studying human infants growing up in Western industrialized societies only might be misleading. Infants growing up in a larger variety of developmental contexts, including small-scale societies, would allow for the most meaningful comparison. Even though cross-cultural data are scarce, the available studies suggest similar patterns in the development of exploratory object manipulations across human societies (Bakeman et al., Reference Bakeman, Adamson, Konner and Barr1990; Bock, Reference Bock2005; Crittenden, Reference Crittenden2016; Gosso et al., Reference Gosso, Otta, Morais, Ribeiro, Bussab, Pellegrini and Smith2005).

As predicted, in orangutans, just as in humans, with increasing age, exploration events become more variable in terms of the variety of manipulations and actions performed with the objects. This is not in line with the results of a previous study on object manipulation behaviour in orangutans (Bard, Reference Bard1987), but that might be because the later study looked at a wider range of object manipulation behaviours than our study did. Furthermore, just as in humans, in orangutans, exploratory object mouthing (i.e., object manipulations with the mouth as opposed to with the fingers) decreased with increasing age. In humans, the increasing variety of manipulations and the increase in the number of object–object combinations used during exploratory object manipulations with age are an indicator of overall advancing cognitive development (Caruso, Reference Caruso1993; Jennings et al., Reference Jennings, Harmon, Morgan, Gaiter and Yarrow1979; McCall, Reference McCall1974; McCall et al., Reference McCall, Hogarty and Hurlburt1972; Muentener et al., Reference Muentener, Herrig and Schulz2018; Power et al., Reference Power, Chapieski and McGrath1985; Schuetze et al., Reference Schuetze, Lewis and DiMartino1999). Our results suggest that exploration may also be indicative of individuals’ advancing cognitive abilities in orangutans. These findings are consistent with the phylogenetic pattern of a correlated increase of manipulation complexity (defined via the number of object–object combinations and bimanual manipulations, and the number of body parts involved) with increasing levels of cognitive performance across species (Byrne et al., Reference Byrne, Corp and Byrne2001; Hayashi & Matsuzawa, Reference Hayashi and Matsuzawa2003; Heldstab et al., Reference Heldstab, Kosonen, Koski, Burkart, van Schaik and Isler2016; Power, Reference Power1999; Redshaw, Reference Redshaw1978; Torigoe, Reference Torigoe1985; Vauclair & Bard, Reference Vauclair and Bard1983). The ontogenetic increase in object manipulation diversity over age is consistent with findings on wild chimpanzees whereas in wild bonobos there is no such age trend (Koops, Furuichi, Hashimoto, et al., Reference Koops, Furuichi, Hashimoto and van Schaik2015).

We predicted that, with increasing age, orangutans would become more persistent explorers and thus show longer exploration durations. However, our results showed that exploration durations did not increase over age. In humans, the results for the relationship between exploration duration and age are mixed: whereas some studies find a positive effect of age on exploration duration (Belsky et al., Reference Belsky, Goode and Most1980), others don't (Power et al., Reference Power, Chapieski and McGrath1985). However, across studies, human infants were found to become more efficient and competent explorers with increasing age (Belsky & Most, Reference Belsky and Most1981; McCall, Reference McCall1974; Muentener et al., Reference Muentener, Herrig and Schulz2018). An increase in efficiency and competence (e.g., owing to increasing motor skills and physical strength) may overrule any effect that an increase in exploration persistence may have on exploration duration because more efficient and competent individuals will reach their manipulation goals faster. The lack of increasing exploration duration with age may also be a result of time constraints that orangutan immatures in the wild experience. To test this hypothesis, one might look at the development of exploration persistence in zoo orangutans: zoo orangutans are expected to show fewer time constraints because of the reduced time that they spend foraging and the reduced need for vigilance (Kummer & Goodall, Reference Kummer and Goodall1985).

The orangutan's arboreal lifestyle (Ashbury et al., Reference Ashbury, Posa, Dunkel, Spillmann, Atmoko, van Schaik and Van Noordwijk2015) may be one factor that affects object manipulations and may explain differences in object manipulation found between orangutans and humans as well as other non-human great apes. The orangutans at Suaq spend less than 0.1% of their time on the ground (Schuppli, personal communication), which probably limits their access to detached objects and may make bimanual manipulations more difficult. The arboreal lifestyle may also be an explanation for the high prevalence of mouthing in infant orangutans because they spend most of their time holding onto their mothers for the first two years of life and later onto tree branches. The decrease in exploratory mouthing may reflect an increase in the ability to use their hands while being in the trees because of overall increased motor skills (Van Noordwijk et al., Reference van Noordwijk, Sauren, Abulani, Morrogh-Bernard, Utami Atmoko, Van Schaik, Atmoko, Setia and van Schaik2009).

An obvious difference between humans and other great apes is that humans integrate a significantly wider variety of objects, including a larger variety of tools, into their everyday actions. In humans, objects are also frequently involved in mother–infant interactions, often in the form of object offerings by the mother to the infant (Bakeman et al., Reference Bakeman, Adamson, Konner and Barr1990), which is not the case in orangutans or other non-human great apes (Bard & Vauclair, Reference Bard and Vauclair1984). Furthermore, there is an almost complete lack of maternal efforts to focus the infant's attention on objects to explore in wild orangutans, which is consistent with results for other non-human great apes in captivity (Bard & Vauclair, Reference Bard and Vauclair1984). Non-human great ape mothers would be physically and most likely also cognitively capable of using attention-focusing behaviours (such as repositioning objects into the immature's reach; Belsky et al., Reference Belsky, Goode and Most1980) as well as verbal attention-focusing behaviours in the form of simple calls (as all great ape species use calls in the mother–offspring context). However, within more than 10,000 hours of detailed mother–offspring observations at Suaq, spanning more than 13 years, we have only observed a handful of physical attention focusing events by mothers in the social learning context (during feeding and nest building). Similarly, we have only anecdotal reports of occasions where orangutan mothers seemed to use a ‘come-here’ call to attract their immatures to food sources. In short, whereas human mothers actively stimulate their infants’ exploratory behaviour (Belsky et al., Reference Belsky, Goode and Most1980), orangutan mothers are much more passive. These findings are supported by similar findings for other non-human great apes in captivity (Bard & Vauclair, Reference Bard and Vauclair1984).

In humans, aside from the active involvement of the mother in infants’ exploratory object manipulations, there are also more passive mechanisms at work: maternal attachment positively affects infants’ exploration behaviour (Main, Reference Main1983). Given that maternal attachment can be measured reliably in orangutans, the immediate and long-term effects of maternal attachment on exploratory tendency can be tested in a follow-up study, once sample sizes have increased.

Object manipulation is often described as an important developmental precursor of tool use (Bruner, Reference Bruner1972; Hayashi et al., Reference Hayashi, Takeshita and Matsuzawa2006; Koops, Furuichi, & Hashimoto, Reference Koops, Furuichi and Hashimoto2015; Koops, Furuichi, Hashimoto, et al., Reference Koops, Furuichi and Hashimoto2015; McGrew, Reference McGrew, Chevalier-Skolnikoff and Poirier1977). In several species, including chimpanzees, capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) and macaques (Macaca fascicularis), detailed studies on the ontogeny of tool use behaviour showed that the underlying manipulative actions and in particular their appropriate sequential combinations develop gradually with age (De Resende et al., Reference De Resende, Ottoni and Fragaszy2008; Inoue-Nakamura & Matsuzawa, Reference Inoue-Nakamura and Matsuzawa1997; Tan, Reference Tan2017). Furthermore, there are species differences in the developmental trajectories of these manipulative tool use actions which are correlated with locomotor development and feeding ecology (De Resende et al., Reference De Resende, Ottoni and Fragaszy2008). At Suaq, tool use is commonly performed by adult individuals in a large range of contexts (van Schaik et al., Reference van Schaik, Fox and Fechtman2003; van Schaik & Knott, Reference van Schaik and Knott2001). In line with this hypothesis, in a previous study, we showed that immature orangutans’ exploratory object manipulations were significantly higher at Suaq than at Tuanan, a different orangutan population without habitual tool use. This finding is further supported by differences in immatures’ object manipulations between wild chimpanzees and bonobos (Koops, Furuichi, & Hashimoto, Reference Koops, Furuichi and Hashimoto2015; Koops, Furuichi, Hashimoto, et al., Reference Koops, Furuichi and Hashimoto2015): in wild chimpanzees, who use tools in a variety of contexts, immatures show higher rates and more diverse types of object manipulation than bonobo immatures, which use few tools. Accordingly, obligate tool use in humans, in contrast to non-obligate tool use or the complete absence thereof great apes, may explain some of the differences in found in exploration frequency and complexity between human and great ape infants (Redshaw, Reference Redshaw1978; Vauclair & Bard, Reference Vauclair and Bard1983).

Also, in the context of object manipulations of immatures as preparation for tool use (or adult activity patterns in general), studies looked at sex differences in the predisposition of immatures to engage with objects. In humans, there seem to be no gender differences in infants’ exploration diversity (McCall, Reference McCall1974; Muentener et al., Reference Muentener, Herrig and Schulz2018), but object play contexts differ in concordance with gender-specific adult activity patterns (e.g., playing with dolls in girls as preparation for mothering; Pellegrini & Bjorklund, Reference Pellegrini and Bjorklund2004; Pellegrini & Gustafson, Reference Pellegrini and Gustafson2005; Pellegrini & Smith, Reference Pellegrini and Smith1998). In our study, we did not find any evidence for sex differences in any of the aspects of exploratory object manipulations analysed here. However, our data was unbalanced in terms of the age–sex distribution, which means that the negative result we find here need to be treated with caution. These findings are consistent with findings on wild immature bonobos but contrast with findings on wild immature chimpanzees, where males show higher rates of object manipulations than females (Koops, Furuichi, Hashimoto, et al., Reference Koops, Furuichi, Hashimoto and van Schaik2015; Lamon et al., Reference Lamon, Neumann and Zuberbühler2018), whereas the opposite pattern was found for specific forms of object carrying (Kahlenberg & Wrangham, Reference Kahlenberg and Wrangham2010). However, female chimpanzees develop their termite-fishing skills faster than males (Lonsdorf, Reference Lonsdorf2005), despite faster motor development in males (Lonsdorf et al., Reference Lonsdorf, Markham, Heintz, Anderson, Ciuk, Goodall and Murray2014).

In summary, the similarity of the developmental patterns between wild orangutans’ and humans’ object exploration suggests that immature orangutans, like humans, learn about the world around them through the exploration of objects and that exploratory object manipulations may reflect cognitive development during immaturity. Differences in exploratory object manipulations between humans and non-human great apes and among the different great ape species are probably based on differences in lifestyle, ecology, the species reliance on tool use, and the amount and kind of social stimulation that individuals receive during development.

Acknowledgements

We acknowledge all students, volunteers and local field assistants involved in the collection of standard behavioural data at Suaq and Tuanan. Special thanks go to Natasha Bartolotta, Belinda Kunz, Sofia Forss, Kevin Lee, Lara Nellissen, Natalie Oliver-Caldwell and Paula Willi, who contributed to the detailed data on exploration behaviour. We gratefully acknowledge the Indonesian State Ministry for Research and Technology (RISTEK), the Indonesian Institute of Science (LIPI), Departemen Dalam Negri, the Sumatran Orangutan Conservation Program (SOCP), the local government in South Aceh, the Balai Besar Taman Nasional Gunung Leuser (TNGL) in Medan and Tapak Tuan and the Director General Departemen Kehutanan (PHKA) for their permission and support to conduct this research. We also thank the Fakultas Biologi Universitas Nasional (UNAS) in Jakarta for their collaboration and support, in particular, Dr. Sri Utami Atmoko.

Supplementary material

To view supplementary material for this article, please visit https://doi.org/10.1017/ehs.2021.34

Data availability

The data associated with this research will be uploaded to a suitable data repository.

Ethical statement

As a strictly observational study on wild animals, there was no interaction with our study animals in any way. The research protocols were approved by the Ministry of research and technology (RISTEK; research permit no. 152/SIP/FRP/SM/V/2012 and following) and complied with the legal requirements of Indonesia.

Authors contributions

CS conceptualized the study, collected and entered part of the data, analysed the data and wrote the manuscript. AVC collected and entered part of the data and commented on the manuscript. TMS helped to coordinate the study and provided crucial support for the long-term orangutan project. DH supervised the project and gave substantial input to the manuscript.

Declaration of competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Funding

This work is supported by the Swiss National Science Foundation, grant number P400PM_180844, the A.H. Schultz-Foundation, the Leakey Foundation (Primate Research Fund), the Stiftung Mensch und Tier Freiburg im Breisgau, the Claraz Foundation, and the University of Zürich.

References

Ashbury, A. M., Posa, M. R. C., Dunkel, L. P., Spillmann, B., Atmoko, S. S. U., van Schaik, C. P., & Van Noordwijk, M. A. (2015). Why do orangutans leave the trees? Terrestrial behavior among wild Bornean orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus wurmbii) at Tuanan, Central Kalimantan. American Journal of Primatology, 77(11), 12161229.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Auersperg, A. M., Von Bayern, A. M., Gajdon, G. K., Huber, L., & Kacelnik, A. (2011). Flexibility in problem solving and tool use of kea and New Caledonian crows in a multi access box paradigm. PLoS One, 6(6), e20231.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bakeman, R., Adamson, L. B., Konner, M., & Barr, R. G. (1990). !Kung infancy: The social context of object exploration. Child Development, 61(3), 794809.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baldwin, D. A., Markman, E. M., & Melartin, R. L. (1993). Infants' ability to draw inferences about nonobvious object properties: Evidence from exploratory play. Child Development, 64(3), 711728.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bard, K. A. (1987). Behavioral development in young orangutans: Ontogeny of object manipulation, arboreal behavior, and food sharing (Ph.D.). Georgia State University.Google Scholar
Bard, K. A. (1995). Sensorimotor cognition in young feral orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus). Primates, 36(3), 297321.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bard, K. A., & Gardner, K. H. (1996). Influences on development in infant chimpanzees: Enculturation, temperament, and cognition. In Russon, A. E., Bard, K. A., & Parker, S. T. (Eds.), Reaching into thought: The minds of the great apes (pp. 235256). New York, NY, US: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bard, K. A., & Vauclair, J. (1984). The communicative context of object manipulation in ape and human adult–infant pairs. Journal of Human Evolution, 13(2), 181190.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bartoń, K. (2009). Mu-MIn: Multi-model inference. Version 0.12.2/r18. http://R-Forge.R-project.org/projects/mumin/Google Scholar
Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., Walker, S., Christensen, R. H. B., Singmann, H., … Grothendieck, G. (2011). Package ‘lme4’. In Linear mixed-effects models using S4 classes. R package version 1.1–27.1.Google Scholar
Baumgartner, H. A., & Oakes, L. M. (2011). Infants' developing sensitivity to object function: Attention to features and feature correlations. Journal of Cognition and Development, 12(3), 275298.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baumgartner, H. A., & Oakes, L. M. (2013). Investigating the relation between infants' manual activity with objects and their perception of dynamic events. Infancy, 18(6), 9831006.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Belsky, J., Goode, M. K., & Most, R. K. (1980). Maternal stimulation and infant exploratory competence: Cross-sectional, correlational, and experimental analyses. Child Development, 51(4), 11681178.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Belsky, J., & Most, R. K. (1981). From exploration to play: A cross-sectional study of infant free play behavior. Developmental Psychology, 17(5), 630.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benson-Amram, S., Dantzer, B., Stricker, G., Swanson, E. M., & Holekamp, K. E. (2016). Brain size predicts problem-solving ability in mammalian carnivores. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, 113(9), 25322537.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bock, J. (2005). Farming, foraging, and children's play in the Okavango Delta, Botswana. In A. D. Pellegrini & P. K. Smith (Eds.), The nature of play: Great apes and humans (pp. 254281). New York, US: The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Boesch, C., & Boesch, H. (1993). Different hand postures for pounding nuts with natural hammers by wild chimpanzees. In Holger Preuschoft & David J. Chivers (Eds.), Hands of primates (pp. 3143). Vienna, Austria: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bourgeois, K. S., Khawar, A. W., Neal, S. A., & Lockman, J. J. (2005). Infant manual exploration of objects, surfaces, and their interrelations. Infancy, 8(3), 233252.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bruner, J. S. (1972). Nature and uses of immaturity. American Psychologist, 27(8), 687708.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Byrne, R. W. (2016). Evolving insight. New York, US: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Byrne, R. W., & Byrne, J. M. (1993). Complex leaf-gathering skills of mountain gorillas (Gorilla g. beringei): Variability and standardization. American Journal of Primatology, 31(4), 241261.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Byrne, R. W., Corp, N., & Byrne, J. M. (2001). Manual dexterity in the gorilla: bimanual and digit role differentiation in a natural task. Animal Cognition, 4(3–4), 347361.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (1996). The effect of humans on the cognitive development of apes. In Anne E. Russon, Kim A. Bard & Sue Taylor Parker (Eds.), Reaching into thought: The minds of the great apes (pp. 371403). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Caruso, D. A. (1993). Dimensions of quality in infants' exploratory behavior: Relationships to problem-solving ability. Infant Behavior and Development, 16(4), 441454.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chevalier-Skolnikoff, S. (1989). Tool use in Cebus: Its relation to object manipulation, the brain, and ecological adaptations. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 12(3), 610627.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crittenden, A. N. (2016). Children's foraging and play among the Hadza. Courtney L. Meehan & Alyssa N. Crittenden (Eds.), Childhood: Origins, Evolution, and Implications (pp. 155172). Santa Fe, US: School for Advanced Research Press.Google Scholar
Damerius, L. A., Burkart, J. M., van Noordwijk, M. A., Haun, D. B., Kosonen, Z. K., Galdikas, B. M., … van Schaik, C. P. (2019). General cognitive abilities in orangutans (Pongo abelii and Pongo pygmaeus). Intelligence, 74, 311.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Damerius, L. A., Forss, S. I., Kosonen, Z. K., Willems, E. P., Burkart, J. M., Call, J., … Van Schaik, C. P. (2017). Orientation toward humans predicts cognitive performance in orang-utans. Scientific Reports, 7, 40052.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Damerius, L. A., Graber, S. M., Willems, E. P., & van Schaik, C. P. (2017). Curiosity boosts orang-utan problem-solving ability. Animal Behaviour, 134, 5770.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Resende, B. D., Ottoni, E. B., & Fragaszy, D. M. (2008). Ontogeny of manipulative behavior and nut-cracking in young tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella): A perception–action perspective. Development Science, 11(6), 828840.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dobson, A. J., & Barnett, A. G. (2018). An introduction to generalized linear models. CRC Press.Google Scholar
Fenson, L., & Schell, R. E. (1985). The origins of exploratory play. Early Child Development and Care, 19(1–2), 324.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Forss, S. I., Schuppli, C., Haiden, D., Zweifel, N., & Van Schaik, C. P. (2015). Contrasting responses to novelty by wild and captive orangutans. American Journal of Primatology, 77(10), 11091121.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fox, J. (2015). Applied regression analysis and generalized linear models. Sage.Google Scholar
Galef, B. G. (2015). Laboratory studies of imitation/field studies of tradition: Towards a synthesis in animal social learning. Behavioural Processes, 112, 114119.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gibson, K. (1986). Cognition, brain size and the extraction of embedded food resources. Primate Ontogeny, Cognition and Social Behaviour, 3, 93104.Google Scholar
Gosso, Y., Otta, E., Morais, M. d. L. S., Ribeiro, F. J. L., & Bussab, V. S. R. (2005). Play in hunter-gatherer society. In Pellegrini, A. D. & Smith, P. K. (Eds.), The nature of play: Great apes and humans (pp. 213253). New York, US: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Greenberg, R. S. (2003). The role of neophobia and neophilia in the development of innovative behaviour of birds. In Reader, S., & Laland, K. N. (Eds.), Animal innovation (pp. 175196). Oxford, England: Oxford University.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greenberg, R., & Mettke-Hofmann, C. (2001). Ecological aspects of neophobia and neophilia in birds. In Current ornithology (pp. 119178). Springer.Google Scholar
Griffin, A. S., & Guez, D. (2014). Innovation and problem solving: a review of common mechanisms. Behavioural Processes, 109, 121134.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harlow, H. F., Harlow, M. K., & Meyer, D. R. (1950). Learning motivated by a manipulation drive. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 40(2), 228.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harrell, F. E. Jr (2015). Regression modeling strategies: With applications to linear models, logistic and ordinal regression, and survival analysis. Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hayashi, M., & Matsuzawa, T. (2003). Cognitive development in object manipulation by infant chimpanzees. Animal Cognition, 6(4), 225233.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hayashi, M., Takeshita, H., & Matsuzawa, T. (2006). Cognitive development in apes and humans assessed by object manipulation. In Cognitive development in chimpanzees (pp. 395410). Springer.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heldstab, S. A., Kosonen, Z., Koski, S., Burkart, J., van Schaik, C., & Isler, K. (2016). Manipulation complexity in primates coevolved with brain size and terrestriality. Scientific Reports, 6, 24528.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heyes, C. (2012). What's social about social learning? Journal of Comparative Psychology, 126(2), 193.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hothorn, T., Bretz, F., Westfall, P., Heiberger, R. M., Schuetzenmeister, A., Scheibe, S., & Hothorn, M. T. (2016). Package ‘multcomp’. In Simultaneous inference in general parametric models. Project for Statistical Computing.Google Scholar
Hutt, C. (1966). Exploration and play in children. Symposia of the Zoological Society of LondonGoogle Scholar
Inoue-Nakamura, N., & Matsuzawa, T. (1997). Development of stone tool use by wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 111(2), 159.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jennings, K. D., Harmon, R. J., Morgan, G. A., Gaiter, J. L., & Yarrow, L. J. (1979). Exploratory play as an index of mastery motivation: Relationships to persistence, cognitive functioning, and environmental measures. Developmental Psychology, 15(4), 386.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kahlenberg, S. M., & Wrangham, R. W. (2010). Sex differences in chimpanzees' use of sticks as play objects resemble those of children. Current Biology, 20(24), R1067R1068.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koops, K., Furuichi, T., & Hashimoto, C. (2015). Chimpanzees and bonobos differ in intrinsic motivation for tool use. Scientific Reports, 5, 11356.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koops, K., Furuichi, T., Hashimoto, C., & van Schaik, C. P. (2015). Sex differences in object manipulation in wild immature chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) and bonobos (Pan paniscus): Preparation for tool use? PLoS One, 10(10), e0139909.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kummer, H., & Goodall, J. (1985). Conditions of innovative behaviour in primates. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. B, Biological Sciences, 308(1135), 203214.Google Scholar
Lamon, N., Neumann, C., & Zuberbühler, K. (2018). Development of object manipulation in wild chimpanzees. Animal Behaviour, 135, 121130.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leca, J.-B., Gunst, N., & Huffman, M. (2011). Complexity in object manipulation by Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata): A cross-sectional analysis of manual coordination in stone handling patterns. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 125(1), 61.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lockman, J. J. (2000). A perception–action perspective on tool use development. Child Development, 71(1), 137144.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lonsdorf, E. V. (2005). Sex differences in the development of termite-fishing skills in the wild chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii, of Gombe National Park, Tanzania. Animal Behaviour, 70(3), 673683.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lonsdorf, E. V., Markham, A. C., Heintz, M. R., Anderson, K. E., Ciuk, D. J., Goodall, J., & Murray, C. M. (2014). Sex differences in wild chimpanzee behavior emerge during infancy. PLoS One, 9(6), e99099.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Main, M. (1983). Exploration, play, and cognitive functioning related to infant–mother attachment. Infant Behavior and Development, 6(2–3), 167174.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCall, R. B. (1974). Exploratory manipulation and play in the human infant. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCall, R. B., Hogarty, P. S., & Hurlburt, N. (1972). Transitions in infant sensorimotor development and the prediction of childhood IQ. American Psychologist, 27(8), 728.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGrew, W. C. (1977). Socialization and object manipulation of wild chimpanzees. In Chevalier-Skolnikoff, S. & Poirier, F. E. (Eds.), Primate bio-social development (pp. 261288). New York, US: Garland Publishing.Google Scholar
Meulman, E. J., Sanz, C. M., Visalberghi, E., & van Schaik, C. P. (2012). The role of terrestriality in promoting primate technology. Evolutionary Anthropology, 21(2), 5868.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meulman, E., Seed, A., & Mann, J. (2013). If at first you don't succeed … Studies of ontogeny shed light on the cognitive demands of habitual tool use. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 368(1630), 20130050.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Montgomery, K. C. (1954). The role of the exploratory drive in learning. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 47(1), 60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Muentener, P., Herrig, E., & Schulz, L. (2018). The efficiency of infants' exploratory play is related to longer-term cognitive development. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nakagawa, S., Johnson, P. C., & Schielzeth, H. (2017). The coefficient of determination R 2 and intra-class correlation coefficient from generalized linear mixed-effects models revisited and expanded. Journal of the Royal Society Interface, 14(134), 20170213.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oakes, L. M., & Baumgartner, H. A. (2012). Manual object exploration and learning about object features in human infants. Paper presented at the 2012 IEEE International Conference on Development and Learning and Epigenetic Robotics (ICDL), 7–9 November, San Diego, CA, USA.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Overington, S. E., Cauchard, L., Côté, K.-A., & Lefebvre, L. (2011). Innovative foraging behaviour in birds: what characterizes an innovator? Behavioural Processes, 87(3), 274285.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Palmer, C. F. (1989). The discriminating nature of infants' exploratory actions. Developmental Psychology, 25(6), 885.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pellegrini, A. D., & Bjorklund, D. F. (2004). The ontogeny and phylogeny of children's object and fantasy play. Human Nature, 15(1), 2343.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pellegrini, A. D., & Gustafson, K. (2005). Boys’ and girls’ uses of objects for exploration, play, and tools in early childhood. In A. D. Pellegrini & P. K. Smith (Eds.), The nature of play: Great apes and humans (pp. 113135). New York, US: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Pellegrini, A. D., & Smith, P. K. (1998). Physical activity play: The nature and function of a neglected aspect of play. Child Development, 69(3), 577598.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piaget, J. (1952). Play, dreams and imitation in childhood. New York, NY, US: W. W. Norton & Company.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1962). The stages of the intellectual development of the child. Bulletin of the Menninger Clinic, 26(3), 120.Google ScholarPubMed
Pisula, W. (2008). Play and exploration in animals – A comparative analysis. Polish Psychological Bulletin, 39(2), 104107.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Power, T. G. (1999). Play and exploration in children and animals. Psychology Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Power, T. G., Chapieski, M. L., & McGrath, M. P. (1985). Assessment of individual differences in infant exploration and play. Developmental Psychology, 21(6), 974.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
R Development Core Team. (2019). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing.Google Scholar
Reader, S. M., & Laland, K. N. (2002). Social intelligence, innovation, and enhanced brain size in primates. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 99(7), 44364441.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Redshaw, M. (1978). Cognitive development in human and gorilla infants. Journal of Human Evolution, 7(2), 133141.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ruff, H. A., Saltarelli, L. M., Capozzoli, M., & Dubiner, K. (1992). The differentiation of activity in infants' exploration of objects. Developmental Psychology, 28(5), 851.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schuetze, P., Lewis, A., & DiMartino, D. (1999). Relation between time spent in daycare and exploratory behaviors in 9-month-old infants. Infant Behavior and Development, 22(2), 267276.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schuppli, C., Forss, S., Meulman, E., Atmoko, S. U., van Noordwijk, M., & van Schaik, C. (2017). The effects of sociability on exploratory tendency and innovation repertoires in wild Sumatran and Bornean orangutans. Scientific Reports, 7(1), 15464.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schuppli, C., Graber, S. M., Isler, K., & van Schaik, C. P. (2016). Life history, cognition and the evolution of complex foraging niches. Journal of Human Evolution, 92, 91100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schuppli, C., Meulman, E. J., Forss, S. I., Aprilinayati, F., Van Noordwijk, M. A., & Van Schaik, C. P. (2016). Observational social learning and socially induced practice of routine skills in immature wild orang-utans. Animal Behaviour, 119, 8798.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schuppli, C., van Noordwijk, M., Atmoko, S. U., & van Schaik, C. (2020). Early sociability fosters later exploratory tendency in wild immature orangutans. Science Advances, 6(2), eaaw2685.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tan, A. W. (2017). From play to proficiency: The ontogeny of stone-tool use in coastal-foraging long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis) from a comparative perception-action perspective. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 131(2), 89.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tayler, C. K., & Saayman, G. S. (1976). Play and imitation in dolphins. In Bruner, J. S. & Jolly, A. (Eds.), Play its role in development and evolution (pp. 387394). International Psychotherapy Institute. e-Book.Google Scholar
Torigoe, T. (1985). Comparison of object manipulation among 74 species of non-human primates. Primates, 26(2), 182194.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Noordwijk, M. A., Atmoko, S. S. U., Knott, C. D., Kuze, N., Morrogh-Bernard, H. C., Oram, F., … Willems, E. P. (2018). The slow ape: High infant survival and long interbirth intervals in wild orangutans. Journal of Human Evolution, 125, 3849.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Noordwijk, M., Sauren, S., Abulani, A., Morrogh-Bernard, H., Utami Atmoko, S., Van Schaik, C. (2009). Development of indepencence: Sumatran and Bornean orangutans compared. In Atmoko, S. S. U., Setia, T. M., & van Schaik, C. P. (Eds.), Orangutans: Geographic variation in behavioral ecology and conservation (pp. 189203). New York, US: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
van Schaik, C. P., Deaner, R. O., & Merrill, M. Y. (1999). The conditions for tool use in primates: implications for the evolution of material culture. Journal of Human Evolution, 36(6), 719741.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Schaik, C. P., Fox, E. A., & Fechtman, L. T. (2003). Individual variation in the rate of use of tree-hole tools among wild orang-utans: implications for hominin evolution. Journal of Human Evolution, 44(1), 1123.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Schaik, C. P., & Knott, C. D. (2001). Geographic variation in tool use on Neesia fruits in orangutans. American Journal of Physical Anthropology: The Official Publication of the American Association of Physical Anthropologists, 114(4), 331342.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vauclair, J. (1984). Phylogenetic approach to object manipulation in human and ape infants. Human Development, 27(5–6), 321328.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vauclair, J., & Bard, K. A. (1983). Development of manipulations with objects in ape and human infants. Journal of Human Evolution, 12(7), 631645.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Webster, S. J., & Lefebvre, L. (2001). Problem solving and neophobia in a columbiform–passeriform assemblage in Barbados. Animal Behaviour, 62(1), 2332.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whiten, A. (2015). Experimental studies illuminate the cultural transmission of percussive technologies in Homo and Pan. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 370(1682), 20140359.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. Development of exploratory tendency: average hourly exploration rates over age for immature females and males, based on the age-individual data blocks. Error bars depict the variation across different observation days and symbol–colour combinations represent different individuals.

Figure 1

Figure 2. Development of exploration duration: daily average durations of exploration events over age for female and male dependent immatures for each data block with symbol–colour combinations representing different individuals.

Figure 2

Figure 3. Development of exploratory manipulation action diversity: daily average number of exploratory actions performed per exploratory event over age for female and male dependent immatures for each data block with symbol–colour combinations representing different individuals.

Figure 3

Table 1. Effects of age on exploration diversity, number of body parts used and exploratory mouthing. Estimates, standard errors and p-values of the preferred full models. Significant effects of predictor variables are indicated in bold. For the model with the Gaussion family distribution, the p-values of the effects were obtained via the cftest function implemented in the multcomp package in R (Hothorn et al., 2016). R2 refers to conditional pseudo delta R2 values, obtained via the MuMln package (Bartoń, 2009; Nakagawa et al., 2017).

Figure 4

Figure 4. Development of exploratory body part diversity: daily average number of body parts used per exploratory event over age for female and male dependent immatures for each data block with symbol–colour combinations representing different individuals.

Figure 5

Figure 5. Development of exploratory mouthing: daily average shares of exploration events that involved the mouth (as a percentage of total exploration events) over age for female and male dependent immatures for each data block with symbol–colour combinations representing different individuals.

Supplementary material: File

Schuppli et al. supplementary material

Tables S1-S4

Download Schuppli et al. supplementary material(File)
File 70.9 KB