Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gxg78 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-25T19:18:10.558Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Where innovations flourish: an ethnographic and archaeological overview of hunter–gatherer learning contexts

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 June 2020

Sheina Lew-Levy*
Affiliation:
Simon Fraser University, Department of Psychology, Burnaby, BC, Canada Department of Archaeology and Heritage Studies, Aarhus University, Aarhus, Denmark
Annemieke Milks
Affiliation:
Institute of Archaeology, University College London, London, UK
Noa Lavi
Affiliation:
Department of Anthropology, University College London, London, UK
Sarah M. Pope
Affiliation:
Department of Comparative and Cultural Psychology, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany
David E. Friesem
Affiliation:
McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK Zinman Institute of Archaeology, University of Haifa, Haifa, Israel
*
*Corresponding author. E-mail: sheinalewlevy@gmail.com

Abstract

Research in developmental psychology suggests that children are poor tool innovators. However, such research often overlooks the ways in which children's social and physical environments may lead to cross-cultural variation in their opportunities and proclivity to innovate. In this paper, we examine contemporary hunter–gatherer child and adolescent contributions to tool innovation. We posit that the cultural and subsistence context of many hunter–gatherer societies fosters behavioural flexibility, including innovative capabilities. Using the ethnographic and developmental literature, we suggest that socialisation practices emphasised in hunter–gatherer societies, including learning through autonomous exploration, adult and peer teaching, play and innovation seeking may bolster children's ability to innovate. We also discuss whether similar socialisation practices can be interpreted from the archaeological record. We end by pointing to areas of future study for understanding the role of children and adolescents in the development of tool innovations across cultures in the past and present.

Type
Review
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2020. Published by Cambridge University Press

Media summary: Socialisation practices emphasised in hunter–gatherer societies may bolster child and adolescent innovativeness.

Both social learning and innovation are central to cumulative cultural evolution (Legare & Nielsen, Reference Legare and Nielsen2015). However, while children in diverse societies demonstrate a suite of cognitive traits which make them especially sensitive to identifying and learning cultural information (Henrich & McElreath, Reference Henrich and McElreath2003; Kline, Reference Kline2015), psychologists working primarily in WEIRD societies (Western, Educated, Industrialised, Rich, and Democratic – Henrich et al., Reference Henrich, Heine and Norenzayan2010) have noted that making novel tools is difficult for children under the age of 10 (Beck et al., Reference Beck, Apperly, Chappell, Guthrie and Cutting2011; Cutting et al., Reference Cutting, Apperly and Beck2011; Nielsen et al., Reference Nielsen, Tomaselli, Mushin and Whiten2014; Whalley et al., Reference Whalley, Cutting and Beck2017). Here, we argue that cross-cultural diversity in subsistence and socialisation practices complicates the image of children as infrequent tool innovators. Drawing upon the ethnographic literature, we argue that the developmental niche documented among several contemporary hunter–gatherer societies may set the stage for children's innovative capabilities to flourish. Elsewhere, we have argued that hunter–gatherer children have probably contributed to changes in cultural values (Reckin et al., Reference Reckin, Lew-levy, Lavi, Ellis-Davies and Moreauin press). Here, we focus our discussion on children's potential and actual contributions to technological and/or material innovations in the past and present.

In what follows, we first describe the results of psychological studies on innovation, and outline how gaps in prior knowledge, including previous familiarity with the testing materials, and asocial experimental settings, may limit researchers’ ability to measure variation in children's tool innovation across cultural contexts. Second, we outline how the cultural and subsistence contexts of hunter–gatherers foster behavioural flexibility, including in childhood and adolescence. Third, we summarise findings from two meta-ethnographic reviews on learning in hunter–gatherer societies (Lew-Levy et al., Reference Lew-Levy, Reckin, Lavi, Cristóbal-Azkarate and Ellis-Davies2017, Reference Lew-Levy, Lavi, Reckin, Cristóbal-Azkarate and Ellis-Davies2018), focusing on specific socialisation practices which may encourage hunter–gatherer child and adolescent innovative capabilities. While many of these practices appear to be widespread, it is important to note that hunter–gatherers are diverse, and thus, not all descriptions will generalise to all hunter–gatherer societies. Wherever possible, we also outline clear ethnographic examples of children's innovations. Fourth, using examples from the Palaeolithic, Paleoindian, and Late Stone age, we illustrate how the ethnographic record can provide an interpretative framework for enriching how archaeologists think about, and identify, children as innovators. We also outline some of the challenges to identifying social learning processes in the archaeological record. Finally, we outline areas of future research for psychology, anthropology, and archaeology.

Defining ‘innovation’ across disciplines

Innovation is central to the behavioural flexibility of human and non-human animals alike (Reader & Laland, Reference Reader and Laland2001). Through innovations, species can rapidly respond to environmental novelty (Laland, Reference Laland1992). In humans, innovations in social conventions, such as rituals, and instrumental skills, such as hunting technology, have helped our species inhabit diverse and challenging environments, and coordinate within and across social groups (Boyd & Richerson, Reference Boyd and Richerson1985; Fogarty et al., Reference Fogarty, Creanza and Feldman2015; Legare & Nielsen, Reference Legare and Nielsen2015; McElreath et al., Reference McElreath, Boyd and Richerson2003). Because of its importance to human evolution and culture, the study of innovation spans several academic disciplines, and each discipline defines the term differently (Fogarty et al., Reference Fogarty, Creanza and Feldman2015; Walsh et al., Reference Walsh, Riede, O'Neill and Prentiss2019). In the present paper, we draw upon definitions from psychology and archaeology.

In psychology, innovation, and specifically, tool innovation, has been defined as the construction of ‘new tools, or using old tools in new ways, to solve new problems’ (Legare & Nielsen, Reference Legare and Nielsen2015, p. 689). Although innovative behaviours, including tool use and modification, have been described in other species (see Griffin & Guez, Reference Griffin and Guez2014 for review; Lefebvre et al., Reference Lefebvre, Reader and Sol2004; Overington et al., Reference Overington, Morand-Ferron, Boogert and Lefebvre2009; Reader et al., Reference Reader, Hager and Laland2011), psychologists are often concerned with understanding the unique cognitive and developmental processes which underpin human innovation, including advanced analogical (Chan et al., Reference Chan, Fu, Schunn, Cagan, Wood and Kotovsky2011; Markman et al., Reference Markman, Wood, Linsey, Murphy and Laux2011) or counterfactual reasoning (Tijus et al., Reference Tijus, Brézillon, Poitrenaud and Léger2009) to devise solutions, and inhibition (Gönül et al., Reference Gönül, Takmaz, Hohenberger and Corballis2018) or cognitive flexibility (Gönül et al., Reference Gönül, Hohenberger, Corballis and Henderson2019; Pope et al., Reference Pope, Fagot, Meguerditchian, Watzek, Lew-Levy, Autrey and Hopkins2020) to apply them.

Archaeologists often separate innovation from invention, the latter defined as ‘a wholly new phenomenon’, and the former, as ‘a novel modification of something already in existence that proves adaptive and diffuses through a population by processes of selection’ (Walsh et al., Reference Walsh, Riede, O'Neill and Prentiss2019, p. 54; see also Richerson & Boyd, Reference Richerson and Boyd2005; Mesoudi & O'Brien, Reference Mesoudi and O'Brien2008; Rogers, Reference Rogers1983; Shennan, Reference Shennan2001). Archaeologists are concerned with understanding the contexts under which a phenomenon has undergone observable adaptive change over time (Walsh et al., Reference Walsh, Riede, O'Neill and Prentiss2019). Usually, archaeologists are restricted to the study of innovations that have ‘spread in a population to a detectable frequency’ (Fogarty et al., Reference Fogarty, Creanza and Feldman2015, p. 737; see also Walsh et al., Reference Walsh, Riede, O'Neill and Prentiss2019). Archaeological research typically emphasises the role of technological and cultural innovation in relation to the evolution of behavioural and cognitive complexity, the ability to thrive in new ecological niches, and/or the transmission and diffusion of cultural traits (e.g. Hussain & Will, Reference Hussain and Willin press; Knecht, Reference Knecht1991; McBrearty & Brooks, Reference McBrearty and Brooks2000; Shea & Sisk, Reference Shea and Sisk2010).

Building on these definitions, the present paper views innovations as creative outputs that are new and useful in specific settings (Fogarty et al., Reference Fogarty, Creanza and Feldman2015), and which are transmitted throughout a group. We consider innovations as arising through multiple individual- and group-level processes, including invention, modification, recombination, trial-and-error and copying error (Ramsey et al., Reference Ramsey, Bastian and van Schaik2007). We discuss both the processes (i.e. socialisation practices) and products (e.g. new subsistence technologies) of child and adolescent innovators.

Experimental research on children as innovators

Evidence of innovative capacities can be found early in human development. During this time, innovations serve an important role in supplementing knowledge or ability in both social and physical domains. For example, when learning to walk, infants develop unique strategies for using supports (Adolph & Robinson, Reference Adolph and Robinson2013) and 2–3-year-olds fill gaps in their lexicon by combining or modifying known words, such as saying ‘I can button it’ rather than ‘I can turn it on’ (Clark, Reference Clark1982). Throughout childhood, innovations are seen in children's pretence and game play (Carr et al., Reference Carr, Kendal and Flynn2016; Nielsen et al., Reference Nielsen, Cucchiaro and Mohamedally2012) and during this time cognitive skills which probably support innovation improve drastically, including analogical reasoning (Richland et al., Reference Richland, Morrison and Holyoak2006, Reference Richland, Chan, Morrison and Au2010), counterfactual reasoning (Rafetseder et al., Reference Rafetseder, Schwitalla and Perner2013), inhibition (Montgomery & Koeltzow, Reference Montgomery and Koeltzow2010) and cognitive flexibility (Deák & Wiseheart, Reference Deák and Wiseheart2015; Doebel & Zelazo, Reference Doebel and Zelazo2015). Further, 5-year-old children outperform older children and adults in contexts which require them to consider causal relationships or object uses in innovative ways (Defeyter & German, Reference Defeyter and German2003; Gopnik et al., Reference Gopnik, Griffiths and Lucas2015; Lucas et al., Reference Lucas, Bridgers, Griffiths and Gopnik2015).

Although creative problem-solving metrics like divergent thinking (Gönül et al., Reference Gönül, Hohenberger, Corballis and Henderson2019), floating object (Cheke et al., Reference Cheke, Loissel and Clayton2012; Hanus et al., Reference Hanus, Mendes, Tennie and Call2011; Nielsen, Reference Nielsen2013) and functional fixedness (Defeyter & German, Reference Defeyter and German2003) tasks have been conducted alongside or as proxies for innovation metrics (Carr et al., Reference Carr, Kendal and Flynn2016; but see Beck et al., Reference Beck, Williams, Cutting, Apperly and Chappell2016), here we focus on direct measures of tool innovation. For children, the most prevalent tool innovation metric is the hook task, a paradigm originally devised for corvids (Bird & Emery, Reference Bird and Emery2009; Weir et al., Reference Weir, Chappell and Kacelnik2002) and later modified for humans (Beck et al., Reference Beck, Apperly, Chappell, Guthrie and Cutting2011; Cutting et al., Reference Cutting, Apperly and Beck2011). In this paradigm, participants must bend a malleable stick, often a pipe cleaner, into a hook in order to retrieve a basket containing a sticker or other reward from the bottom of a clear tube. Although children aged 3–4 are able to reproduce this action after watching a demonstration (Cutting et al., Reference Cutting, Apperly and Beck2011; Gönül et al., Reference Gönül, Takmaz, Hohenberger and Corballis2018), most children below the ages of 7–8 are unable to successfully innovate the hook. Even 10–11-year-olds do not exhibit mature levels of hook use (Beck et al., Reference Beck, Apperly, Chappell, Guthrie and Cutting2011; Cutting et al., Reference Cutting, Apperly and Beck2011, Reference Cutting, Apperly, Chappell and Beck2014; Gönül et al., Reference Gönül, Takmaz, Hohenberger and Corballis2018; Whalley et al., Reference Whalley, Cutting and Beck2017; but see Sheridan et al., Reference Sheridan, Konopasky, Kirkwood and Defeyter2016). Children similarly struggle to innovate in tasks requiring them to create a functional stick tool, meant to push a reward out of the middle of a horizontal clear tube, by unbending, combining or taking apart non-functional precursors (Cutting et al., Reference Cutting, Apperly and Beck2011; Neldner et al., Reference Neldner, Redshaw, Murphy, Tomaselli, Davis, Dixson and Nielsen2019). These findings have been replicated in several small-scale societies; recently settled San hunter–gatherer, Indigenous Australian and NiVanuatu agriculturalist children exhibit low rates of tool innovation in experimental paradigms (Neldner et al., Reference Neldner, Mushin and Nielsen2017, Reference Neldner, Redshaw, Murphy, Tomaselli, Davis, Dixson and Nielsen2019; Nielsen et al., Reference Nielsen, Tomaselli, Mushin and Whiten2014).

There are several methodological reasons why tool innovation tasks, like the hook task, might occlude the true breadth of children's innovative potential. Empirical measures of innovation are ill-structured, and rely upon a great deal of prior information in order to reach the prescribed solution (Chappell et al., Reference Chappell, Cutting, Apperly and Beck2013; Cutting et al., Reference Cutting, Apperly, Chappell and Beck2014). For example, to appropriately solve the hook task, children must understand the physical affordances of the pipe cleaner and basket handle, remember and flexibly apply prior knowledge of hook use to a novel context, and have the cognitive and motoric dexterity to perform all of the required actions within a 1–3 minute testing period. Under these constraints, failure does not necessarily indicate a lack of innovative ability (Reader et al., Reference Reader, Morand-Ferron and Flynn2016), but may be attributable to any number of gaps in prior information or simply an inability to access it fast enough. These issues are compounded in experiments conducted in small-scale societies, where children are less likely than WEIRD children to have manipulated the precursor materials prior to the experiment, making object affordances especially opaque. For example, in a remote village in the Republic of the Congo, a Bondongo fisher–farmer child responded to the hook task by requesting he be allowed to retrieve his fishing hook, suggesting that a lack of familiarity with the affordances of the pipe cleaner may have prevented his success (Pope, unpublished data).

Further, in order to avoid confounding innovation with social learning, most experimental studies are designed so that children are tested individually to prevent the use of socially acquired information. However, applying socially learned information to new settings is probably central to tool innovation in everyday contexts (Muthukrishna & Henrich, Reference Muthukrishna and Henrich2016). For example, after seeing a pre-formed hook, dyads were more successful than individuals at solving the hook task (Gönül et al., Reference Gönül, Hohenberger, Corballis and Henderson2019). In another extractive foraging task, complex tool innovation was observed in groups of 3–4-year-olds, but not individuals (McGuigan et al., Reference McGuigan, Burdett, Burgess, Dean, Lucas, Vale and Whiten2017). In cultural evolution studies, in which tasks are solved over multiple generations, larger group size has been associated with greater improvement to existing technologies (Derex et al., Reference Derex, Beugin, Godelle and Raymond2013), increased complexity (Muthukrishna et al., Reference Muthukrishna, Shulman, Vasilescu and Henrich2014) and higher solution rates (Kempe & Mesoudi, Reference Kempe and Mesoudi2014). Several modelling and ethnographic studies also demonstrate that group size and inter-group contact are positively correlated with material culture diversity (Caldwell et al., Reference Caldwell, Cornish and Kandler2016; Collard et al., Reference Collard, Ruttle, Buchanan and O'Brien2013; Henrich, Reference Henrich2004; Kline & Boyd, Reference Kline and Boyd2010; Shennan, Reference Shennan2001). These results suggest that innovation, even in the strictest sense, does not occur in an information vacuum. Instead, social and physical environments may influence innovative proclivity (Ivcevic, Reference Ivcevic2009).

Hunter–gatherer social and subsistence contexts

Hunting and gathering societies are commonly defined by a subsistence economy that primarily relies on non-domesticated resources obtained via hunting, fishing and foraging (Kelly, Reference Kelly1995; Lee & Daly, Reference Lee and Daly1999). Until approximately 12,000 years ago and before the emergence of agriculture, all human societies hunted and gathered for subsistence. Today, hunter–gatherers inhabit diverse environments, and have been shaped, among other things, by a long history of relationships with agropastoralists, colonisation and nation-states (Guenther, Reference Guenther2007). Reliance on non-foraged food is increasing, owing to growing engagements with neighbouring societies, new economic opportunities, state intervention and restrictions on the use of wild resources (Reyes-García & Pyhälä, Reference Reyes-García and Pyhälä2016). While we acknowledge that defining hunter–gatherers by their subsistence economy reflects eighteenth-century European classifications rather than local ones (e.g. Barnard, Reference Barnard2004), in the absence of better terminology, we make use of the term ‘hunter–gatherer’ throughout the text.

Anthropologists have pointed to distinctive cultural and social traits that are shared by many hunter–gatherer societies varying in geography, ecology and histories, and which are usually not shared with their immediate agrarian or pastoralist neighbours (Endicott, Reference Endicott, Gibson and Sillander2011; Finlayson & Warren, Reference Finlayson and Warren2010; Hewlett et al., Reference Hewlett, Fouts, Boyette and Hewlett2011; Lee & Daly, Reference Lee and Daly1999; Schweitzer et al., Reference Schweitzer, Biesele and Hitchcock2000). These cultural values include high levels of egalitarianism, which allow equal access to resources to all group members (Woodburn, Reference Woodburn1982); a tendency towards mobility in which people and/or entire dwelling sites move frequently (Kelly, Reference Kelly1983; MacDonald & Hewlett, Reference MacDonald and Hewlett1999); sharing which maintains the redistribution of material resources and social relationships (Lavi & Friesem, Reference Lavi and Friesem2019); respect for personal autonomy, including the freedom and independence of individuals (Gardner, Reference Gardner1991; Woodburn, Reference Woodburn1982); and lastly, small residential groups (Bird-David, Reference Bird-David2017) with large lifelong and intergenerational networks (Bird et al., Reference Bird, Bird, Codding and Zeanah2019; Dyble et al., Reference Dyble, Salali, Chaudhary, Page, Smith, Thompson and Migliano2015). Many hunter–gatherers also exhibit shared features of infancy, childhood and adolescence. These include close physical contact with mother, indulgence towards infants, frequent nursing, co-sleeping, weaning around three years of age, four-year birth spacing, separation and stranger rejection, dense social contexts, primary care by the mother, more father care than in other societies, transition into a multi-aged, mixed-gender playgroup in middle childhood, little child responsibility for subsistence and childcare, and few restrictions on childhood and adolescent sexuality (Konner, Reference Konner, Hewlett and Lamb2005, Reference Konner, Meehan and Crittenden2016). Because most research on child development is conducted in WEIRD societies (Nielsen et al., Reference Nielsen, Haun, Kärtner and Legare2017), studying hunter–gatherer childhoods can provide an alternative and diverse perspective on the role of children as innovators, and the social contexts that encourage innovative propensities to flourish. In this paper, we consider aspects of the social and cultural environment, as documented among many contemporary hunter–gatherers, as especially conducive to the development of innovative children and adolescents.

Specifically, hunter–gatherers rely on non-domesticated resources that shift in availability and abundance seasonally, yearly and across generations (Kelly, Reference Kelly1983). And yet many hunter–gatherers routinely view their environment as abundant and giving (e.g. Bird-David, Reference Bird-David1990). This may be due to social and subsistence strategies that mitigate risk, including widespread sharing of resources (Lavi & Friesem, Reference Lavi and Friesem2019; Lewis et al., Reference Lewis, Vinicius, Strods, Mace and Migliano2014; Peterson, Reference Peterson1993) and high levels of mobility (Kelly, Reference Kelly1983; MacDonald & Hewlett, Reference MacDonald and Hewlett1999). Behavioural flexibility and increased rates of innovation may also help mitigate resource fluctuation. Indeed, several modelling studies suggest that rates of innovation increase in fluctuating environments (Acerbi & Parisi, Reference Acerbi and Parisi2006; Fogarty et al., Reference Fogarty, Creanza and Feldman2015; Fogarty & Creanza, Reference Fogarty and Creanza2017). An ethnographic survey of 20 hunter–gatherer societies conducted by Collard and colleagues (Reference Collard, Kemery and Banks2005) further showed that communities living in environments with a higher risk of resource failure had more diverse toolkits. Ethnographic studies also suggest that some hunter–gatherer societies exhibit high interpersonal variance in beliefs and skill (e.g. Gardner, Reference Gardner1991). Since diversity probably results in better group-level problem solving skills because individuals can draw upon a breadth of different experiences (Post et al., Reference Post, Lia, DiTomaso, Tirpak and Borwankar2009; Smaldino, Reference Smaldino2014), interpersonal variability may be adaptive to hunter–gatherers because it allows societies to continuously develop diverse toolkits that are better suited to novel environmental circumstances. In what follows, we suggest that aspects of socialisation documented in many contemporary hunter–gatherer societies may foster interpersonal variation and innovation in early life. Specifically, we argue that an emphasis on learning through autonomous exploration, adult and peer teaching, play and innovation seeking in adolescence probably encourages the development of children's tool innovation capabilities, especially in the domain of subsistence. In order to make our case, we link these socialisation practices to existing psychological research focusing on the development of problem-solving skills.

Socialising innovative capabilities

Autonomous exploration

As mentioned, respect for individual autonomy is a central social value in diverse hunter–gatherer societies (Endicott, Reference Endicott, Gibson and Sillander2011; Gardner, Reference Gardner1991). In order to respect individual autonomy, people avoid telling others what to do, and all community members have the freedom to choose their actions, whereabouts, and social associations. This emphasis on autonomy structures children's learning and development (Gardner, Reference Gardner2000; Lavi, Reference Laviin press; Morris, Reference Morris1982). Indeed, autonomy is encouraged from infancy. Studies among the Aka, Batek, Paliyan and Inuit suggest that indulgence in the form of frequent touching, holding and on-demand breastfeeding allows parents to wait for children's initiative before they respond, and thus, supports the development of autonomy (Briggs, Reference Briggs1979; Endicott & Endicott, Reference Endicott, Endicott, McKenna, Gray, Narvaex, Valentino and Fuentes2014; Gardner, Reference Gardner1966; Hewlett, Reference Hewlett, Roopnarine and Carter1992; Hewlett et al., Reference Hewlett, Lamb, Leyendecker and Scholmerich2000). For example, Bird-David (Reference Bird-David2008) argues that Nayaka parents believe that babies feed themselves, rather than being fed by parents.

In diverse hunter–gatherer societies, parents rarely interfere with young children's activities, even when they play with sharp knives or near fires (Crittenden, Reference Crittenden, Meehan and Crittenden2016a; Harris, Reference Harris1980; Hewlett, Reference Hewlett, Roopnarine and Carter1992; Lancy, Reference Lancy2016a, Reference Lancy, Geary and Berchb; Lew-Levy et al., Reference Lew-Levy, Crittenden, Boyette, Mabulla, Hewlett and Lamb2019b; Naveh, Reference Naveh2014). In addition, parents occasionally make toy versions of adult tools for children, including bows, arrows, spears, digging sticks, fishing lines and baskets (Crittenden, Reference Crittenden, Meehan and Crittenden2016a; Dira & Hewlett, Reference Dira and Hewlett2016; Hewlett et al., Reference Hewlett, Fouts, Boyette and Hewlett2011; Imamura, Reference Imamura, Terashima and Hewlett2016; Neuwelt-Truntzer, Reference Neuwelt-Truntzer1981; Nishiaki, Reference Nishiaki, Nishiaki and Aoki2013; Thompson, Reference Thompson2003; Wallace & Hoebel, Reference Wallace and Hoebel1952). Among the Aka, parents also show infants how to use these tools (Hewlett, Reference Hewlett, Roopnarine and Carter1992; Hewlett & Roulette, Reference Hewlett and Roulette2016). From early childhood onwards, children are afforded extensive autonomy to explore their surroundings. By the age of three, Nayaka children circulate among relatives and experiment with tools at will (Lavi, Reference Laviin press). Tsimane children's independent daily travel distance increases with age (Davis & Cashdan, Reference Davis and Cashdan2019, Reference Davis, Cashdan, Ashdown and Faherty2020). Exploration in childhood probably gives children opportunities to learn the causal affordances of their cultural toolkits, observe how parents and other community members use these objects and determine when and where these tools are used (Bjorklund & Gardiner, Reference Bjorklund and Gardiner2012; Davis & Cashdan, Reference Davis, Cashdan, Ashdown and Faherty2020; Lancy, Reference Lancy2016a, Reference Lancy2017; Riede et al., Reference Riede, Johannsen, Högberg, Nowell and Lombard2018).

Adult and peer teaching

From an evolutionary perspective, teaching can be defined as ‘behaviour that evolved to facilitate learning in others’ (Kline, Reference Kline2015, p. 6). This definition leads to the broad inclusion of several social learning activities, such as opportunity scaffolding, chore assignment, instruction, correction and negative feedback (e.g. Boyette & Hewlett, Reference Boyette and Hewlett2017; Hewlett & Roulette, Reference Hewlett and Roulette2016; Kline, Reference Kline2015). While lesson-style didactic teaching is commonly observed in WEIRD societies and frequently exported to non-WEIRD societies in school settings (Rogoff et al., Reference Rogoff, Matusov, White, Olson and Torrance1996, Reference Rogoff, Paradise, Arauz, Correa-Chávez and Angelillo2003), such out-of-context child-focused activities probably play a limited role in knowledge acquisition in small-scale societies (Lancy, Reference Lancy2010, Reference Lancy, Geary and Berch2016b). Instead, more subtle forms of teaching, which tend to be embedded within meaningful community activities, are abundant. For example, among the Baka, children position themselves in participatory situations, such as by assisting in butchering, where they can overhear and elicit teaching from adults (Sonoda, Reference Sonoda, Terashima and Hewlett2016a, Reference Sonodab). Similarly, Christian and Gardner (Reference Christian and Gardner1977) note that parents make efforts to induce a Dene child to learn by listening and observing adults. In most cases, however, it is believed that the learner decides whether – and to what – she or he listens (Bombjaková, Reference Bombjaková2018; Christian & Gardner, Reference Christian and Gardner1977).

When it comes to tool manufacture, in many cases parents teach through opportunity scaffolding, during which a caregiver provides a child with an object, but does not provide cues on how this object should be used (Hewlett & Roulette, Reference Hewlett and Roulette2016; see also Kline, Reference Kline2016). For example, among the Gidra, parents give children well-made child-sized bows as gifts (Nishiaki, Reference Nishiaki, Nishiaki and Aoki2013). Gidra children are expected to discover how to reproduce these bows without direct intervention from adults, and begin to skilfully produce bows at around 14 years of age. Similarly, Aka adults frequently made net fragments available to children, possibly so that children could reverse engineer their manufacture (Neuwelt-Truntzer, Reference Neuwelt-Truntzer1981). Nayaka adults refrain from interfering and instructing when children experiment with trap setting, preferring to let children learn from their own errors (Naveh, Reference Naveh2016).

Object exploration may facilitate children's innovative capabilities. Bonawitz et al. (Reference Bonawitz, Shafto, Gweon, Chang, Katz and Schulz2009, Reference Bonawitz, Shafto, Gweon, Goodman, Spelke and Schulz2011), working with WEIRD pre-schoolers, examined the role of pedagogy in children's propensity for exploration. In their experiment, demonstrators presented children with a novel toy which could be manipulated to produce hidden functions, such as a squeaking sound, music or flashing light. In one condition, experimenters demonstrated a single function of the toy, while in another condition, children were left to explore the toy themselves. Children who were left to explore the toy autonomously discovered more of the toy's affordances than those in the pedagogical demonstration condition. By facilitating tool exploration through opportunity scaffolding, parents may encourage the discovery of novel object affordances.

While in situ learning is the norm in most societies surveyed, not all knowledge can be acquired through direct experience. In such cases, storytelling may be central to knowledge transmission among hunter–gatherers (Scalise Sugiyama, Reference Scalise Sugiyama2011; Weissner, Reference Weissner2014). Stories often address recurrent problems, such as inclement weather, missed hunting opportunities and the violation of social norms and practices. By listening to stories, children acquire a cumulative body of knowledge that they would be unable to develop independently, and can generalise this knowledge to solve unfamiliar problems (Scalise Sugiyama, Reference Scalise Sugiyama2017). Further, by listening to stories, learners gain new information that they can experiment with, improve upon and incorporate into their behavioural repertoire (Scalise Sugiyama, Reference Scalise Sugiyama2017), leading to innovative ways of performing tasks over time.

Horizontal teaching via demonstration, commands, feedback and instruction also plays a central role in knowledge acquisition from middle childhood onwards. Several developmental studies in WEIRD societies suggest that peer learning and teaching, or what Tomasello et al. (Reference Tomasello, Kruger and Ratner1993) call collaborative learning, increases children's ability to solve novel tasks (see also Azmitia, Reference Azmitia1988; Perlmutter et al., Reference Perlmutter, Kuo, Behrend and Muller1989; Rendell et al., Reference Rendell, Fogarty, Hoppitt, Morgan, Webster and Laland2011). For example, children are more likely to employ logical reasoning when discussing a task with a peer than an adult (Kruger & Tomasello, Reference Kruger and Tomasello1986; Tomasello et al., Reference Tomasello, Kruger and Ratner1993). Peer teaching may be beneficial because it forces children to take on another's perspective and assume complementary roles, ultimately facilitating the incorporation of new problem-solving stances into children's repertoire (Damon, Reference Damon1984; Kruger & Tomasello, Reference Kruger and Tomasello1986; Phelps & Damon, Reference Phelps and Damon1989). In school settings, collaborative learning has been shown to be generative, in the sense that, by sharing information with peers, children can produce new knowledge unknown to either peer, including in the domains of mathematical concepts, moralistic reasoning and Piagetian conservation (Ames & Murray, Reference Ames and Murray1982; Forman, Reference Forman1989; Phelps & Damon, Reference Phelps and Damon1989; Kruger, Reference Kruger1992). Modelling studies suggest that horizontal transmission is especially important in fluctuating environments because it ‘creates the conditions for exploring the space of possible behaviours and for the emergence of the new behaviours appropriate to the changed environments’ (Acerbi & Parisi, Reference Acerbi and Parisi2006, para. 4.3).

A small but growing body of evidence suggests that collaborative horizontal learning is central to knowledge transmission in hunter–gatherer societies. For example, in a study of teaching subsistence skills using structured behavioural observations of Hadza and BaYaka 3–18-year-olds, child-to-child teaching represented approximately 75% of the observed teaching interactions, even as adults were in visual or auditory range of surveyed children 57–69% of the time (Lew-Levy et al., Reference Lew-Levy, Kissler, Boyette, Crittenden, Mabulla and Hewlett2020; see also Boyette & Hewlett, Reference Boyette and Hewlett2017). Among the San (Imamura, Reference Imamura, Terashima and Hewlett2016; Imamura & Akiyama, Reference Imamura and Akiyama2016; Shostak, Reference Shostak, Lee and DeVore1976, Reference Shostak1981), Chabu (Dira & Hewlett, Reference Dira and Hewlett2016), Aka (Boyette & Hewlett, Reference Boyette and Hewlett2017; Hewlett et al., Reference Hewlett, Fouts, Boyette and Hewlett2011), Baka (Gallois et al., Reference Gallois, Duda, Hewlett and Reyes-García2015, Reference Gallois, Duda and Reyes-Garcia2017, Reference Gallois, Lubbers, Hewlett and Reyes-García2018), BaYaka (Lewis, Reference Lewis2002; Salali et al., Reference Salali, Chaudhary, Thompson, Grace, van der Burgt, Dyble and Migliano2016, Reference Salali, Chaudhary, Bouer, Thompson, Vinicius and Migliano2019), Kaytetye (Thompson, Reference Thompson2003), Jenu Kuruba (Demps et al., Reference Demps, Zorondo-Rodríguez, García and Reyes-García2012), Batek (Lye, Reference Lye1997), Agta (Hagen et al., Reference Hagen, Ploeg and Minter2016), Pitjantjatjara (Ilyatjari, Reference Ilyatjari1991) and Hadza (Crittenden, Reference Crittenden, Meehan and Crittenden2016a), researchers report that hunting, tree climbing, navigation, fishing, tool manufacture, medicinal plant knowledge and foraging knowledge are learned from and with other children. Such collaborative learning may improve children's ability to find novel ways of producing material culture.

Learning through play

Play makes up a large proportion of the time budgets of all juvenile mammals (Bekoff & Byers, Reference Bekoff and Byers1992), and involves the combination and recombination of established patterns and actions, which, over time, contributes to behavioural flexibility (Bateson, Reference Bateson2014; Fagen, Reference Fagen1981). Since play usually emulates the behaviours of mature species, play arguably allows juveniles to practice adult behaviours in safe settings (Smith, Reference Smith1982). Unlike other mammals, humans engage in a species-specific type of play, known as pretence play, which involves ‘the projecting of a supposed situation onto an actual one, in the spirit of fun’ (Lillard, Reference Lillard1993, p. 349). Cognitively, pretence play involves ‘a capacity to generate, and to reason with, novel suppositions or imaginary scenarios’ (Carruthers, Reference Carruthers2002, p. 229). Thus, pretence play may lay the groundwork for innovative thinking and problem-solving skills (Carruthers, Reference Carruthers2002).

Many scholars report that much learning occurs in the mixed-sex, multi-age playgroup in hunter–gatherer societies (see Konner, Reference Konner, Hewlett and Lamb2005, Reference Konner, Meehan and Crittenden2016 for review). Pretence play, and specifically, work-themed pretence play, makes up about 20% of hunter–gatherer children's play time (see Boyette, Reference Boyette2018 for review). During pretence, children emulate adult social behaviours and practise subsistence skills (Fouts et al., Reference Fouts, Bader and Neitzel2016; Gosso et al., Reference Gosso, Morais and Otta2007; Lew-Levy & Boyette, Reference Lew-Levy and Boyette2018; MacDonald, Reference MacDonald2007; Morelli et al., Reference Morelli, Rogoff and Angelillo2003; Neuwelt-Truntzer, Reference Neuwelt-Truntzer1981). For example, children build small huts with hearths adjacent to adult camps (Bombjaková, Reference Bombjaková2018; Crittenden, Reference Crittenden, Meehan and Crittenden2016a; Flannery, Reference Flannery1953; Ilyatjari, Reference Ilyatjari1991; Lewis, Reference Lewis2002; Lew-Levy et al., Reference Lew-Levy, Boyette, Crittenden, Hewlett and Lamb2019a; Mackie et al., Reference Mackie, Surovell and O'Brien2015; Neuwelt-Truntzer, Reference Neuwelt-Truntzer1981; Shostak, Reference Shostak, Lee and DeVore1976; Thompson, Reference Thompson2003; Tonkinson, Reference Tonkinson1978; Vanstone, Reference Vanstone1965). In these play camps, children emulate the sexual division of labour, with boys pretending to hunt or hunting small game such as rodents or birds, and girls pretending to, or actually, cooking small versions of meals in cooking pots or tin cans. Among the Hadza and BaYaka, cooked food will carefully be shared among all those present, following the conventions of adult sharing (Crittenden, Reference Crittenden, Terashima and Hewlett2016b; Crittenden & Zes, Reference Crittenden and Zes2015; Lew-Levy et al., Reference Lew-Levy, Boyette, Crittenden, Hewlett and Lamb2019a). Among the Mbuti, Turnbull (Reference Turnbull and Montagu1978) suggests that children emulate recently observed adult fights during play, while coming to a different resolution than adults. While playing hunter and hunted, Nayaka children vocalised animals’ fears, feelings and emotions (Naveh, Reference Naveh2014). Through these early play experiences, children both develop the subsistence skills necessary for full participation in the family economy and learn the rules of moral and social engagement (Bird & Bliege Bird, Reference Bird and Bliege Bird2002, Reference Bird, Bliege Bird, Hewlett and Lamb2005; Bliege Bird & Bird, Reference Bliege Bird and Bird2002; Bock & Johnson, Reference Bock and Johnson2004; Boyette, Reference Boyette2019; Crittenden et al., Reference Crittenden, Conklin-Brittain, Zes, Schoeninger and Marlowe2013; Crittenden, Reference Crittenden, Meehan and Crittenden2016a; Gallois et al., Reference Gallois, Duda, Hewlett and Reyes-García2015; Hewlett & Cavalli-Sforza, Reference Hewlett and Cavalli-Sforza1986; Lye, Reference Lye1997; Tucker & Young, Reference Tucker, Young, Hewlett and Lamb2005). For example, Gardner (Reference Gardner1966) argues that Paliyan children are socially skilled and independent by the age of eight, and economically independent between the ages of 13 and 14. Likewise, Harris (Reference Harris1980) describes Yolngu children between the ages of 6 and 8 foraging, fishing, and swimming away from the supervision of adults.

While some of the cognitive benefits associated with pretence play may be deferred to adulthood, several developmental studies conducted in WEIRD societies suggest that children's play improves their problem-solving skills in the short term (Pellegrini & Gustafson, Reference Pellegrini, Gustafson, Pellegrini and Smith2005; Sylva et al., Reference Sylva, Bruner, Genova, Bruner, Jolly and Sylva1976). For example, 3–5-year-olds who had the opportunity to incorporate sticks and clamps into their free play prior to a problem-solving task were able to combine these objects in novel ways to retrieve chalk from a box more quickly than children who were shown how to clamp the sticks together by a demonstrator (Sylva et al., Reference Sylva, Bruner, Genova, Bruner, Jolly and Sylva1976). The positive effect of play over demonstration on children's problem solving was heightened in problem-solving tasks that required more complex solutions (Smith & Dutton, Reference Smith and Dutton1979). Since children are smaller and weaker than adults, they face different adaptive challenges when participating in foraging; size and strength are a greater constraint for children than adults, while free time is not (Bliege Bird & Bird, Reference Bliege Bird and Bird2002; Riede et al., Reference Riede, Johannsen, Högberg, Nowell and Lombard2018; Tucker & Young, Reference Tucker, Young, Hewlett and Lamb2005). As a result, children in hunter–gatherer societies sometimes use distinct, child-specific technologies when participating in food collecting. For example, Hadza children set sticky traps for collecting weaverbirds, an activity not conducted by adults (Crittenden, Reference Crittenden, Meehan and Crittenden2016a). Mikea children target smaller and shallower ovy tubers ignored by adult foragers (Tucker & Young, Reference Tucker, Young, Hewlett and Lamb2005). Baka children use tools unique to the playgroup, such as slingshots and small bows for hunting birds, squirrels and mice (Gallois et al., Reference Gallois, Duda and Reyes-Garcia2017). Baka children also develop unique names for edible plants, birds and mice that adults do not recognise. It is possible that the extensive participation in pretence play not only prepares children for adult work, but also generates child-specific food-producing innovations.

Innovation seeking in adolescence

Features of adolescence observed among several hunter–gatherer societies make this developmental period especially tailored to learning innovations (Hewlett, Reference Hewlettin press, Reference Hewlett, Akazawa, Nishiaki and Aoki2013, Reference Hewlett, Terashima and Hewlett2016). First, since most basic competencies are acquired by early adolescence, older adolescents may seek out more knowledgeable models from whom they can learn specialised skills or refine previously acquired skills (Henrich & Gil-White, Reference Henrich and Gil-White2001; Hewlett & Hewlett, Reference Hewlett, Hewlett and Hewlett2012), such as in the domains of basketry (Puri, Reference Puri, Ellen, Lycett and Johns2013), hunting (Dira & Hewlett, Reference Dira and Hewlett2016) and hide work (Erikson, Reference Erikson1939; Ohmagari & Berkes, Reference Ohmagari and Berkes1997), and for the manufacture of skis, sledges and canoes (Jordan, Reference Jordan2014). Second, adolescents have more free time than adults; while adolescents can, and often do, participate in many aspects of subsistence and childcare, they are not required to do so (Hewlett & Hewlett, Reference Hewlett, Hewlett and Hewlett2012). Compared with farmers, hunter–gatherer adolescence is characterised by greater sexual freedom (Hewlett & Hewlett, Reference Hewlett, Hewlett and Hewlett2012; Konner, Reference Konner, Hewlett and Lamb2005). Furthermore, with excess time and an increasing desire to find mates, adolescents, and particularly boys, often travel long distances (Hewlett & Hewlett, Reference Hewlett, Hewlett and Hewlett2012; MacDonald & Hewlett, Reference MacDonald and Hewlett1999), which provide opportunities to encounter, or even seek out, innovations from afar.

Three studies by Hewlett (Reference Hewlettin press, Reference Hewlett, Akazawa, Nishiaki and Aoki2013, Reference Dira and Hewlett2016) working with the Aka and Chabu suggest that, indeed, adolescents are highly receptive to acquiring novel technologies and culture forms. Both Aka and Chabu adolescents identified innovations as modifications and recombinations of previous technologies, such as new trap, house, basketry or pottery designs. In addition, Chabu adolescents identified innovations as being sought after. Aka adolescents identified innovators as calm and wise, while Chabu adolescents identified innovators as hard workers, kind and generous. Thus, for both the Chabu and Aka, prosociality was a central characteristic of innovators. Both Chabu and Aka adolescents were willing to travel long distances to learn from particularly good and innovative teachers, usually via oblique transmission. In both societies, adolescents autonomously selected individuals from whom to learn innovations. Aka adolescents were keener to learn innovative behaviours than adults, and stated that one of the reasons they did so was to impress potential mates, and to lead a good life. Chabu adolescents more frequently listed self-sufficiency and attracting mates as reasons to learn innovations. Both Aka and Chabu adolescents stated that learning innovations, particularly in the realm of subsistence and/or trade, is beneficial for supporting their parents and/or a future family. These studies suggest that adolescence is an opportune and adapted period of development for learning technological innovations.

Archaeological implications

In archaeology, a growing number of researchers are considering the role of children and adolescents in the production and reproduction of technological and cultural products (e.g. Lillehammer, Reference Lillehammer2010a, Reference Lillehammer2010b; Nowell, Reference Nowell2015, Reference Nowell, Haidle, Conard and Bolus2016; Tehrani & Riede, Reference Tehrani and Riede2008), including innovations (Riede et al., Reference Riede, Johannsen, Högberg, Nowell and Lombard2018). However, identifying child and adolescent innovators in the past has been hindered by the resolution the archaeological record has to offer. As opposed to the contemporary ethnographic context where fine-grained social processes can be directly observed, the nature of the archaeological record only allows us to infer social processes through the prism of material evidence, which is incomplete owing to archaeological formation and taphonomic processes (see Kelly et al., Reference Kelly, Pelton, Robinson, Lavi and Friesem2019 for discussion). To elucidate invisible social processes, ethnoarchaeologists have, at times, used selective ethnographic examples to support universal models of the human past, or to represent people of the past as ‘premodern’ or ‘primitive’ (see Athreya & Rogers Ackermann, Reference Athreya, Rogers Ackermann, Porr and Mathews2018; French, Reference French2019; Gosselain, Reference Gosselain2016 for discussion). In the present paper, we reject a direct analogy between contemporary hunter–gatherer societies and the day-to-day reality of past hunter–gatherers. However, by calling upon evidence from a wide array of ethnographic, experimental and psychological studies, archaeologists can broaden their interpretive framework for understanding the cultural and behavioural diversity of past societies. Here, we hope to show that ethnographic data from contemporary hunter–gatherer societies can enrich how archaeologists think about, and interpret the archaeological contributions of, children and adolescents as innovators. In what follows, we examine whether the socialisation practices described above can be interpreted from the archaeological record. We focus on archaeological sites attributed to Homo sapiens, in order to avoid current debates around the limits of comparison of ethnographic data to other species of Homo (French, Reference French2019), as well as debates around the authorship of transitional industries (e.g. Higham et al., Reference Higham, Douka, Wood, Ramsey, Brock, Basell and Jacobi2014; Negrino & Riel-Salvatore, Reference Negrino, Riel-Salvatore, Borgia and Cristiani2018). Nonetheless, many of the perspectives discussed could be relevant to other species of Homo (e.g. Nowell, Reference Nowell, Haidle, Conard and Bolus2016; Spikins et al., Reference Spikins, Hitchens, Needham and Rutherford2014).

Autonomous exploration

Numerous artefacts have been interpreted as child-sized tools. For example, sites from North America and Europe have produced complete spears associated with the burial of adolescents (Trinkaus & Buzhilova, Reference Trinkaus and Buzhilova2018), miniature spear throwers from Thule cultures and the Oregon Coast (Losey & Hull, Reference Losey and Hull2019; Park, Reference Park1998), harpoon and dart heads from Thule and Dorset sites (Kenyon & Arnold, Reference Kenyon and Arnold1985; Park, Reference Park1998; Park & Mousseau, Reference Park and Mousseau2003), as well as bows, arrows and projectile tips of varying materials from Paleoindian and Palaeolithic European sites (Dawe, Reference Dawe1997; Frison, Reference Frison1970; Kenyon & Arnold, Reference Kenyon and Arnold1985; Langley, Reference Langley2018; Park, Reference Park1998; Rosendahl et al., Reference Rosendahl, Beinhauer, Löscher, Kreipl, Walter and Rosendahl2006). Some of these weapons and weapon components may be functional (e.g. Dawe, Reference Dawe1997; Kenyon & Arnold, Reference Kenyon and Arnold1985; Losey & Hull, Reference Losey and Hull2019; Rosendahl et al., Reference Rosendahl, Beinhauer, Löscher, Kreipl, Walter and Rosendahl2006), while others are deemed too small to have been functionally useful, and thus have been interpreted as toys used in imitative play (e.g. Kenyon & Arnold, Reference Kenyon and Arnold1985). Some artefacts may have been manufactured by children; poorly made arrows from Rosebud Creek in Montana were arguably made for and by children (Dawe, Reference Dawe1997). Atypical use-wear patterns on poorly and irregularly shaped points at Trollesgave in Denmark have been interpreted as showing that they were used by children for woodworking and meat cutting (Donahue & Fischer, Reference Donahue and Fischer2015), suggesting that children participated in work activities at this site. Whether scaled-down functional tools, or toys, these objects could have facilitated enskilment in the manufacture and use of complex tools.

Another line of evidence for childhood autonomy comes from Australian and European cave sites. Finger flutings and footprints may have been made by children exploring caves, including dangerous and difficult to access areas, either without adults (Bednarik, Reference Bednarik1986; Roveland, Reference Roveland2000; Van Gelder, Reference Van Gelder2015b), or with older members of a group (e.g. Romano et al., Reference Romano, Citton, Salvador, Arobba, Rellini, Firpo, Negrino and Avan2019). The ability to explore tools and sites may have provided children with the opportunity to familiarise themselves with the affordances of their material culture and environment. For example, although not direct archaeological evidence, in an experimental study, a child innovated bipolar knapping. Following a period of attempting but failing to imitate the adult producing bladelets by holding the core on their lap, the child proceeded to place the flint on the pavement, essentially bipolar knapping on an anvil, thereby providing a solution to problems with motor control and hand–eye coordination (Sternke & Sørensen, Reference Sternke and Sørensen2009). Such creative solutions to child-sized problems serve to illustrate the positive consequences of allowing independent exploration, potentially leading to innovation.

Adult and peer teaching

Lithics comprise an important evidence base for highlighting potential teaching in the archaeological record. The production sequence for making stone tools, known as the chaîne opératoire approach to analysis, has often been operationalised to identify instances of pedagogy in past societies (e.g. Audouze & Cattin Reference Audouze and Cattin2011; Fischer, Reference Fischer1990a, Reference Fischer, Cziesla, Eickhoff, Arts and Winterb; Grimm, Reference Grimm and Derevenski2000; Karlin et al., Reference Karlin, Ploux, Bodu, Pigeot, Berthelet and Chavaillon1993; Pigeot, Reference Pigeot1990; Takakura, Reference Takakura2013; Cunnar Reference Cunnar2015). Some scholars proposed that the spatial distributions of lithic raw material, tools and production waste may reflect different skill levels, indicating that novices were directly instructed by, or sought input from, experts (e.g. Fisher Reference Fischer1990a, Reference Fischer, Cziesla, Eickhoff, Arts and Winterb; Grimm Reference Grimm and Derevenski2000; Karlin et al. Reference Karlin, Ploux, Bodu, Pigeot, Berthelet and Chavaillon1993). For example, at Palaeolithic and Paleoindian sites in Europe, Japan and North America, archaeologists have interpreted lithic cores, knapping scatters and/or completed preforms and tools left at the site as being produced specifically for the purpose of teaching (e.g. Audouze & Cattin, Reference Audouze and Cattin2011; Bodu et al., Reference Bodu, Karlin, Ploux and Cziesla1990; Cattin, Reference Cattin, Zubrow, Audouze and Enloe2010; Cunnar, Reference Cunnar2015; Fischer, Reference Fischer1990a, Reference Fischer, Cziesla, Eickhoff, Arts and Winterb; Karlin et al., Reference Karlin, Ploux, Bodu, Pigeot, Berthelet and Chavaillon1993; Simonet, Reference Simonet2009, Reference Simonet2012; Takakura, Reference Takakura2013). Academic cores (Johansen & Stapert, Reference Johansen and Stapert2008), involving the expert production of blade manufacture, with all by-products left at the site, have been found at Hattoridai 2 (Japan) and Pincevent (France) (e.g. Bodu et al., Reference Bodu, Karlin, Ploux and Cziesla1990; Karlin et al., Reference Karlin, Ploux, Bodu, Pigeot, Berthelet and Chavaillon1993; Takakura, Reference Takakura2013). Poorly executed preforms and debitage scatters, positioned in an arc around more expertly produced debitage, have been interpreted as face-to-face learning experiences in the Great Basin (USA) (Cunnar, Reference Cunnar2015) and at Trollesgave (Denmark) (Fischer, Reference Fischer1990a, Reference Fischer, Cziesla, Eickhoff, Arts and Winterb).

At some sites, spatial patterns suggest that novice knappers were more spatially distant from skilled knappers, and therefore may not have received direct instruction at the time that novice knapping occurred. For example, at Etiolles (France), Pigeot (Reference Pigeot1990) argues that master knappers worked close to the hearth, while less experienced knappers were kept on the outer edges of the knapping workshop. Based on an analysis of lithic assemblages at Hattoridai 2 (Japan), Takakura (Reference Takakura2013, p. 160) argued that ‘the activity zones of the skilled knappers and novice knappers were differentiated based on well-defined spatial rules’. At Solvieux (France), Grimm (Reference Grimm and Derevenski2000, p. 64) interprets lithic products from Location 1 as suggesting that novice flintknappers ‘may also occupy a space of benign community neglect […] where they configure their own learning relations with other apprentices’. Such spatially distinct practice areas may have served as a setting for peer teaching. At some of these sites, authors make the case for multiple learning processes, including expert–novice teaching and spatially distinct novice practice areas (e.g. Grimm, Reference Grimm and Derevenski2000; Takakura, Reference Takakura2013).

It is important to note that the interpretation of social interactions based on spatial patterning should be treated very carefully, especially in Stone Age, Palaeolithic or Paleoindian sites. For example, Hammond and Hammond (Reference Hammond and Hammond1981) suggest that children might disrupt original spatial patterning when experimenting with knapping and while collecting raw materials and tools left on the activity's surface. The clearing away of debitage into ‘dump’ areas can also disrupt or eliminate evidence of original activity areas (e.g. Bodu et al., Reference Bodu, Karlin, Ploux and Cziesla1990). Furthermore, the archaeological resolution in such sites rarely enables us to unequivocally determine the contemporaneity of such deposits (Bailey, Reference Bailey2007). For instance, whether more skilful knapping happened alongside novice production or a few hours/days/months later can be difficult to determine given the archaeological resolution. It is also debatable whether poorly manufactured tools should be used as a direct evidence for inexperience (see discussion in Hovers et al., Reference Hovers, Malinksy-Buler, Goder-Goldberger, Keshtain, Le Tensorer, Jagher and Otte2011).

Nonetheless, experimental archaeological studies can help researchers consider the social interactions involved during learning in the deep past (see d'Errico & Banks, Reference d'Errico and Banks2015 for a conceptual framework on teaching in Palaeolithic archaeology). For instance, Tostevin (Reference Tostevin, Lavi and Friesem2019) argues that learning flint knapping requires close intimacy and sharing of time. An experimental study performed by Putt et al. (Reference Putt, Woods and Franciscus2014) suggests that novice knappers produced more efficient flakes in non-verbal lessons compared with novice knappers who received verbal instruction. Thus, while archaeological evidence for face-to-face learning and the involvement of didactic lesson-style teaching as opposed to non-verbal observations and trial and error remains elusive, the convergence of ethnographic evidence for participatory teaching reviewed previously and the experimental findings outlined here brings to the forefront the possibility that didactic lessons may not be necessary to the successful teaching of complex skills, and also compel us to consider the role of peer interactions during the acquisition of knapping skill.

Beyond lithics, European Upper Palaeolithic parietal art, particularly illustrations of animals depicting their behaviours, social structures, physiology, kill zones through placement of weaponry and movement may have served the purpose of educating younger members of community about animal ethology and hunting (Azéma & Rivère, Reference Azéma and Rivère2012; Guthrie, Reference Guthrie2005; Mithen, Reference Mithen1988). Cooney (Reference Cooney, Crawford, Hadley and Shepherd2018) further argues that children may also have participated in creating parietal art from early ages. Lombard (Reference Lombard2015) argues that making and observing parietal art may have been used as a tool to help children learn, and generalise from, the cumulative body of knowledge that makes up complex skills, such as hunting.

Learning through play

Numerous artefacts have been interpreted as toys, including dolls, balls, a toy snow knife, miniature sleds, animal and human figurines, and a shell (Jacobi, Reference Jacobi2004; Kenyon & Arnold, Reference Kenyon and Arnold1985; Langley, Reference Langley2018; Park, Reference Park2006; Politis, Reference Politis1998; Riede et al., Reference Riede, Johannsen, Högberg, Nowell and Lombard2018). Lithic assemblages can also be interpreted as evidence for flint knapping by ‘beginners’ as part of ‘play’, particularly in assemblages where evidence of unskilled flintknapping appears to be unstructured and/or highly variable (e.g. Bodu et al., Reference Bodu, Karlin, Ploux and Cziesla1990). Finger flutings, prints or stencils of hands, fingers and other body parts found in caves may represent children playing with clay, and possibly making of figurative art (Bednarik, Reference Bednarik1986, Reference Bednarik2008; Cooney, Reference Cooney, Crawford, Hadley and Shepherd2018; Cooney Williams & Janik, Reference Cooney Williams and Janik2018; Groenen, Reference Groenen1988; Hallam, Reference Hallam1971; Romano et al., Reference Romano, Citton, Salvador, Arobba, Rellini, Firpo, Negrino and Avan2019; Sharpe & Van Gelder, Reference Sharpe and Van Gelder2006; Van Gelder, Reference Van Gelder2015a). Stone rings found in northeast Greenland may represent elaborate playhouses made by Thule children (Hardenberg, Reference Hardenberg2010). Using stone and other materials, children are believed to have constructed a range of miniature summer and winter houses, as well as doll houses, in which objects representing key items such as ‘meat’ or ‘blubber’ were included. These houses may be compelling examples of the aforementioned pretence play, which underpin a capacity for innovation.

Toys may have also been incorporated into children's pretence play. For example, thaumatropes are bone discs on which an image is engraved on either side. By attaching a cord thread to the centre, the disc can be flipped back and forth, creating an optical illusion of a moving picture, such as a running doe (Azéma & Rivère, Reference Azéma and Rivère2012; Langley, Reference Langley2018; Nowell, Reference Nowell2015; Riede et al., Reference Riede, Johannsen, Högberg, Nowell and Lombard2018). Riede et al. (Reference Riede, Johannsen, Högberg, Nowell and Lombard2018) interpret these bone discs as potential optical toys used to entertain children while helping them learn animal gaits, and may have also helped novice fibre spinners learn to twist and turn thread, thus developing the manual skills necessary for producing cordage. Thus, thaumatropes may serve as an example of how play, learning, technology and innovation are intertwined. Whether autonomously or while participating in community events (Cooney Williams & Janik, Reference Cooney Williams and Janik2018; Nowell, Reference Nowell2015), play was central to children's lived experience in the past, and probably represented an important avenue for learning.

Innovation seeking in adolescence

Several archaeologists suggest that, owing to availability of raw material, skill acquisition may have been limited to specific seasons and/or locations (e.g. Eigeland, Reference Eigeland2011; Milne, Reference Milne2005; Simonet, Reference Simonet2009, Reference Simonet2012; Sternke, Reference Sternke2011). Such learning experiences may have been reserved for older adolescents. For example, on Baffin Island, chert is only available during the summer months. Milne (Reference Milne2005) argues that, during this time, older adolescents who were strong and skilled enough probably trekked inland carrying chert nodules some 10 km from the source to the workshop, and participated in goose hunting. Since raw material would have been abundant at the teaching workshop, adolescents may have had ample opportunity to observe, experiment with, and learn new knapping innovations from especially skilled teachers.

Concluding Remarks

In this paper, we have reviewed the psychological and ethnographic evidence for hunter–gatherer children as tool innovators. We have argued that the developmental niche of many hunting and gathering societies is conducive to socialisation practices that encourage child and adolescent innovative capabilities. These socialisation practices include autonomous exploration, peer and adult teaching, play, and innovation seeking in adolescence. While few empirical studies have directly investigated children's innovations, we found compelling evidence for children as innovators of child-sized subsistence technologies (Crittenden, Reference Crittenden, Meehan and Crittenden2016a; Gallois et al., Reference Gallois, Duda and Reyes-Garcia2017), and adolescents as active acquirers of innovations (Hewlett, Reference Hewlettin press, Reference Hewlett, Akazawa, Nishiaki and Aoki2013, Reference Dira and Hewlett2016). In doing so, this paper contributes an ethnographically grounded perspective on the cultural contexts which may favour the emergence of innovation in childhood. Further, using archaeological examples, we have provided an interpretive framework through which to consider child and adolescent innovators, and have pointed to some of the methodological challenges associated with identifying complex social learning processes in the past.

This review points to several next steps for broadening our understanding of cross-cultural variation in childhood innovation generally, and among hunter–gatherers specifically. For psychology, while the hook task and variations thereof have helped researchers understand the cognitive underpinnings of innovation, the novelty of the materials and the strangeness of the testing settings make it difficult to conduct in small-scale societies. In order to empirically assess the ontogenesis of innovative capacities in children, our metrics themselves must become more innovative. In addition to reward extraction tasks, we might consider measuring innovation in more naturalistic settings. Studies focused on how children innovate both individually and in groups, with familiar and unfamiliar materials, and that are based on the adaptive relevance of the outcome, are needed.

Ethnographers working with hunter–gatherer children should pay close attention to aspects of child cultures. In particular, we do not know how games, tools and novel subsistence strategies are developed, and whether individual children or groups of children are the sources of these innovations. Further, it is unknown whether adults adopt innovations generated by children, or if these remain within the playgroup. Building on Hewlett's (Reference Hewlettin press, Reference Hewlett, Akazawa, Nishiaki and Aoki2013, Reference Dira and Hewlett2016) diligent work on hunter–gatherer adolescents as innovation acquirers, more work is also needed on the role of adolescents in the transmission and generation of innovations in hunter–gatherer societies. Ethnographers should also examine how the considerable variation within and between hunter–gatherer societies influences children's innovative capabilities. Children inhabiting different ecologies may face different risks, limiting their ability to explore their environments autonomously (e.g. Blurton Jones et al., Reference Blurton Jones, Hawkes, Draper, Burch and Ellanna1994; Hawkes et al., Reference Hawkes, O'Connell and Blurton Jones1995). Within societies, sex differences in behaviour may also influence children's autonomous exploration (Draper, Reference Draper1975; Froehle et al., Reference Froehle, Wells, Pollom, Mabulla, Lew-Levy and Crittenden2019; Neuwelt-Truntzer, Reference Neuwelt-Truntzer1981). Beyond hunter–gatherer societies, changing social and ecological environments, and individualism, may lead to high rates of childhood innovations in other subsistence strategies as well (e.g. Glowacki & Molleman, Reference Glowacki and Molleman2017; Greenfield et al., Reference Greenfield, Maynard and Childs2000). As state intervention and formal schooling become part of hunter–gatherer children's social worlds (e.g. Lavi, Reference Lavi2018; Pollom et al., Reference Pollom, Herlosky, Mabulla and Crittenden2020; Reyes-García & Pyhälä, Reference Reyes-García and Pyhälä2016), understanding how subsistence strategy and cultural transitions affect children's social learning experiences and innovative capabilities is also paramount.

Finally, for archaeology, the study of site formation processes is essential to better understand the integrity of the archaeological context generally, and spatial patterning specifically. In addition, increasing the focus on the effects of skill level in experimental archaeology will further improve our ability to assess technologies and better understand the processes involved in learning to use them (e.g. Eren et al., Reference Eren, Lycett, Patten, Buchanan, Pargeter and O'Brien2016; Milks, Reference Milks2019; Whittaker et al., Reference Whittaker, Pettigrew and Grohsmeyer2017; Whittaker & Kamp, Reference Whittaker and Kamp2006). Experimentation using unskilled WEIRD adults, whose educational experiences may shape their ability and willingness to learn in more exploratory and autonomous ways, can only go part of the way towards better identifying children in the archaeological record. In addition to adults having been socialised to learn in particular ways, there are also multiple physiological differences including motor skills, strength and neurological functions (e.g. Ford et al., Reference Ford, Ward, Hodges and Williams2009; Voelcker-Rehage, Reference Voelcker-Rehage2008; Voelcker-Rehage & Willimczik, Reference Voelcker-Rehage and Willimczik2006). While ethical considerations can pose greater challenges in recruiting children for experimental studies, these challenges are not insurmountable. In particular, ethnoarchaeological research with hunter–gatherer children (e.g. Politis, Reference Politis1998) can shed new light on how children play, learn and behave within their communities, and how these behaviours impact the formation of archaeological sites.

Acknowledgement

Thanks to participants of the Children & Innovation workshop for feedback on an earlier version of this paper. Thanks to Michael Vine for suggestions regarding the adaptability of behavioural flexibility in hunter–gatherer societies. Thanks to Alyssa Crittenden for suggestions regarding contextualising the inclusion of hunter–gatherers in the study of human evolution. Finally, thanks to Felix Riede and Shumon Hussain for comments on the manuscript.

Author contributions

SLL conceived of the paper. All authors wrote the manuscript and reviewed the final draft.

Financial support

SLL was funded by the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada Postdoctoral Fellowship (award no. 756-2019-0102).

Conflicts of interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

Research transparency and reproducibility

No data are associated with this article.

References

Acerbi, A., & Parisi, D. (2006). Cultural transmission between and within generations. Journal of Artificial Societies and Social Simulation, 9(1). https://econpapers.repec.org/article/jasjasssj/2005-35-3.htmGoogle Scholar
Adolph, K. E., & Robinson, S. R. (2013). The road to walking: What learning to walk tells us about development. In The Oxford handbook of developmental psychology (Vol 1): Body and mind (pp. 403443). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Ames, G. J., & Murray, F. B. (1982). When two wrongs make a right: Promoting cognitive change by social conflict. Developmental Psychology, 18(6), 894897. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.18.6.894CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Athreya, S., & Rogers Ackermann, R. (2018). Colonialism and narratives of human origins in Asia and Africa. In Porr, M. & Mathews, J. (Eds.), Interrogating human origins: Decolonisation and the deep past. Routledge. https://doi.org/10.31730/osf.io/jtkn2Google Scholar
Audouze, F., & Cattin, M.-I. (2011). Flint wealth versus scarcity: Consequences for Magdalenian apprenticeship. Lithic Technology, 36(2), 109126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Azéma, M., & Rivère, F. (2012). Animation in Palaeolithic art: A pre-echo of cinema. Antiquity, 86, 316324.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Azmitia, M. (1988). Peer Interaction and problem solving: When are two heads better than one? Child Development, 59(1), 8796.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bailey, G. (2007). Time perspectives, palimpsests and the archaeology of time. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology, 26(2), 198223. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaa.2006.08.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barnard, A. (Ed.). (2004). Hunter–Gatherers in History, Archaeology and Anthropology. Bloomsbury.Google Scholar
Bateson, P. (2014). Play, playfulness, creativity and innovation. Animal Behavior and Cognition, 2(2), 99112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beck, S. R., Apperly, I. A., Chappell, J., Guthrie, C., & Cutting, N. (2011). Making tools isn't child's play. Cognition, 119(2), 301306. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2011.01.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beck, S. R., Williams, C., Cutting, N., Apperly, I. A., & Chappell, J. (2016). Individual differences in children's innovative problem-solving are not predicted by divergent thinking or executive functions. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 371(1690). https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2015.0190CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bednarik, R. G. (1986). Parietal finger markings in Europe and Australia. Rock Art Research, 3(1), 3061.Google Scholar
Bednarik, R. G. (2008). Children as Pleistocene artists. Rock Art Research, 25(2), 173182.Google Scholar
Bekoff, M., & Byers, J. A. (1992). Time, energy and play. Animal Behaviour, 44, 981982.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bird-David, N. (1990). The giving environment: Another perspective on the economic system of gatherer–hunters. Current Anthropology, 31(2), 189196.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bird-David, N. (2008). Feeding Nayaka children and English readers: A bifocal ethnography of parental feeding in ‘the giving environment’. Anthropological Quarterly, 81(3), 523550.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bird-David, N. (2017). Us, relatives: Scaling and plural life in a forager world. University of California Press. https://www.ucpress.edu/book/9780520293427/us-relativesCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bird, D. W., Bird, R. B., Codding, B. F., & Zeanah, D. W. (2019). Variability in the organization and size of hunter–gatherer groups: Foragers do not live in small-scale societies. Journal of Human Evolution, 131, 96108. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jhevol.2019.03.005CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bird, D. W., & Bliege Bird, R. (2002). Children on the reef: Slow learning or strategic foraging? Human Nature, 13, 269297.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bird, D. W., & Bliege Bird, R. (2005). Mardu children's hunting strategies in the Western Desert, Australia. In Hewlett, B. S. & Lamb, M. E. (Eds.), Hunter–gatherer childhoods: Evolutionary, developmental and cultural perspectives (pp. 129147). Transaction.Google Scholar
Bird, C. D., & Emery, N. J. (2009). Insightful problem solving and creative tool modification by captive nontool-using rooks. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(25), 1037010375. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0901008106CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bjorklund, D. F., & Gardiner, A. K. (2012). Object play and tool use: Developmental and evolutionary perspectives. In The Oxford handbook of the development of play. Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780195393002.013.0013Google Scholar
Bliege Bird, R., & Bird, D. W. (2002). Constraints of knowing or constraints of growing? Fishing and collecting by the children of Mer. Human Nature, 13, 239267.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blurton Jones, N., Hawkes, K., & Draper, P. (1994). Differences between Hadza and!Kung children's work: Original affluence or practical reason. In Burch, E. S. J. & Ellanna, L. J. (Eds.), Key issues in hunter–gatherer research (pp. 189215). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bock, J., & Johnson, S. E. (2004). Subsistence ecology and play among the Okavango Delta peoples of Botswana. Human Nature, 15(1), 6381.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bodu, P., Karlin, C., & Ploux, S. (1990). Who's who? The Magdalenian flintknappers of Pincevent, France In Cziesla, E. (Ed.) The Big Puzzle: International symposium on refitting stone artefacts, Monrepos. Studies in Modern Archaeology, 1, 143163.Google Scholar
Bombjaková, D. (2018). The role of public speaking, ridicule, and play in cultural transmission among Mbendjele Bayaka forest hunter–gatherers. PhD thesis. UCL.Google Scholar
Bonawitz, E., Shafto, P., Gweon, H., Chang, I., Katz, S., & Schulz, L. (2009). The double-edged sword of pedagogy: Modeling the effect of pedagogical contexts on preschoolers’ exploratory play. Proceedings of the 31st Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, 2, 15751580.Google Scholar
Bonawitz, E., Shafto, P., Gweon, H., Goodman, N. D., Spelke, E., & Schulz, L. (2011). The double-edged sword of pedagogy: Instruction limits spontaneous exploration and discovery. Cognition, 120(3), 322330. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2010.10.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boyd, R. T., & Richerson, P. J. (1985). Culture and the evolutionary process. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Boyette, A. H. (2018). Play in foraging societies. The Cambridge Handbook of Play: Developmental and Disciplinary Perspectives. Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108131384.017CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boyette, A. H. (2019). Autonomy, cognitive development, and the socialisation of cooperation in foragers: Aka children's views of sharing and caring. Hunter–Gatherer Research, 3, 475500.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boyette, A. H., & Hewlett, B. S. (2017). Autonomy, equality and teaching among Aka foragers and Ngandu farmers of the Congo Basin. Human Nature, 28, 289322.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Briggs, J. L. (1979). Aspects of Inuit Value Socialization (Mercury Se). National Museums of Canada.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caldwell, C. A., Cornish, H., & Kandler, A. (2016). Identifying innovation in laboratory studies of cultural evolution: Rates of retention and measures of adaptation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 371(1690). https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2015.0193CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carr, K., Kendal, R. L., & Flynn, E. G. (2016). Eureka!: What is innovation, how does it develop, and who does it? Child Dev, 87(5), 15051519. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12549CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carruthers, P. (2002). Human creativity: Its evolution, its cognitive basis, and its connections with childhood pretense. British Journal of the Philosophy of Science, 53, 225249.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cattin, M.-I. (2010). Comparing social organizations of Magdalenian hunter–gatherers. In Zubrow, E., Audouze, F., & Enloe, J. (Eds.), The Magdalenian household unravelling domesticity (pp. 213221). State University of New York Press.Google Scholar
Chan, J., Fu, K., Schunn, C., Cagan, J., Wood, K., & Kotovsky, K. (2011). On the benefits and pitfalls of analogies for innovative design: Ideation performance based on analogical distance, commonness, and modality of examples. Journal of Mechanical Design, 133(8), 081004. https://doi.org/10.1115/1.4004396CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chappell, J., Cutting, N., Apperly, I. A., & Beck, S. R. (2013). The development of tool manufacture in humans: What helps young children make innovative tools? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 368(1630), 20120409. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2012.0409Google ScholarPubMed
Cheke, L. G., Loissel, E., & Clayton, N. S. (2012). How do children solve Aesop's fable? PLoS ONE, 7(7). https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0040574CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Christian, J., & Gardner, P. (1977). The individual in northern Dene thought and communication: A study in sharing and diversity. National Museums of Canada.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, E. (1982). The young word maker: A case study of innovation in the child's lexicon. Language Acquisition: The State of the Art, 1, 390425.Google Scholar
Collard, M., Kemery, M., & Banks, S. (2005). Causes of toolkit variation among hunter–gatherers: A test of four competing hypotheses. Canadian Journal of Archaeology / Journal Canadien d'Archéologie, 29(1), 119.Google Scholar
Collard, M., Ruttle, A., Buchanan, B., & O'Brien, M. J. (2013). Population size and cultural evolution in nonindustrial food-producing societies. PLoS ONE, 8(9), e72628. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0072628CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cooney, J. (2018). Portrait of a Palaeolithic family: Art, ornamentation, and children's relationship with their community. In Crawford, S., Hadley, D. M., & Shepherd, G. (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of the archaeology of childhood (pp. 125). Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199670697.013.17Google Scholar
Cooney Williams, J., & Janik, L. (2018). Community art: Communities of practice, situated learning, adults and children as creators of cave art in Upper Palaeolithic France and northern Spain. Open Archaeology, 4(1), 217238. https://doi.org/10.1515/opar-2018-0014Google Scholar
Crittenden, A. N. (2016a). Children's foraging and play among the Hadza: The evolutionary significance of ‘work play’. In Meehan, C. L. & Crittenden, A. N. (Eds.), Childhood: Origins, evolution and implications (pp. 155170). University of New Mexico Press.Google Scholar
Crittenden, A. N. (2016b). To share or not to share? Social processes of learning to share food among hadza hunter–gatherer children. In Terashima, H. & Hewlett, B. S. (Eds.), Social learning and innovation in contemporary hunter–gatherers: Evolutionary and ethnographic perspectives (pp. 6170). Springer Japan. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-4-431-55997-9_5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crittenden, A. N., Conklin-Brittain, N. L., Zes, D. A., Schoeninger, M. J., & Marlowe, F. W. (2013). Juvenile foraging among the Hadza: Implications for human life history. Evolution and Human Behavior, 34(4), 299304.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crittenden, A. N., & Zes, D. A. (2015). Food sharing among hadza hunter–gatherer children. PLOS ONE, 10(7), e0131996. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0131996CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cunnar, G. E. (2015). Discovering latent children in the archaeological record of the Great Basin. Childhood in the Past, 8(2), 133148. https://doi.org/10.1179/1758571615Z.00000000035CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cutting, N., Apperly, I. A., & Beck, S. R. (2011). Why do children lack the flexibility to innovate tools? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 109(4), 497511. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.JECP.2011.02.012CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cutting, N., Apperly, I. A., Chappell, J., & Beck, S. R. (2014). The puzzling difficulty of tool innovation: Why can't children piece their knowledge together? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 125(1), 110117. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2013.11.010CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Damon, W. (1984). Peer education: The untapped potential. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 5(4), 331343. https://doi.org/10.1016/0193-3973(84)90006-6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davis, H. E., & Cashdan, E. (2019). Spatial cognition, navigation, and mobility among children in a forager–horticulturalist population, the Tsimané of Bolivia. Cognitive Development, 52, 100800. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2019.100800CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davis, H. E., & Cashdan, E. (2020). You don't have to know where your kids are, just where they aren't: Exploring free-range parenting in the Bolivian Amazon. In Ashdown, B. K. & Faherty, A. N. (Eds.), Parents and caregivers across cultures (pp. 5974). Springer International Publishing. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-35590-6_5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dawe, B. (1997). Tiny arrowheads: Toys in the toolkit. Plains Anthropologist, 42(161), 303318.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deák, G. O., & Wiseheart, M. (2015). Cognitive flexibility in young children: General or task-specific capacity? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 138, 3153. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2015.04.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Defeyter, M. A., & German, T. P. (2003). Acquiring an understanding of design: Evidence from children's insight problem solving. Cognition, 89(2), 133155. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(03)00098-2CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Demps, K., Zorondo-Rodríguez, F., García, C., & Reyes-García, V. (2012). Social learning across the life cycle: Cultural knowledge acquisition for honey collection among the Jenu Kuruba, India. Evolution and Human Behavior, 33(5), 460470. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.EVOLHUMBEHAV.2011.12.008CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Derex, M., Beugin, M.-P., Godelle, B., & Raymond, M. (2013). Experimental evidence for the influence of group size on cultural complexity. Nature, 503(7476), 389391. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature12774CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
d'Errico, F., & Banks, W. E. (2015). The archaeology of teaching: A conceptual framework. Cambridge Archaeological Journal, 25(4), 859866. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0959774315000384CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dira, S. J., & Hewlett, B. S. (2016). Learning to spear hunt among Ethiopian Chabu adolescent hunter–gatherers. In Social learning and innovation in contemporary hunter–gatherers (pp. 7181). Springer Japan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Doebel, S., & Zelazo, P. D. (2015). A meta-analysis of the dimensional change card sort: Implications for developmental theories and the measurement of executive function in children. Development Reviews, 38, 241268. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2015.09.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Donahue, R. E., & Fischer, A. (2015). A Late Glacial family at Trollesgave, Denmark. Journal of Archaeological Science, 54(C), 313324. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jas.2014.12.018CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Draper, P. (1975). Cultural pressure on sex differences. American Ethnologist, 2(4), 602616.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dyble, M., Salali, G. D., Chaudhary, N., Page, A., Smith, D., Thompson, J., …, Migliano, A. B. (2015). Sex equality can explain the unique social structure of hunter–gatherer bands. Science, 348(6236), 796798. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aaa5139CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eigeland, L. (2011). No man is an island: Transmission of lithic knowledge in flint-scarce regions. Lithic Technology, 36(2), 127140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Endicott, K. M. (2011). Cooperative autonomy: Social solidarity among the Batek of Malaysia. In Gibson, T. & Sillander, K. (Eds.), Anarchic solidarity: Autonomy, equality, and fellowship in Southeast Asia (pp. 6287). Yale University Council on Southeast Asia Studies.Google Scholar
Endicott, K. M., & Endicott, K. L. (2014). Batek childrearing and morality. In McKenna, J. J., Gray, D., Narvaex, Peter, Valentino, K., & Fuentes, A. (Eds.), Ancestral landscapes in human evolution: Culture, childrearing and social wellbeing (pp. 108125). Oxford Scholarship Online.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eren, M. I., Lycett, S., Patten, R. J., Buchanan, B., Pargeter, J., & O'Brien, M. J. (2016). Test, model, and method validation: The role of experimental stone artifact replication in hypothesis-driven archaeology. Ethnoarchaeology, 8(2), 103136. https://doi.org/10.1080/19442890.2016.1213972CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Erikson, E. H. (1939). Observations on Sioux education. The Journal of Psychology, 7, 101156.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fagen, R. M. (1981). Animal play behavior. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Finlayson, B., & Warren, G. (Eds.). (2010). Changing natures: Hunter–gatherers, first farmers and the modern world. Duckworth.Google Scholar
Fischer, A. (1990a). A late Palaeolithic ‘school’ of flintknapping at Trollesgave, Denmark: Results from refitting. Acta Archaeological, 60, 3349.Google Scholar
Fischer, A. (1990b). On being a pupil of a flintknapper of 11,000 years ago. A preliminary analysis of settlement organization and flint technology based on conjoined flint artefacts from the Trollesgave site. In Cziesla, E., Eickhoff, S., Arts, N., & Winter, D. (Eds.), Proceedings of the International Symposium on refitting stone artefacts, Monrepos (pp. 447484).Google Scholar
Flannery, R. (1953). The Gros Ventres of Montana: Part 1, social life. Catholic University of America Press.Google Scholar
Fogarty, L., & Creanza, N. (2017). The niche construction of cultural complexity: Interactions between innovations, population size and the environment. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 372(1735), 20160428. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2016.0428CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fogarty, L., Creanza, N., & Feldman, M. W. (2015). Cultural evolutionary perspectives on creativity and human innovation. Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 30(12), 736754. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tree.2015.10.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ford, P. R., Ward, P., Hodges, N. J., & Williams, A. M. (2009). The role of deliberate practice and play in career progression in sport: The early engagement hypothesis. High Ability Studies, 20(1), 6575. https://doi.org/10.1080/13598130902860721CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Forman, E. (1989). The role of peer interaction in the social construction of mathematical knowledge. International Journal of Educational Research, 13(1), 5570. https://doi.org/10.1016/0883-0355(89)90016-5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fouts, H. N., Bader, L. R., & Neitzel, C. L. (2016). Work-themed play among young children in foraging and farming communities in Central Africa. Behaviour, 153, 663691. https://doi.org/10.1163/1568539X-00003362CrossRefGoogle Scholar
French, J. C. (2019). The use of ethnographic data in Neanderthal archaeological research. Hunter Gatherer Research, 4(1), 2549. https://doi.org/10.3828/hgr.2018.3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frison, G. (1970). The Kobold Site, 24BH406: A post-althithermal record of buffalo-jumping for northwestern Plains. Plains Anthropologist, 15(47), 135.Google Scholar
Froehle, A. W., Wells, G. K., Pollom, T. R., Mabulla, A. Z. P., Lew-Levy, S., & Crittenden, A. N. (2019). Physical activity and time budgets of Hadza forager children: Implications for self-provisioning and the ontogeny of the sexual division of labor. American Journal of Human Biology, 31(1), e23209. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajhb.23209CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gallois, S., Duda, R., Hewlett, B. S., & Reyes-García, V. (2015). Children's daily activities and knowledge acquisition: A case study among the Baka from Southeastern Cameroon. Journal of Ethnobiology and Ethnomedicine, 11, 8699. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13002-015-0072-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gallois, S., Duda, R., & Reyes-Garcia, V. (2017). Local ecological knowledge among Baka children: A case of ‘children's culture’? Journal of Ethnobiology, 37(1), 6080.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gallois, S., Lubbers, M. J., Hewlett, B., & Reyes-García, V. (2018). Social networks and knowledge transmission strategies among Baka Children, Southeastern Cameroon. Human Nature, 29(4), 442463. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-018-9328-0CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gardner, P. M. (1966). Symmetric respect and memorate knowledge: The structure and ecology of individualistic culture. Southwestern Journal of Anthropology, 22111184(4), 389415.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gardner, P. M. (1991). Foragers’ pursuit of individual autonomy. Current Anthropology, 32(5), 543572.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gardner, P. M. (2000). Respect and nonviolence among recently sedentary Paliyan foragers. Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute, 6, 215236. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9655.00013CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glowacki, L., & Molleman, L. (2017). Subsistence styles shape human social learning strategies. Nature Human Behaviour, 1. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-017-0098CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gönül, G., Hohenberger, A., Corballis, M., & Henderson, A. M. E. (2019). Joint and individual tool making in preschoolers: From social to cognitive processes. Social Development, 28(4), 10371053. https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12373CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gönül, G., Takmaz, E. K., Hohenberger, A., & Corballis, M. (2018). The cognitive ontogeny of tool making in children: The role of inhibition and hierarchical structuring. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 173, 222238. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2018.03.017CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gopnik, A., Griffiths, T. L., & Lucas, C. G. (2015). When younger learners can be better (or at least more open-minded) than older ones. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 24(2), 8792. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721414556653CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gosselain, O. P. (2016). To hell with ethnoarchaeology! Archaeological Dialogues, 23(2), 215228. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1380203816000234CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gosso, Y., Morais, M. d. L. S., & Otta, E. (2007). Pretend play of Brazilian children: A window into different cultural worlds. Journal of Cross-cultural Psychology, 38(5), 539558.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greenfield, P. M., Maynard, A. E., & Childs, C. P. (2000). History, culture, learning, and development. Cross-cultural Research, 34(4), 351374. https://doi.org/10.1177/106939710003400404CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Griffin, A. S., & Guez, D. (2014). Innovation and problem solving: A review of common mechanisms. Behavioural Processes, 109(PB), 121134. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2014.08.027CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grimm, L. (2000). Apprentice flintknapping: Relating material culture and social practice in the Upper Palaeolithic. In Derevenski, J. S. (Eds.), Children and material culture (pp. 5371). Routledge.Google Scholar
Groenen, M. (1988). Les représentations de mains négatives dans les grottes de Gargas et de Tibiran (Hautes-Pyrénées): Approche méthodologique. Bulletin de La Société Royale Belge DAnthropogie et de Préhistoire, 99, 81113.Google Scholar
Guenther, M. (2007). Current issues and future directions in hunter–gatherer studies. Anthropos, 102, 371388.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Guthrie, R. D. (2005). The nature of Paleolithic art. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Hagen, R., Ploeg, J. V. D., & Minter, T. (2016). How do hunter–gatherers learn?: The transmission of indigenous knowledge among the Agta of the Philippines. Hunter Gatherer Research, 2(4), 389413. https://doi.org/10.3828/hgr.2016.27CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hallam, S. J. (1971). Roof markings in the ‘Orchestra Shell' Cave, Wanneroo, near Perth, Western Australia. Mankind, 8, 90103.Google Scholar
Hammond, G., & Hammond, N. (1981). Child's play: A distorting factor in archaeological distribution. American Antiquity, 46(3), 634636. https://doi.org/10.2307/280608CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hanus, D., Mendes, N., Tennie, C., & Call, J. (2011). Comparing the performances of apes (Gorilla gorilla, Pan troglodytes, Pongo pygmaeus) and human children (Homo sapiens) in the floating peanut task. PLoS One, 6(6), e19555e19555. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0019555CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hardenberg, M. (2010). In search of Thule children: Construction of playing houses as a means of socializing children. Geografisk Tidsskrift – Danish Journal of Geography, 110(2), 201214. https://doi.org/10.1080/00167223.2010.10669507CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harris, S. (1980). Culture and learning: Tradition and education in northeast Arnhem Land. Australian Institute of Aboriginal Studies.Google Scholar
Hawkes, K., O'Connell, J. F., & Blurton Jones, N. G. (1995). Hadza children's foraging: Juvenile dependency, social arrangements, and mobility among hunter–gatherers. Current Anthropology, 36(4), 688700.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henrich, J. (2004). Demography and cultural evolution: How adaptive cultural processes can produce maladaptive losses – the Tasmanian case. American Antiquity, 69(2), 197214. https://doi.org/10.2307/4128416CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henrich, J., & Gil-White, F. J. (2001). The evolution of prestige. Evolution and Human Behavior, 22(3), 165196.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). The weirdest people in the world? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33(2–3), 6183. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X0999152XCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henrich, J., & McElreath, R. (2003). The evolution of cultural evolution. Evolutionary Anthropology, 12(3), 123135. https://doi.org/10.1002/evan.10110CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hewlett, B. L. (2013). ‘Ekeloko’ the spirit to create: Innovation and social learning among Aka adolescents of the central African rainforest. In Akazawa, T., Nishiaki, Y., & Aoki, K. (Eds.), Dynamics of learning in Neanderthals and modern humans Volume 1 (pp. 187195). Springer Japan. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-4-431-54511-8_11CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hewlett, B. L. (2016). Innovation, processes of social learning, and modes of cultural transmission among the Chabu adolescent forager-farmers of Ethiopia. In Terashima, H. & Hewlett, B. S. (Eds.), Social learning and innovation in contemporary hunter–gatherers (pp. 203216). Springer Japan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hewlett, B. L. (in press). Social learning and innovation in adolescence: A comparative study of Aka and Chabu hunter–gatherers of Central and Eastern Africa. Human Nature.Google Scholar
Hewlett, B. S. (1992). The parent–child relationship and social emotional development among Aka Pygmies. In Roopnarine, J. L. & Carter, D. B. (Eds.), Parent–child relations in diverse cultures (pp. 223243). Ablex.Google Scholar
Hewlett, B. S., & Cavalli-Sforza, L. L. L. L. (1986). Cultural transmission among Aka pygmies. American Anthropologist, 88(4), 922934. https://doi.org/10.1525/aa.1986.88.4.02a00100Google Scholar
Hewlett, B. S., Fouts, H. N., Boyette, A. H., & Hewlett, B. S. B. L. (2011). Social learning among Congo Basin hunter–gatherers. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B – Biological Sciences: Biological Sciences, 366(1567), 11681178. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2010.0373CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hewlett, B. L., & Hewlett, B. S. (2012). Hunter–gatherer adolescence. In Hewlett, B. L. (Ed.), Adolescent identity: Evolutionary, cultural and developmental perspectives (pp. 73101). Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hewlett, B. S., Lamb, M. E., Leyendecker, B., & Scholmerich, A. (2000). Internal working models, trust, and sharing among foragers. Current Anthropology, 41(2), 287297.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hewlett, B. S., & Roulette, C. J. (2016). Teaching in hunter–gatherer infancy. Royal Society Open Science, 3(150403), 114.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Higham, T., Douka, K., Wood, R., Ramsey, C. B., Brock, F., Basell, L., …, Jacobi, R. (2014). The timing and spatiotemporal patterning of Neanderthal disappearance. Nature News, 512(7514), 306309. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature13621CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hovers, E., Malinksy-Buler, A., Goder-Goldberger, M., & Keshtain, R. (2011). Capturing a moment: Identifying short-lived activity locations in Amud Cave, Israel. In Le Tensorer, J.-M., Jagher, R., & Otte, M. (Eds.), The Lower and Middle Palaeolithic in the Middle East and neighbouring regions (pp. 110114). University of Liege Press.Google Scholar
Hussain, S. T., & Will, M. (in press). Materiality, agency and evolution of lithic technology: An integrated perspective for Palaeolithic archaeology. Journal of Archaeological Method and Theory.Google Scholar
Ilyatjari, N. (1991). Traditional aboriginal learning: How I learned as a Pitjantjatjara child. Aboriginal Children at School, 19, 612.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Imamura, K. (2016). Hunting play among the San children: Imitation, learning, and play. In Terashima, H. & Hewlett, B. S. (Eds.), Social learning and innovation in contemporary hunter–gatherers: Evolutionary and ethnographic perspectives (pp. 179186). Springer Japan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Imamura, K., & Akiyama, H. (2016). How hunter–gatherers have learned to hunt: Transmission of hunting methods and techniques among the central Kalahari San. African Study Monographs, 52, 6176.Google Scholar
Ivcevic, Z. (2009). Creativity map: Toward the next generation of theories of creativity. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 3(1), 1721. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014918CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jacobi, R. (2004). The late Upper Palaeolithic lithic collection from Gough's Cave, Cheddar, Somerset and human use of the cave. Proceedings of the Prehistoric Society, 70, 192. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0079497X00001110CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johansen, L., & Stapert, D. (2008). Stone age kids and their stones. Technology and Archaeology Proceedings of the SILA Workshop, 539.Google Scholar
Jordan, P. D. (2014). Technology as human social tradition: Cultural transmission among hunter–gatherers. University of California Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karlin, C., Ploux, S., Bodu, P., & Pigeot, N. (1993). Some socio-economic aspects of the knapping process among groups of hunter–gatherers in the Paris Basin area. In Berthelet, A. & Chavaillon, J. (Eds.), The use of tools by human and non-human primates. Oxford University Press On Demand.Google Scholar
Kelly, R. L. (1983). Hunter–gatherer mobility strategies. Journal of Anthropological Research, 39(3), 277306.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kelly, R. L. (1995). The lifeways of hunter–gatherers: The foraging spectrum. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kelly, R. L., Pelton, S. R., & Robinson, E. (2019). Studying sharing from the archaeological record: Problems and potential of scale. In Lavi, N. & Friesem, D. E. (Eds.), Towards a broader view of hunter–gatherer sharing (pp. 143152). MacDonald Institute Monograph Series.Google Scholar
Kempe, M., & Mesoudi, A. (2014). An experimental demonstration of the effect of group size on cultural accumulation. Evolution and Human Behavior, 35(4), 285290. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2014.02.009CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kenyon, D., & Arnold, C. (1985). Toys as indicators of socialization in Thule culture. Proceedings of the 16th Annual Chacmool Conference status, structure and stratification current archaeological reconstructions (pp. 347–353). The Archaeological Association of the University of Calgary.Google Scholar
Kline, M. A. (2015). How to learn about teaching: An evolutionary framework for the study of teaching behavior in humans and other animals. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 38(2), 117. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X14000090CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kline, M. A. (2016). TEACH: An ethogram-based method to observe and record teaching behavior. Field Methods, 29(3), 205220. https://doi.org/10.1177/1525822X16669282CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kline, M. A., & Boyd, R. (2010). Population size predicts technological complexity in Oceania. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 277(1693), 25592564. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2010.0452CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Knecht, H. (1991). Technological innovation and design during the Early Upper Palaeolithic: A study of organic projectile technologies. PhD thesis.Google Scholar
Konner, M. J. (2005). Hunter–gatherer infancy and childhood: The!Kung and others. In Hewlett, B. S. & Lamb, M. E. (Eds.), Hunter–Gatherer Childhoods: Evolutionary, Developmental and Cultural Perspectives (pp. 1964). Transaction.Google Scholar
Konner, M. J. (2016). Hunter–gatherer infancy and childhood in the context of human evolution. In Meehan, C. L. & Crittenden, A. N. (Eds.), Childhood: Origins, evolution and implications (pp. 123154). University of New Mexico Press.Google Scholar
Kruger, A. C. (1992). The effect of peer and adult–child transactive discussions on moral reasoning. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 38(2), 191211.Google Scholar
Kruger, A. C., & Tomasello, M. (1986). Transactive discussions with peers and adults. Developmental Psychology, 22(5), 681685. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.22.5.681CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laland, K. N. (1992). A theoretical investigation of the role of social transmission in evolution. Ethology and Sociobiology, 13(2), 87113. https://doi.org/10.1016/0162-3095(92)90020-5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lancy, D. F. (2010). Learning ‘from nobody’: The limited role of teaching in folk models of children's development. Childhood in the Past, 3(July), 79106. https://doi.org/10.1179/cip.2010.3.1.79CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lancy, D. F. (2016a). Playing with knives: The socialization of self-initiated learners. Child Development, 87(3), 654665. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12498CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lancy, D. F. (2016b). Teaching: Natural or cultural? In Geary, D. C. & Berch, D. B. (Eds.), Evolutionary perspectives on education and child development (pp. 3365). Springer Switzerland. https://doi.org/10.13140/RG.2.1.4151.7928CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lancy, D. F. (2017). Homo faber juvenalis: A multidisciplinary survey of children as tool makers/users. Childhood in the Past, 10(1), 7290. https://doi.org/10.1080/17585716.2017.1316010CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Langley, M. C. (2018). Magdalenian children: Projectile points, portable art and playthings. Oxford Journal of Archaeology, 37(1), 324. https://doi.org/10.1111/ojoa.12128CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lavi, N. (2018). Developing’ relations: Rethinking the experience of aid and development interventions, a case study from the Nayaka. PhD dissertation. University of Haifa.Google Scholar
Lavi, N. (in press). The freedom to stop being free: Rethinking school education and personal autonomy among Nayaka children in South India. Hunter–Gatherer Research.Google Scholar
Lavi, N., & Friesem, D. E. (2019). Towards a broader view of hunter–gatherer sharing. McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research. https://doi.org/10.17863/CAM.47185Google Scholar
Lee, R. B., & Daly, H. (Eds.). (1999). The Cambridge encyclopedia of hunters and gatherers. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Lefebvre, L., Reader, S. M., & Sol, D. (2004). Brains, innovations and evolution in birds and primates. Brain, Behavior and Evolution, 63(4), 233246. https://doi.org/10.1159/000076784CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Legare, C. H., & Nielsen, M. (2015). Imitation and innovation: The dual engines of cultural learning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19(11), 688699. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2015.08.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lew-Levy, S., & Boyette, A. H. (2018). Evidence for the adaptive learning function of work and work-themed play among Aka Forager and Ngandu farmer children from the Congo Basin. Human Nature, 29(2), 157185. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-018-9314-6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lew-Levy, S., Boyette, A. H., Crittenden, A. N., Hewlett, B. S., & Lamb, M. E. (2019a). Gender-typed and gender-segregated play among Tanzanian Hadza and Congolese BaYaka hunter–gatherer children and adolescents. Child Development. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13306Google Scholar
Lew-Levy, S., Crittenden, A. N., Boyette, A. H., Mabulla, I. A., Hewlett, B. S., & Lamb, M. E. (2019b). Inter- and intra-cultural variation in learning-through-participation among Hadza and BaYaka forager children and adolescents from Tanzania and Congo. Journal of Psychology in Africa, 29(4), 309318. https://doi.org/10.1080/14330237.2019.1647957CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lew-Levy, S., Kissler, S. M., Boyette, A. H., Crittenden, A. N., Mabulla, I. A., & Hewlett, B. S. (2020). Who teaches children to forage? Exploring the primacy of child-to-child teaching among Hadza and BaYaka hunter–gatherers of Tanzania and Congo. Evolution and Human Behavior, 40(1), 1222. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2019.07.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lew-Levy, S., Lavi, N., Reckin, R., Cristóbal-Azkarate, J., & Ellis-Davies, K. (2018). How do hunter–gatherer children learn social and gender norms? A meta-ethnographic review. Cross-Cultural Research, 52(2), 213255. https://doi.org/10.1177/1069397117723552CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lew-Levy, S., Reckin, R., Lavi, N., Cristóbal-Azkarate, J., & Ellis-Davies, K. (2017). How do hunter–gatherer children learn subsistence skills?: A meta-ethnographic review. Human Nature, 28(4), 367394. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-017-9302-2CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lewis, J. (2002). Forest hunter–gatherers and their world: A study of Mbendjele Yaka Pygmies of Congo–Brazzaville and their secular and religious activities and representations. PhD dissertation. London School of Economics and Political Science.Google Scholar
Lewis, H. M., Vinicius, L., Strods, J., Mace, R., & Migliano, A. B. (2014). High mobility explains demand sharing and enforced cooperation in egalitarian hunter–gatherers. Nature Communications, 5, 18. https://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms6789CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lillard, A. S. (1993). Pretend play skills and the child's hteory of mind. Child Development, 64(2), 348371. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1993.tb02914.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lillehammer, G. (2010a). Introduction to socialisation: Recent research on childhood and children in the past. Arkeologisk Museum i Stavanger, September 2008, 9–19.Google Scholar
Lillehammer, G. (2010b). Archaeology of children. Complutum, 21, 1545.Google Scholar
Lombard, M. (2015). Hunting and hunting technologies as proxy for teaching and learning during the Stone Age of southern Africa. Cambridge Archaeological Journal, 114.Google Scholar
Losey, R. J., & Hull, E. (2019). Learning to use atlatls: Equipment scaling and enskilment on the Oregon Coast. Antiquity, 93(372), 15691585. https://doi.org/10.15184/aqy.2019.172CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lucas, C. G., Bridgers, S., Griffiths, T. L., & Gopnik, A. (2015). When children are better (or at least more open-minded) learners than adults: Developmental differences in learning the forms of causal relationships. Cognition, 131(2), 284299. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2013.12.010CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lye, T.-P. (1997). Knowledge, forest, and hunter–gatherer movement: The Batek of Pahang, Malaysia. PhD dissertation. University of Hawaii.Google Scholar
MacDonald, K. (2007). Cross-cultural comparison of learning in human hunting implications for life history evolution. Human Nature, 18(4), 386402. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-007-9019-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacDonald, D. H., & Hewlett, B. S. (1999). Reproductive interests and forager mobility. Current Anthropology, 40(4), 501524. https://doi.org/10.1086/200047CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mackie, M. E., Surovell, T. A., & O'Brien, M. (2015). Identifying stone alignments created by adults and children: A case study from a Dukha Reindeer herder summer camp, Khövsgöl Aimag, Mongolia. Sibirica, 14(2), 2944. https://doi.org/10.3167/sib.2015.140202CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Markman, A., Wood, K., Linsey, J., Murphy, J., & Laux, J. (2011). Supporting innovation by promoting analogical reasoning. Rotman Magazine (Issue: Thinking About Thinking II, Rotman School of Management, University of Toronto), Winter, 65–69.Google Scholar
McBrearty, S., & Brooks, A. S. (2000). The revolution that wasn't: A new interpretation of the origin of modern human behavior. Journal of Human Evolution, 39(5), 453563. https://doi.org/10.1006/jhev.2000.0435CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McElreath, R., Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (2003). Shared norms and the evolution of ethnic markers. Current Anthropology, 44(1), 122129. https://doi.org/10.1086/345689CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGuigan, N., Burdett, E., Burgess, V., Dean, L. G., Lucas, A., Vale, G., & Whiten, A. (2017). Innovation and social transmission in experimental micro-societies: Exploring the scope of cumulative culture in young children. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 372(1735), 2016042520160425. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2016.0425CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mesoudi, A., & O'Brien, M. (2008). The cultural transmission of Great Basin projectile-point technology I: An experimental simulation. American Antiquity, 328.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Milks, A. (2019). Skills shortage: A critical evaluation of the use of human participants in early spear experiments. EXARC Journal, 2, 111. https://exarc.net/ark:/88735/10426Google Scholar
Milne, S. B. (2005). Palaeo-Eskimo novice flintknapping in the eastern Canadian Arctic. Journal of Field Archaeology, 30(3), 329345.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mithen, S. J. (1988). Looking and learning: Upper Palaeolithic art and information gathering. World Archaeology, 19(3), 297327.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Montgomery, D. E., & Koeltzow, T. E. (2010). A review of the day–night task: The Stroop paradigm and interference control in young children. Developmental Review, 30(3), 308330. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2010.07.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morelli, G. A., Rogoff, B., & Angelillo, C. (2003). Cultural variation in young children's access to work or involvement in specialised child-focused activities. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 27(3), 264274. https://doi.org/10.1080/01650250244000335CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morris, B. (1982). Forest traders: A socio-economic study of the Hill Pandaram. Berg.Google Scholar
Muthukrishna, M., & Henrich, J. (2016). Innovation in the collective brain. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 371(1690). https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2015.0192Google ScholarPubMed
Muthukrishna, M., Shulman, B. W., Vasilescu, V., & Henrich, J. (2014). Sociality influences cultural complexity. Proceedings. Biological Sciences, 281(1774), 20132511. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2013.2511Google ScholarPubMed
Naveh, D. (2014). Knowing and learning among Nayaka hunter–gatherers. Eastern Anthropology, 67, 34.Google Scholar
Naveh, D. (2016). Social and epistemological dimensions of learning among Nayaka hunter–gatherers. In Social learning and innovation in contemporary hunter–gatherers (pp. 125133). Springer, Tokyo. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-4-431-55997-9_10CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Negrino, F., & Riel-Salvatore, J. (2018). From Neanderthals to anatomically modern humans in Liguria (Italy): The current state of knowledge. In Borgia, V. & Cristiani, E. (Eds.), Palaeolithic Italy (pp. 161181). Sidestone Press.Google Scholar
Neldner, K., Mushin, I., & Nielsen, M. (2017). Young children's tool innovation across culture: Affordance visibility matters. Cognition, 168, 335343. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2017.07.015CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neldner, K., Redshaw, J., Murphy, S., Tomaselli, K., Davis, J., Dixson, B., & Nielsen, M. (2019). Creation across culture: Children's tool innovation is influenced by cultural and developmental factors. Developmental Psychology. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000672CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neuwelt-Truntzer, S. (1981). Ecological influences on the physical, behavioral, and cognitive development of Pygmy children, PhD dissertation. University of Chicago.Google Scholar
Nielsen, M. (2013). Young children's imitative and innovative behaviour on the floating object task. Infant and Child Development, 22(1), 4452.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nielsen, M., Cucchiaro, J., & Mohamedally, J. (2012). When the transmission of culture is child's play. PLoS ONE, 7(3). https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0034066CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nielsen, M., Haun, D., Kärtner, J., & Legare, C. H. (2017). The persistent sampling bias in developmental psychology: A call to action. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 162, 3138. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.JECP.2017.04.017CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nielsen, M., Tomaselli, K., Mushin, I., & Whiten, A. (2014). Exploring tool innovation: A comparison of Western and Bushman children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 126, 384394. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2014.05.008CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nishiaki, Y. (2013). ‘Gifting’ as a means of cultural transmission: The archeological implications of bow-and- arrow technology in Papua New Guinea. In Nishiaki, Y. & Aoki, K. (Eds.), Dynamics of learning in Neanderthals and modern humans, volume 1: Cultural perspectives (pp. 173285). Springer Japan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nowell, A. (2015). Learning to see and seeing to learn: Children, communities of practice and Pleistocene visual cultures. Cambridge Archaeological Journal, 25(4), 889899.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nowell, A. (2016). Childhood, play and the evolution of cultural capacity in Neanderthals and modern humans. In Haidle, M. N., Conard, N. J., & Bolus, M. (Eds.), The nature of culture (pp. 8797). Springer Netherlands. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-017-7426-0_9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ohmagari, K., & Berkes, F. (1997). Transmission of Indigenous knowledge and bush skills among the Western James Bay Cree Women of Subarctic Canada. Human Ecology, 25(2), 197222. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1021922105740CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Overington, S. E., Morand-Ferron, J., Boogert, N. J., & Lefebvre, L. (2009). Technical innovations drive the relationship between innovativeness and residual brain size in birds. Animal Behaviour, 78(4), 10011010. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.06.033CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Park, R. W. (1998). Size counts: The miniature archaeology of childhood in Inuit societies. Antiquity, 72, 269281.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Park, R. W. (2006). Growing up North: Exploring the archaeology of childhood in the Thule and Dorset cultures of Arctic Canada. Archaeological Papers of American Anthropological Association, 15, 5364.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Park, R. W., & Mousseau, P. M. (2003). How small is too small? Dorset culture ‘miniature’ harpoon heads. Canadian Journal of Archaeology/ Journal Canadien d'archéologie, 27(2), 258272.Google Scholar
Pellegrini, A. D., & Gustafson, K. (2005). Boys’ and girls’ uses of objects in exploration, play and tools in early childhood. In Pellegrini, A. D. & Smith, P. K. (Eds.), The nature of play: Great apes and humans. New York.Google Scholar
Perlmutter, M., Kuo, F., Behrend, S. D., & Muller, A. (1989). Social influences on children's problem solving. Developmental Psychology, 25(5), 744754.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peterson, N. (1993). Demand sharing: Reciprocity and the pressure for generosity among foragers. American Anthropologist, 95(4), 860874. https://doi.org/10.1525/aa.1993.95.4.02a00050CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Phelps, E., & Damon, W. (1989). Problem solving with equals: Peer collaboration as a context for learning mathematics and spatial concepts. Journal of Educational Psychology, 81(4), 639646. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.81.4.639CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pigeot, N. (1990). Technical and social actors: Flintknapping specialists and apprentices at Magdalenian Etoilles. Archaeological Review from Cambridge, 9(1), 126141.Google Scholar
Politis, G. G. (1998). Arqueología de al infacia: Una perspectiva ethnoarqueológica/ Archaeology of childhood: An ethnoarchaeological perspective. Trabajos de Prehistoria, 55(2), 519.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pollom, T. R., Herlosky, K. N., Mabulla, I. A., & Crittenden, A. N. (2020). Changes in juvenile foraging behavior among the Hadza of Tanzania during early transition to a mixed-subsistence food economy. Human Nature. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-020-09364-7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pope, S. M., Fagot, J., Meguerditchian, A., Watzek, J., Lew-Levy, S., Autrey, M. M., & Hopkins, W. D. (2020). Optional-switch cognitive flexibility in primates: Chimpanzees’ (Pan troglodytes) intermediate susceptibility to cognitive set. Journal of Comparative Psychology (Washington, D.C.: 1983), 134(1), 98109. https://doi.org/10.1037/com0000194CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Post, C., Lia, E. D., DiTomaso, N., Tirpak, T. M., & Borwankar, R. (2009). Capitalizing on thought diversity for innovation. Research-technology Management, 52(6), 1425. https://doi.org/10.1080/08956308.2009.11657596CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Puri, R. K. (2013). Transmitting Penan basketry knowledge and practice. In Ellen, R., Lycett, S. J., & Johns, S. E. (Eds.), Understanding cultural transmission in anthropology: A critical synthesis (pp. 266299). Berghahn Books.Google Scholar
Putt, S. S., Woods, A. D., & Franciscus, R. G. (2014). The role of verbal instraction during experimental bifacial stone tool manufacture. Lithic Technology, 39(2), 96112. https://doi.org/10.1179/0197726114Z.00000000036CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rafetseder, E., Schwitalla, M., & Perner, J. (2013). Counterfactual reasoning: From childhood to adulthood. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 114(3), 389404. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2012.10.010CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ramsey, G., Bastian, M. L., & van Schaik, C. (2007). Animal innovation defined and operationalized. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30(4), 393407; discussion 407–432. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X07002373CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reader, S. M., Hager, Y., & Laland, K. N. (2011). The evolution of primate general and cultural intelligence. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 366(1567), 10171027. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2010.0342CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reader, S., & Laland, K. (2001). Primate innovation: Sex, age and social rank\differences. International Journal of Primatology, 22(5), 787805. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1012069500899CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reader, S. M., Morand-Ferron, J., & Flynn, E. (2016). Animal and human innovation: Novel problems and novel solutions. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 371(1690), 20150182. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2015.0182CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reckin, R., Lew-levy, S., Lavi, N., & Ellis-Davies, K. (in press). Mobility, autonomy and learning: Could the transition from egalitarian to non-egalitarian social structures start with children? In Moreau, L. (Ed.), Social inequality before farming: Multidisciplinary approaches to the investigation of egalitarian and non-egalitarian social relationships in prehistoric hunter–gatherer societies. MacDonald Institute Monograph Series.Google Scholar
Rendell, L., Fogarty, L., Hoppitt, W. J. E., Morgan, T. J. H., Webster, M. M., & Laland, K. N. (2011). Cognitive culture: Theoretical and empirical insights into social learning strategies. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(2), 6876. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2010.12.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reyes-García, V., & Pyhälä, A. (Eds.). (2016). Hunter–gatherers in a changing world. Springer.Google Scholar
Richerson, P. J., & Boyd, R. (2005). Not by genes alone: How culture transformed human evolution (pp. ix, 332). University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Richland, L. E., Chan, T.-K., Morrison, R. G., & Au, T. K.-F. (2010). Young children's analogical reasoning across cultures: Similarities and differences. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 105(1–2), 146153. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2009.08.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Richland, L. E., Morrison, R. G., & Holyoak, K. J. (2006). Children's development of analogical reasoning: Insights from scene analogy problems. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 94(3), 249273. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2006.02.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Riede, F., Johannsen, N. N., Högberg, A., Nowell, A., & Lombard, M. (2018). The role of play objects and object play in human cognitive evolution and innovation. Evolutionary Anthropology, 27(1), 4659. https://doi.org/10.1002/evan.21555CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rogers, E. M. (1983). Diffusion of innovations (3rd ed.). Free Press.Google Scholar
Rogoff, B., Matusov, E., & White, C. (1996). Models of teaching and learning: Participation in a community of learners. In Olson, David, & Torrance, Nancy (Eds.), The handbook of education and human development (pp. 388414). Blackwell Publishers Ltd. DOI:10.1111/b.9780631211860.1998.xGoogle Scholar
Rogoff, B., Paradise, R., Arauz, R. M., Correa-Chávez, M., & Angelillo, C. (2003). Firsthand learning through intent participation. Annual Review of Psychology, 54(1), 175203. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.54.101601.145118CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Romano, M., Citton, P., Salvador, I., Arobba, D., Rellini, I., Firpo, M., Negrino, F.,… Avan, . (2019). A multidisciplinary approach to a unique palaeolithic human ichnological record from Italy (Basura Cave). Elifesciences.Org, 131. https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.45204.001Google Scholar
Rosendahl, G., Beinhauer, K.-W., Löscher, M., Kreipl, K., Walter, R., & Rosendahl, W. (2006). Le plus vieil arc du monde? Une pièce intéressante en provenance de Mannheim, Allemagne. L'Anthropologie, 110(3), 371382. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anthro.2006.06.008CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roveland, B. (2000). Footprints in the clay: Upper Palaeolithic children in ritual and secular contexts. In Children and material culture (pp. 2938). Routledge.Google Scholar
Salali, G. D., Chaudhary, N., Bouer, J., Thompson, J., Vinicius, L., & Migliano, A. B. (2019). Development of social learning and play in BaYaka hunter–gatherers of Congo. Scientific Reports, 9(1). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-47515-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salali, G. D., Chaudhary, N., Thompson, J., Grace, O. M., van der Burgt, X. M., Dyble, M., … Migliano, A. B. (2016). Knowledge-sharing networks in hunter–gatherers and the evolution of cumulative culture. Current Biology, 26(18), 25162521. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2016.07.015CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scalise Sugiyama, M. (2011). The forager oral tradition and the evolution of prolonged juvenility. Frontiers in Psychology, 2. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00133CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scalise Sugiyama, M. (2017). Oral storytelling as evidence of pedagogy in forager societies. Frontiers in Psychology, 8. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00471CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schweitzer, P., Biesele, M., & Hitchcock, R. (Eds.). (2000). Hunter–gatherers in the modern world: Conflict, resistance, and self-determination. Berghahn Books.Google Scholar
Sharpe, K., & Van Gelder, L. (2006). Evidence for cave marking by Palaeolithic children. Antiquity, 80(310), 937947.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shea, J., & Sisk, M. (2010). Complex projectile technology and Homo sapiens dispersal into western Eurasia. PaleoAnthropology, 2010, 100122.Google Scholar
Shennan, S. (2001). Demography and cultural innovation: A model and its implications for the emergence of modern human culture. Cambridge Archaeological Journal, 11(1), 516.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sheridan, K. M., Konopasky, A. W., Kirkwood, S., & Defeyter, M. A. (2016). The effects of environment and ownership on children's innovation of tools and tool material selection. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 371(1690). https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2015.0191CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shostak, M. (1976). A!Kung Woman's Memories of Childhood. In Lee, R. B. & DeVore, I. (Eds.), Kalahari hunter–gatherers: Studies of the!Kung San and their neighbors (pp. 246278). Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Shostak, M. (1981). Nisa, the life and words of a!Kung woman. Vintage Books.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simonet, A. (2009). L'atelier de taille gravettien de Tercis (Landes). In Recherches sur les armatures de projectiles du Paléolithique supérieur au Néolithique (pp. 192219). Palethnologie.Google Scholar
Simonet, A. (2012). Des apprentis gravettiens ont-ils confectionné des armatures lithiques à Tercis (Landes, France)? Paléo. Revue d'archéologie Préhistorique, 23, 249276.Google Scholar
Smaldino, P. E. (2014). The cultural evolution of emergent group-level traits. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 37(3), 243254. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X13001544CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, P. K. (1982). Does play matter? Functional and evolutionary aspects of animal and human play. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 5(01), 139184.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, P. K., & Dutton, S. (1979). Play and training in direct and innovative problem solving. Child Development, 50(3), 830836. JSTOR. https://doi.org/10.2307/1128950CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sonoda, K. (2016a). Constructing social learning interactions among Baka hunter–gatherers. In Terashima, H. & Hewlett, B. S. (Eds.), Social learning and innovation in contemporary hunter–gatherers: Evolutionary and ethnographic perspectives (pp. 113124). Springer Japan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sonoda, K. (2016b). ‘Give me the meat, the child said’: Cultural practice among the children of the Baka Hunter–Gatherers. Hunter Gatherer Research, 2(1), 3962. https://doi.org/10.3828/hgr.2016.3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spikins, P., Hitchens, G., Needham, A., & Rutherford, H. (2014). The cradle of thought: Growth, learning, play and attachment in Neanderthal children. Oxford Journal of Archaeology, 33(3), 111134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sternke, F. (2011). Stuck between a rock and hard place: Skill transmission and differential raw material use in Mesolithic Ireland. Lithic Technology, 36(2), 221235.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sternke, F., & Sørensen, M. (2009). The identification of children's flintknapping products in Mesolithic Scandinavia. Seventh International Conference on the Mesolithic in Europe, Belfast, 720726.Google Scholar
Sylva, K., Bruner, J. S., & Genova, P. (1976). The role of play in the problem-solving behavior of children 3–5 years old. In Bruner, J. S., Jolly, A., & Sylva, K. (Eds.), Play: Its role in development and evolution (pp. 244261). Basic Books.Google Scholar
Takakura, J. (2013). Using lithic refitting to investigate the skill learning process: Lessons from Upper Paleolithic assemblages at the Shirataki sites in Hokkaido, northern Japan. In Dynamics of learning in Neanderthals and modern humans, Volume 1 (pp. 151171). Springer, Tokyo. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-4-431-54511-8_9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tehrani, J. J., & Riede, F. (2008). Towards an archaeology of pedagogy: Learning, teaching and the generation of material culture traditions. World Archaeology, 40(3), 316331. https://doi.org/10.1080/00438240802261267CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thompson, T. K. (2003). Growing up Kaytetye: Stories. Jukurrpa Books.Google Scholar
Tijus, C., Brézillon, P., Poitrenaud, S., & Léger, L. (2009). Counterfactual based innovation, a Galois lattice approach. 2009 IEEE-RIVF International Conference on Computing and Communication Technologies, Da Nang, 2009, pp. 14, doi: 10.1109/RIVF.2009.5174633.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M., Kruger, A. C., & Ratner, H. H. (1993). Cultural learning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16(03), 495552. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X0003123XCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tonkinson, R. (1978). The Mardudjara aborigines: Living the dream in Australia's desert. Holt, Rinehart and Winston.Google Scholar
Tostevin, G. B. (2019). The sharing of lithic technological knowledge. In Lavi, N. & Friesem, D. E. (Eds.), Towards a broader view of hunter–gatherer sharing (pp. 195211). MacDonald Institute Monograph Series.Google Scholar
Trinkaus, E., & Buzhilova, A. P. (2018). Diversity and differential disposal of the dead at Sunghir. Antiquity, 92(361), 721. https://doi.org/10.15184/aqy.2017.223CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tucker, B., & Young, A. G. (2005). Growing up Mikea: Children's time allocation and tuber foraging in southwestern Madagascar. In Hewlett, B. S. & Lamb, M. E. (Eds.), Hunter–Gatherer childhoods: Evolutionary, developmental and cultural perspectives (pp. 147171). Transaction.Google Scholar
Turnbull, C. M. (1978). The politics of non-aggression. In Montagu, A. (Ed.), Learning non-aggression: The experience of non-literate societies (pp. 161211). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Van Gelder, L. (2015a). Counting the children: The role of children in the production of finger flutings in four Upper Palaeolithic caves. Oxford Journal of Archaeology, 34(2), 119138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Gelder, L. (2015b). The role of children in the creation of finger flutings in Koonalda Cave, South Australia. Childhood in the Past, 8(2), 149160. https://doi.org/10.1179/1758571615Z.00000000036CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vanstone, J. W. (1965). The changing culture of the snowdrift Chipewyan. National Museums of Canada.Google Scholar
Voelcker-Rehage, C. (2008). Motor-skill learning in older adults – A review of studies on age-related differences. European Review of Aging and Physical Activity, 5(1), 516. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11556-008-0030-9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Voelcker-Rehage, C., & Willimczik, K. (2006). Motor plasticity in a juggling task in older adults – A developmental study. Age and Ageing, 35(4), 422427. https://doi.org/10.1093/ageing/afl025CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wallace, E., & Hoebel, E. A. (1952). The Comanches: Lords of the South Plains. University of Oklahoma Press.Google Scholar
Walsh, M. J., Riede, F., & O'Neill, S. (2019). Cultural transmission and innovation in archaeology. In Prentiss, A. M. (Ed.), Handbook of evolutionary research in archaeology (pp. 4970). Springer International. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-11117-5_3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weir, A. A. S., Chappell, J., & Kacelnik, A. (2002). Shaping of hooks in New Caledonian crows. Science, 297(5583), 981981. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1073433CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weissner, P. (2014). Embers of society: Firelight talk among the Ju/’hoansi Bushmen. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 111(39), 14027–35. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1404212111CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whalley, C. L., Cutting, N., & Beck, S. R. (2017). The effect of prior experience on children's tool innovation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 161, 8194. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2017.03.009CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whittaker, J. C., & Kamp, K. A. (2006). Primitive weapons and modern sport: Atlatl capabilities, learning, gender, and age. Plains Anthropologist, 51(198), 213221. https://doi.org/10.1179/pan.2006.016CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whittaker, J. C., Pettigrew, D. B., & Grohsmeyer, R. J. (2017). Atlatl dart velocity: Accurate measurements and implications for Paleoindian and Archaic archaeology. PaleoAmerica, 3(2), 161181.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Woodburn, J. (1982). Egalitarian societies. Man, 17(3), 431451.CrossRefGoogle Scholar