Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-fscjk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-25T07:25:51.635Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Active mobility and mental health: A scoping review towards a healthier world

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  21 November 2023

Luana Scrivano*
Affiliation:
Department of Sciences for the Quality of Life, Alma Mater Studiorum, University of Bologna, Bologna, Italy
Alessia Tessari
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology “Renzo Canestrari”, Alma Mater Studiorum, University of Bologna, Bologna, Italy
Samuele M. Marcora
Affiliation:
Department of Sciences for the Quality of Life, Alma Mater Studiorum, University of Bologna, Bologna, Italy
David N. Manners
Affiliation:
Department of Sciences for the Quality of Life, Alma Mater Studiorum, University of Bologna, Bologna, Italy
*
Corresponding author: Luana Scrivano; Email: luana.scrivano2@unibo.it
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Research has proven that engaging in active mobility (AM), namely walking and cycling for transportation, significantly enhances physical activity levels, leading to better physical health. It is still unclear whether AM could also offer any mental health benefits. This scoping review aims to provide a comprehensive understanding of the current knowledge on the relationship between AM and mental health, given its crucial role in public health. The authors searched online databases to isolate primary studies written in English involving an adult sample (16 or over). AM was the exposure factor. Many mental health elements were included as outcomes (depression, anxiety, self-esteem, self-efficacy, stress, psychological and subjective well-being, resilience, loneliness and social support, quality of life, mood, life satisfaction and sleep). The results were organised in a narrative summary per each outcome selected, graphical syntheses and an overview of gaps to be further examined. The authors identified a total of 55 papers as relevant. The results show inconsistency in study designs, definition and operationalisation of the variables, approach and methodologies used. A cross-sectional design was the dominant choice, primarily examining data from national public health surveys. Nonetheless, there has been improvement in outcomes of interests, initially mainly the quality of life and affect. Lately, authors have focused on a broader range of mental health-related factors (such as travel satisfaction). The experimental studies showed promising mental health improvements in those who used active modes more than those who used motorised vehicles. It creates a rationale for further research towards implementing a unified theoretical and methodological framework to study the link between AM and mental health. The ultimate goal is to generate solid conclusions that could support building societies and cities through public health promotion and sustainable strategies, like walking and cycling as a means of transport.

Type
Overview Review
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

Impact statement

Walking and cycling are effective modes of transportation that can help achieve various public, medical and societal goals. Scientific evidence has shown that active transportation reduces air pollution, helps increase physical activity and improves physical health. However, mental health has received comparatively less attention in the literature. The global spread of mental health conditions, including depression and anxiety, is a significant challenge for individuals and societies. Moreover, factors like good quality of life, positive self-esteem, sleep quality and social connections contribute to overall well-being. This review provides an extensive overview of the link between active mobility (AM) and mental health outcomes, allowing us to identify findings and research gaps. However, reaching clear-cut conclusions on the impact of AM on mental health is challenging due to the varied terminologies and methodologies used in relevant studies. While promising, this review’s results emphasise the importance of a fair and systematic approach to understanding active travel’s benefits on mental health. A consistent conceptual and methodological framework is essential to support public health policy changes and encourage healthier and more sustainable lifestyles.

Introduction

The last few decades have seen a rising interest in active transport modes, namely walking and cycling, among academic researchers and public bodies charged with forming health and transport policies. In studying physical activity as a means of transport, researchers have used numerous synonyms for active mobility (AM), such as Active Transport, Transportation, Active Commute and Commuting, Active Travel and Sustainable Transport. The variety of definitions mirrors the inconsistency and the diversity of the approaches used to study this concept. Nonetheless, these descriptions have in common the idea of physical activity transportation as a way “to get from one place to another” to differentiate AM from leisure-time physical activity, exercise and sport.

The positive consequences of physical activity are well-established in the literature. Regular physical activity is known to have long-term benefits on the population’s health. As a non-pharmacology therapy, physical activity does not show drug side effects and is associated with better physical performance and cardiovascular improvements (Marques et al., Reference Marques, Peralta, Henriques-Neto, Frasquilho, Gouveira and Gomez-Baya2020). Furthermore, physical activity has been reported as a protective factor against mental illness of various natures (Marques et al., Reference Marques, Peralta, Henriques-Neto, Frasquilho, Gouveira and Gomez-Baya2020).

In particular, public health experts have promoted walking and cycling as they contribute to total physical activity levels across all age groups, improving populations’ health and well-being (Kelly et al., Reference Kelly, Williamson, Niven, Hunter, Mutrie and Richards2018). In addition, walking and cycling as active travel modes are of interest from urban and environmental perspectives because of their potential impact on reducing traffic congestion and air pollution (Alattar et al., Reference Alattar, Cottrill and Beecroft2021). Purposeful travel, especially to and from work or educational sites, is a substantial part of people’s daily routines. AM could often substitute for journeys undertaken by motorised transport, but policies and interventions are needed to shift the transport paradigm away from using motor vehicles. There has been a considerable amount of research on health promotion, focussed on specific outcomes, such as the relationship between physical activity and overall health and AM and physical health. However, no clear evidence exists of the relationship between AM and Mental Health outcomes, even though mental health problems contribute to the global health burden (Kelly et al., Reference Kelly, Williamson, Niven, Hunter, Mutrie and Richards2018).

Most published evidence refers to mental health or well-being as umbrella terms, operationalised and measured using instruments that assess elements of behaviour connected to psychological health, where the items investigate mental health components as a total score. However, mental health is a complex concept characterised by a “conglomerate of indicators”, including “health determinants, and the severity of symptoms” (Tannenbaum et al., Reference Tannenbaum, Lexchin, Tamblyn and Romans2009). In the early 2000s, Keyes posited that it was best explained as a combination of emotional, psychological and social well-being (Keyes, Reference Keyes2007). “Well-being” is linked more to psychological than physical health (Galderisi et al., Reference Galderisi, Heinz, Kastrup, Beezhold and Sartorius2015). Quality of life is a fundamental aspect of well-being regarding physical and psychological components (De Geus et al., Reference De Geus, Van Hoof, Aerts and Meeusen2008). Consequently, it is an essential element in the public health perspective; indeed, it is one of the most studied mental health-related outcomes regarding active transport.

Beyond the concepts listed above, other aspects of mental well-being that have been evaluated in the context of AM include eudaimonia (Table 1; Ryff, Reference Ryff1989), positive and negative affects, life satisfaction (Gatersleben and Uzzell, Reference Gatersleben and Uzzell2007), self-efficacy (Bandura, Reference Bandura1977; Pearlin and Schooler, Reference Pearlin and Schooler1978; Rothbaum et al., Reference Rothbaum, Weisz and Snyder1982), negative indicators such as stress (Cohen et al., Reference Cohen, Kamarck and Mermelstein1983; Anable and Gatersleben, Reference Anable and Gatersleben2005; Gottholmseder et al., Reference Gottholmseder, Nowotny, Pruckner and Theurl2009), anxiety (Katsarou et al., Reference Katsarou, Triposkiadis and Panagiotakos2013), exhaustion (Hansson et al., Reference Hansson, Mattisson, Bjork, Ostergren and Jakobsson2011), sleep quality (Scott et al., Reference Scott, Webb, Martyn-St James, Rowse and Weich2021) and depression (Kelly et al., Reference Kelly, Williamson, Niven, Hunter, Mutrie and Richards2018).

Table 1. Definitions of the included mental health outcomes

To evaluate outcomes more closely related to the specific kinds of behaviour of interest, researchers have developed a context-dependent state termed travel satisfaction or commuter satisfaction, derived from customer satisfaction research (Fornell et al., Reference Fornell, Johnson, Anderson, Cha and Bryant1996), which has been linked to more general aspects of mental health (Olsson et al., Reference Olsson, Gärling, Ettema, Friman and Fujii2013; De Vos et al., Reference De Vos, Le and Kroesen2022). Numerous factors mediate the relationship between the two levels of analysis: environmental, such as social support (Pearson, Reference Pearson1986; Panter and Jones, Reference Panter and Jones2010; Rovniak et al., Reference Rovniak, Sallis, Saelens, Frank, Marshall, Norman, Conway, Cain and Hovell2010; Van Dyck et al., Reference Van Dyck, Cardon, Deforche, Giles-Corti, Sallis, Owen and De Bourdeaudhuij2011; Paudel et al., Reference Paudel, Owen and Smith2021), and personal, including mental health “protective” factors like self-efficacy, self-esteem and resilience (Keyes, Reference Keyes2002, Reference Keyes2005, Reference Keyes2007).

Most evidence about the beneficial impact of AM on people’s mental health comes from the economic or transport studies fields, not from psychologists. A science journalist recently noted that a “morass of definitions and measurements” is a common problem in social sciences and other scientific fields, quoting Dr Jessica Flake, a quantitative psychologist from McGill University (Gupta, Reference Gupta2022). The findings of the present review demonstrate the presence of this inconsistency in the AM literature. Hence, this scoping review aims to identify evidence of the relationship between active transport modes and mental health outcomes, create a picture of what is already known, what methodologies have been used, and detect research gaps, to identify the areas where research is still required, and proven research methodologies that can evaluate and the beneficial connection between AM and overall healthier life.

The initial research questions were (1) “What methods have been used to investigate the relationship between AM and Mental health?” (2) “What evidence exists in the research literature about the relationship between AM and Mental Health in the general adult population?” (3) “What limitations are apparent in previous studies?”

Materials and methods

Considering this phenomenon has been addressed only recently and through various approaches, the review was structured as a scoping study “to address broader topics where many different study designs might be applicable” (Arksey and O’Malley, Reference Arksey and O’Malley2005).

Search strategy

The search strategy aimed to find studies in the research literature investigating the link between walking and cycling as a means of transport and mental health outcomes up to December 2022. Eligibility criteria were primary study; any study design; at least one mental health-related outcome; AM as the exposure; adult sample (18 over) with no specific health condition (e.g., stroke, cancer, postpartum, fibromyalgia and others listed in the literature); articles published in an indexed scientific journal in the English language. The strategy aimed to conduct an exhaustive search of electronic databases. EBSCOhost, PROQUEST, SCOPUS, Web of Science, and PubMed were identified as appropriate topic-related databases and last searched on 30 January 2023; the search had no time limits, any results prior to the date of search were accepted.

Each collection was searched using the following combination of terms:

active mobility OR active travel* OR active transport* OR active commut* OR cycl* OR walk*

AND

mental health OR psychological health OR brain health OR cognitive function* OR depress* OR anxiety OR quality of life OR life satisfaction OR self-esteem OR stress OR psychological well-being OR personal well-being OR subjective well-being OR resilience OR social support OR loneliness.

The search revealed that scientific evidence about AM has primarily focused on young populations and school travelling, especially regarding cognitive functioning. Therefore, the authors added NOT children, NOT adolescents, NOT school to the initially selected terms. The keywords were previously identified following the main objective of this paper, investigating the literature that has previously considered the relationship between AM and mental health. Relevant studies were screened at a title and abstract level and downloaded on Mendeley software by one of the authors (L.S.). Duplicates were automatically removed. The same author (L.S.) conducted the full-text assessment, and the inclusion of potential studies was agreed upon by consensus with the others (D.N.M., A.T., S.M.M.).

Data extraction and data management

In line with FAIR data management (FAIR Principles, n.d.) and following PRISMA guidelines (Moher et al., Reference Moher, Liberati, Tetzlaff, Altman and Group2009), the authors planned and mutually agreed upon a data extraction and management process. First, they created a chart listing relevant information as follows:

One of the authors (L.S.) completed the extraction and was verified by a second author (D.N.M.). Where disagreements arose, these were resolved by consensus. The extracted data were used to describe characteristics and findings for each outcome using narrative summaries. Then, the authors identified gaps in the literature and outlined areas that need further research. According to an Open Science framework, all data and metadata are available online within Supplementary Appendices A and B.

Study quality and risk of bias

Study quality was assessed mainly in terms of study design. In terms of the Oxford Centre for Evidence-Based Medicine classification (OCEBM Levels of Evidence Working Group, 2011), all studies included in the review were in the range of step 2 (“randomised trial” (s)), step 3 (“non-randomised controlled cohort/follow-up studies”) or step 4 (“Case-series or case–control studies”). Studies using designs at a higher step are more at risk of generating findings biased by confounding variables than those at a lower step.

The OCEBM Levels of Evidence Table was used to assign the designation of each study, without taking into account effect size or systematically assessing study quality. The consensus process used was the same as that employed in the search phase (section “Search strategy”).

Graphical syntheses

The first step in synthesising results after tabulating study characteristics was to summarise data items graphically to facilitate a qualitative comparison of all the studies and identify relevant points of similarity or difference. Next, extracted data were processed using scripts developed in the R programming language.

Word cloud

The most commonly occurring two-word expressions associated with the studies summarised were determined as follows. First, the pdf files of the studies were cleaned to remove punctuation and generic commonly occurring words. Then two-word expressions were extracted and counted, merging entries that differed only for capitalisation (Benoit et al., Reference Benoit, Watanabe, Wang, Nulty, Obeng, Müller and Matsuo2018). The 500 most commonly occurring two-word expressions were manually reviewed by two authors and sorted as being related to exposures, covariates, outcomes, or other aspects of the studies. Word clouds were created using version 0.2.2 of the Wordcloud2 package, with entries related to the frequency of occurrence, to provide an objective presentation of the themes contained within the studies.

Geographical mapping

The distribution of sampled populations was projected onto a world map, using symbols proportional to the sample size. Extracted locations were identified using geocoding based on the cities or regions named as sample locations or the geographical centre of any country from which a national sample was obtained.

Albatross plot

The present review does not aim to assess the weight of evidence but to map the claimed results so that the reader can rapidly assess the different studies. Using the Albatross plot (Harrison et al., Reference Harrison, Jones, Martin, Lewis and Higgins2017), an established method in line with this aim, four plots were constructed through the Metap package (version 1.8). Based on the p-value of the statistical test undertaken (recorded in the data extraction table) and the sample size used to provide the data, an implicit effect size can be calculated. Statistical tests were included if they related to a difference between the use of active transport modalities and other forms of transport (taking car use as the baseline comparison where possible, or public transport otherwise). However, given the various tests used in the different studies and differences in reporting the p-value, this should be interpreted only as a rough guide as to which studies reported which outcomes and whether these were small or large, beneficial or harmful.

Descriptive analytical synthesis

Data were organised using the list of mental-health-related outcomes in Table 1, congruent with the wordcloud described above. Then, we mapped the instruments and noticed that outcomes were given different names but measured with the same instrument or vice versa. Considering the variability of exposures, outcomes and methodology approach used in the included literature, the authors outlined the findings per each outcome following the narrative tradition approach of descriptive-analytical method, which consists of applying a standard analytical framework to each study and collecting specific information reported (Arksey and O’Malley, Reference Arksey and O’Malley2005), as in section “Data extraction and data management”.

Results

Search results

Figure 1 presents the flow diagram of the study selection. Database searches identified 502 records. After duplicates were excluded, each paper was assessed at a title/abstract level and, if selected, full-text level. Studies were excluded when the outcome or exposure did not align with the research questions. A total of 55 relevant studies were retained.

Figure 1. PRISMA 2020 flow diagram for new systematic reviews which included searches of databases and registers only.

Study characteristics

Study characteristics are summarised in Table 2. The majority (40) employed a cross-sectional design based mainly on data collected by public health surveys, typically including subjects who could autonomously travel using passive or active travel modes. Nine studies reported a longitudinal design; however, data were mainly analysed following a cross-sectional approach. Lastly, two studies had a quasi-experimental design in which the sample was assigned to intervention or control groups following deterministic criteria. We found only one unrandomized controlled trial and four randomised controlled trials, all investigating changes in the quality of life from 2000 to 2020. There is no evidence of an evolution in study designs; the oldest study (Mutrie et al., Reference Mutrie, Cerney, Blamey, Whitelaw, Crawford and Aitchison2000) had an experimental design (RCT), while the most recent (Scarabottolo et al., Reference Scarabottolo, Tebar, Gobbo, Ohara, Ferreira, Canhin, da and Christofaro2022) used a cross-sectional analysis. On the other hand, outcomes of interest have developed. Before 2010, studies mainly focused on outcomes like Quality of Life and Affect, while more recently, the focus has shifted to other potentially important aspects. Demographically, most study participants were women. Given our selection criteria, the study populations ranged from 15 to 98 years old. Some of the included studies specifically focused on older adult samples (60–98 years old) where AM (“outdoor mobility”) is considered essential to maintaining good quality of life levels.

Table 2. Summary of study characteristics and participants, ordered by study design and year of publication

Note: Ages presented as range when available or otherwise average where available. Design – RCT, randomised controlled trial. Participant characteristics – CtW, commuters to work. Coded mobility mode – C, cycling; E, e-biking; M, multi-mode; P, public transport; W, walking. Coded active mobility measure – D, duration; Di, distance; F, frequency; IPAQ, international physical activity questionnaire; M, mode; SPAQ, Scottish physical activity questionnaire.

Graphical mapping of studies

Firstly, word clouds of phrases related to outcomes and covariates are shown in Figure 2. They were used to validate the concepts attended to in the data summary phase and are referenced at appropriate points in the text below. Secondly, the distribution of studies is illustrated on a world map in Figure 3. Except for some Chinese and Brazilian studies, most work in this field is concentrated in Europe and the English-speaking world. Necessarily, the largest studies are cross-sectional, primarily based on data from national surveys, not purposively collected to evaluate either mental health, active transport or the relationship between these. Finally, the implicit effect sizes of statistically assessed outcomes are shown in Figure 4, dividing the literature into four quadrants, since the high number of mental health outcomes is considered. It is immediately apparent that most studies are compatible with a hypothesis of moderate benefit due to AM or no effect. Only one finding is of a large dis-benefit.

Figure 2. Word clouds summarising the most common study-specific phrases found in articles included in the review are categorised as (a) related to outcomes or (b) related to covariates. The size of the text is proportional to the relative number of occurrences of the phrase.

Figure 3. The study distribution in the review is projected onto the world map (left panel), while European studies are shown in the enlargement (right panel). The size of the symbols is related to the number of participants. The location of each symbol is centred on the centre of the city, region or country from which participants were drawn.

Figure 4. Albatross plots mapping the imputed effect size of each study for which data were available regarding differences in outcome between study participants selecting active and motorised mobility for transport needs in daily life. Curved lines join points with equal effect sizes (labelled 0.1, 0.25, 0.5). The x-axis indicates the reported p-value. Points to the right of the origin (null p-value) show a benefit for active mobility, and those to the left show a benefit for motorised transport. The y-axis shows the number of participants on a logarithmic scale. Points are positioned conservatively when only the relationship to a significance threshold is reported (i.e., p < 0.05 is treated as p = 0.05). Results are divided between four panels for easier visualisation, grouping outcomes by alphabetical order. Symbol shapes indicate study design (legend in the bottom right-hand panel). Labels indicate the first three letters of the first author’s name and the 2-digit year of publication.

Mapping of terminology

This section examines how AM as a causal factor and mental health as an outcome have been described and categorised.

AM, the exposure factor

While studies have shown that AM improves physical health (Alattar et al., Reference Alattar, Cottrill and Beecroft2021), it is not always the case for mental health. The current scoping review is part of a larger project examining the effects of AM on different aspects of well-being in the adult population. As such, it focused on studies that considered walking and cycling as a means of transport, travelling, and commuting, in short, “to get from one place to another” (Physical Activity Guidelines Advisory Committee, 2018), and not for exercise, sport or leisure purposes. As such, authors approved “AM” as an advantageous nomenclature since it communicates the action of “moving actively” without excluding any transport purpose (e.g., “active commuting” seems to specifically refer to travels from home to work/school or vice versa). Also, AM is commonly used in Europe, so it might reduce language and understanding biases in our future studies, taking place in Italy. The research literature refers to active travel primarily in terms of walking and cycling, while public transport is often investigated separately and is usually related to comparatively adverse health results (Office for National Statistics, 2014; Neumeier et al., Reference Neumeier, Loidl, Reich, Fernandez La Puente de Battre, Kissel, Templin, Schmied, Niebauer and Niederseer2020; Jacob et al., Reference Jacob, Munford, Rice and Roberts2021), but partially overlapping terms are abundant. Marttila and Nupponen (Reference Marttila and Nupponen2000) defined everyday commuting activity (ECA) as one of two Health Enhancing Physical Activity (HEPA) categories. At the same time, Mutrie et al. (Reference Mutrie, Cerney, Blamey, Whitelaw, Crawford and Aitchison2000, Reference Mutrie, Carney, Blamey, Crawford, Aitchison and Whitelaw2002) implemented two RCTs to study active commuting behaviour and trips explicitly to work or educational sites. Outside the UK, the term “active commuting” has been used worldwide, for example, in Japan (Ohta et al., Reference Ohta, Mizoue, Mishima and Ikeda2007), New Zealand (Tin Tin et al., Reference Tin Tin, Woodward, Thornley and Ameratunga2009), Sweden (Hansson et al., Reference Hansson, Mattisson, Bjork, Ostergren and Jakobsson2011), Canada (St-Louis et al., Reference St-Louis, Manaugh, Van Lierop, Student and El-Geneidy2014) and China (Liu et al., Reference Liu, Chen and Cao2021). While active commuting appears to be limited to trips whose purpose is primarily the journey to/from work (or university), authors have argued that walking and cycling have several utilitarian transportation purposes, and going to work is just one of them (Singleton, Reference Singleton2018). Terms including active travel, travelling, transport or transportation suggest that the end of the movement might be to complete other everyday tasks outside the proper “exercise” and “leisure-time” objectives, such as going to the supermarket or the post office. In the UK, the iConnect Study (Impact of Constructing Non-motorised Networks and Evaluating Changes in Travel) (Ogilvie et al., Reference Ogilvie, Griffin, Jones, Mackett, Guell, Panter, Jones, Cohn, Yang and Chapman2010, Reference Ogilvie, Bull, Powell, Cooper, Brand, Mutrie, Preston and Rutter2011) was a natural experiment that evaluated the effect of active travel, “walking and cycling as travel behaviour” on people’s health, traffic congestion, and carbon emission. Later, the USA Physical Activity Guidelines Advisory Committee (2018) reported that transportation physical activity examples include “walking or bicycling to and from work, school, transportation hubs, or a shopping centre”. Finally, Alattar et al. (Reference Alattar, Cottrill and Beecroft2021) defined active travel as “journeys that have been undertaken either entirely or partially using human-powered transportation modes such as walking, cycling or using a wheelchair”. More recently, European researchers involved in the PASTA (Physical Activity through Sustainable Transport Approaches) project defined activities, including “walking and cycling for transport solely or in combination with public transport”, as AM (Gerike et al., Reference Gerike, De Nazelle, Nieuwenhuijsen, Panis, Anaya, Avila-Palencia, Boschetti, Brand, Cole-Hunter, Dons, Eriksson, Gaupp-Berghausen, Kahlmeier, Laeremans, Mueller, Orjuela, Racioppi, Raser, Rojas-Rueda and Götschi2016). As Redding et al. (Reference Redding, Mundorf, Kobayashi, Brick, Horiuchi, Paiva and Prochaska2014) stated, walking and cycling can be classified as alternative/sustainable transportation “defined as commuting by any means other than a single occupancy vehicle (SOV)”.

Mental health, the outcome factor(s)

In the present study, the authors looked for studies that examined outcomes related to mental, cognitive, neurological and brain health potentially related to AM. Considering the iterative process entailed in a scoping review, we were open to any possible results and ready to collect various factors offered and change the research process accordingly. The wide range of possible outcome measures is evident in Figure 2 on the states and symptoms mentioned repeatedly in the studies initially identified; we selected 17 mental health-related outcomes (Table 1) and examined how these were defined and operationalised.

Most published evidence refers to mental health or well-being as umbrella terms, operationalised and measured using instruments that assess elements of behaviour connected to psychological health, where the items investigate mental health components as a total score or divided between various domains. While mental health refers to a state, “quality of life” is related to individuals’ perceptions and includes physical and psychological components (De Geus et al., Reference De Geus, Van Hoof, Aerts and Meeusen2008). Both are essential components in the public health perspective and furnish necessary outcome measures in active transport studies, although the distinctions are not always clearly defined.

Well-being was another frequently used term, and we decided to treat each component of well-being separately (eudaimonia, affect, life satisfaction), avoiding using general terms like subjective, psychological or personal well-being that could lead to further confusion. We found mainly cross-sectional designs investigating if travelling using physical activity could make people happier and improve their mood or affect. Therefore, we decided to unify these studies under one term, affect, regarding positive and negative affective aspects of well-being. Compared to affective factors, the concept of Eudaimonic Well-Being is less well-represented in the studies reviewed. Eudaimonia refers to the sense of worthiness in people’s lives, not only their mood (Office for National Statistics, 2014). Only recently have researchers considered that commuting might also impact the cognitive component of subjective well-being, life satisfaction.

While searching for this outcome, authors noted the term travel satisfaction, which was not included initially. Also called “commute well-being”, it is defined as a “multi-item measure of how one feels about the commute to work and its associated factors” (Smith, Reference Smith2017), which is a cognitive evaluation of the quality of travel and an affective evaluation of feelings during travel (from stressed to relaxed and from bored to excited) (Ettema et al., Reference Ettema, Gärling, Eriksson, Friman, Olsson and Fujii2011). In line with subjective well-being, travel satisfaction was included in the present review because it represents aspects of personal well-being specifically involved in commuting (Ettema et al., Reference Ettema, Gärling, Olsson and Friman2010) and directly correlates with life satisfaction and affect (Friman et al., Reference Friman, Gärling, Ettema and Olsson2017) and indirectly with eudaimonic well-being (Liu et al., Reference Liu, Chen and Cao2021).

Due to its favourable influence on mental and physical health (Pearson, Reference Pearson1986), social support was included among the original search terms. Meanwhile, we encountered evidence of scientific interest in active commuting and loneliness. It regards a subjective state, distinct from the objective condition of not having social contact, called “social isolation” (Masi et al., Reference Masi, Chen, Hawkley and Cacioppo2011). Negative, distressing emotions are also associated with loneliness (de Jong Gierveld and van Tilburg, Reference de Jong Gierveld and van Tilburg2016). The literature has described the crucial role of social support in promoting or discouraging physical activity. Hence, the paper included social support and loneliness as outcomes of mental health and public health interest.

Similarly, previous research indicated self-efficacy as a considerable correlate for physical activity, more as a motivator than a benefit. Nonetheless, we wanted to emphasise this concept as an essential health outcome. Therefore, according to the definition and operationalisation of this term (Bandura, Reference Bandura1977), we merged the references about “mastery, confidence, achieving goals” and other subscales of self-efficacy (e.g., physical self-efficacy) into one outcome.

While screening the selected literature, Vitality became more susceptible to the benefits of AM. Experimental results reported that improved walking scores in the intervention group also improved quality of life scores, specifically in the Vitality subscale (Mutrie et al., Reference Mutrie, Carney, Blamey, Crawford, Aitchison and Whitelaw2002; De Geus et al., Reference De Geus, Van Hoof, Aerts and Meeusen2008). A longitudinal design reached the same result (Scarabottolo et al., Reference Scarabottolo, Tebar, Gobbo, Ohara, Ferreira, Canhin, da and Christofaro2022). We noticed some literature considering this one quality-of-life domain as the outcome. Still, studies measuring “low levels of energy or vitality” used the title exhaustion, even though the instrument employed was the same (SF-36 Vitality subscale). We decided to reserve specific attention to this item.

We encountered the term self-reported health during the screening phase, also mentioned as self-perceived or self-rated health, or overall health status or physical health. It seems reasonable that active travel research on mental health focused on this outcome since it reciprocally relates to subjective well-being (Rasciute and Downward, Reference Rasciute and Downward2010; Clark et al., Reference Clark, Chatterjee, Martin and Davis2020), and it is also a “valid indicator of morbidity and mortality” (Wilson et al., Reference Wilson, Elliott, Eyles and Keller-Olaman2007). Furthermore, other authors have measured “health status” using self-report instruments like the physical component score of the SF-12 (Jacob et al., Reference Jacob, Munford, Rice and Roberts2021), given that people might interpret this term as only related to physical health (Clark et al., Reference Clark, Chatterjee, Martin and Davis2020). Consequently, self-reported health is a likely mediating factor when designing interventions (Mattisson et al., Reference Mattisson, Idris, Cromley, Håkansson, Östergren and Jakobsson2018). Accordingly, we introduced it in this review.

Three of the original search terms remain to be considered. First, although sleep patterns are crucial intermediates in the relationship between physical and mental health (Scott et al., Reference Scott, Webb, Martyn-St James, Rowse and Weich2021), we could only find two studies focusing on sleep quality and active travel. Secondly, we found no study investigating a potential relationship between AM and self-esteem despite being associated with health-related behaviours and contributing to people’s health status (Bauman, Reference Bauman2012). In line with our results, a review (Kelly et al., Reference Kelly, Williamson, Niven, Hunter, Mutrie and Richards2018) has recently concluded that the evidence about active travelling improving or maintaining good self-esteem is mixed or inconsistent. The authors found no experimental evidence, and only one (Bergland et al., Reference Bergland, Thorsen and Loland2010) of the 11 identified studies examined outdoor walking mobility specifically, while in others, the exposure factor was physical activity or exercise. Finally, we found no evidence of studies on resilience and walking or cycling as transport means. However, the review mentioned above (Kelly et al., Reference Kelly, Williamson, Niven, Hunter, Mutrie and Richards2018) claimed that it is an outcome to attention considering the emergent evidence of physical activity in healthy adults (Childs and de Wit, Reference Childs and de Wit2014). Therefore, we do not further consider results concerning self-esteem and resilience.

While we aimed to investigate the potential health benefits of active travel modes on the general healthy adult population, most studies about cognitive health involved youth samples (children, adolescents, and students under 18) or older adults with cognitive functional impairments, so we excluded studies solely concerned with this outcome. Additionally, we found only one study specifically about brain health (Bos et al., Reference Bos, Jacobs, Nawrot, de Geus, Torfs, Int Panis, Degraeuwe and Meeusen2011), which measured the increase in BDNF serum levels due to active travel moderated by air pollution or the natural environment. Because of the lack of congruence with the goal of this review, we did not include that study either.

Moderating variables

Several variables are known to moderate the relationship between AM and its potential health benefits. For example, travel time and environmental characteristics moderate this link (Office for National Statistics, 2014; Clark et al., Reference Clark, Chatterjee, Martin and Davis2020; Wang et al., Reference Wang, Yin and Shao2021). Many of the studies reviewed evaluated one or more of these factors, as evidenced by Figure 2, but limitations in study design preclude the possibility of studying all of them together.

Mapping of outcomes’ instruments and results

This section examines how the above outcomes have been operationally defined and measured. Also, we provide a verbal summary of the study results for each outcome in Table 3. Extended details about each study are also provided in Supplementary Appendix A.

Table 3. Summary of findings relating to the effect of active mobility on aspects of mental health, ordered by mental health variable, study design, and year of publication

Note: (1) only younger adults; (2) no significant relationship – healthy commuter effect. Study design – CS, cross-sectional; RCT, randomised controlled trial. Mental health variable – CES-D, Centre for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale; GHQ, general health questionnaire; HSCL, Hopkins symptoms checklist; MCS, mental component score; PANAS, positive and negative affect scale; PCS, physical component score; PSQ, perceived stress questionnaire; PSS, perceived stress scale; SF, short form; STS, satisfaction with travel scale; WEMWBS, Warwick-Edinburgh mental well-being scale; WHOQoL-BREF, World Health Organization quality of life short version. Evidence of relationship – C, cycling; RC, reverse causality; W, walking. Largest covariate – PA, physical activity.

Mental health and mental well-being

Numerous studies conducted since 2006 have explored the connection between active transportation and overall mental health. The findings are presented in Figure 4. The tools primarily used for mental health were the General Health Questionnaire (Goldberg, Reference Goldberg1972), validated to assess elements of behaviour connected to psychological health, such as sleep, depression, confidence and focus, the Mental Component Sore from the SF-12 (MCS-SF) (Ware et al., Reference Ware, Kosinski and Keller1996), and the Mental Health Inventory-5 (MHI-5) (Berwick et al., Reference Berwick, Murphy, Goldman, Ware, Barsky and Weinstein1991), which includes dimensions of anxiety, depression, loss of behavioural/emotional control, and psychological well-being. Three longitudinal studies showed a positive association between these variables (Lampinen et al., Reference Lampinen, Heikkinen, Kauppinen and Heikkinen2006; Martin et al., Reference Martin, Goryakin and Suhrcke2014; Kroesen and De Vos, Reference Kroesen and De Vos2020), while two others limited the positive association to cycling, not walking (Mytton et al., Reference Mytton, Panter and Ogilvie2016; Avila-Palencia et al., Reference Avila-Palencia, Int Panis, Dons, Gaupp-Berghausen, Raser, Götschi, Gerike, Brand, de Nazelle, Orjuela, Anaya-Boig, Stigell, Kahlmeier, Iacorossi and Nieuwenhuijsen2018). However, only two cross-sectional papers demonstrated a positive association (one only for men) (Ohta et al., Reference Ohta, Mizoue, Mishima and Ikeda2007; Mason et al., Reference Mason, Curl and Kearns2016), and four did not report any relationship (Hansson et al., Reference Hansson, Mattisson, Bjork, Ostergren and Jakobsson2011; Humphreys et al., Reference Humphreys, Goodman and Ogilvie2013; Chng et al., Reference Chng, White, Abraham and Skippon2016; Clark et al., Reference Clark, Chatterjee, Martin and Davis2020). Recently, two quasi-experimental studies using different instruments (GHQ-12 and MCS-SF12) showed a positive association between cycling as a transport (Page and Nilsson, Reference Page and Nilsson2017) or active travel modes (Jacob et al., Reference Jacob, Munford, Rice and Roberts2021) and mental health scores. Previous research has identified the “healthy commuter effect” (Hansson et al., Reference Hansson, Mattisson, Bjork, Ostergren and Jakobsson2011) as a selection bias whereby unhealthy people are less keen on commuting actively. However, one later study (Kroesen and De Vos, Reference Kroesen and De Vos2020) showed that active travel significantly impacts mental health and not vice versa, so this bias might not be as significant for the outcomes of interest here.

Quality of life

Most reviewed studies, particularly experimental designs, relate to Health-Related Quality of Life (HRQoL). On one side, this evidence seems to approach the outcomes by clearly distinguishing the purpose of physical activity (work/occupational, leisure time, transportation, sport and exercise). Conversely, Karimi and Brazier (Reference Karimi and Brazier2016) note that health, HRQoL and quality of life have been wrongly employed and confused (e.g., HRQoL tools measuring self-perceived health). In this review, we differentiate between the studies that used the Short Form Health Survey (SF-36) (Ware and Sherbourne, Reference Ware and Sherbourne1992) and all its versions to measure the quality of life. Mutrie et al. (Reference Mutrie, Cerney, Blamey, Whitelaw, Crawford and Aitchison2000) conducted the first inconclusive randomised controlled trial (RCT) and followed up with a similar study 2 years later (Mutrie et al., Reference Mutrie, Carney, Blamey, Crawford, Aitchison and Whitelaw2002). The findings confirmed a positive association, although significant only for walking (not cycling) as a means of transport. Later, the positive association was also reported by a non-randomised controlled trial using the same instrument (SF-36), but this time results were significant only for cycling and not for walking (De Geus et al., Reference De Geus, Van Hoof, Aerts and Meeusen2008). That same year, another RCT, using the Euroqol EQ-5D (The EuroQoL Group, 1990), claimed no correlation between active travel as walking (steps/day) and quality of life (Baker et al., Reference Baker, Gray, Wright, Fitzsimons, Nimmo, Lowry and Mutrie2008). More recently, an RCT (Neumeier et al., Reference Neumeier, Loidl, Reich, Fernandez La Puente de Battre, Kissel, Templin, Schmied, Niebauer and Niederseer2020) and a longitudinal study (Scarabottolo et al., Reference Scarabottolo, Tebar, Gobbo, Ohara, Ferreira, Canhin, da and Christofaro2022) have found a positive relationship between the SF-36 scores and active travel modes. A cross-sectional study (85) recently showed a positive correlation through the WHOQoL-BREF (The Whoqol Group, 1998; Murphy et al., Reference Murphy, Herrman, Hawthorne, Pinzone and Evert2000). However, the same tool in a different country (Fleck et al., Reference Fleck, Louzada, Xavier, Chachamovich, Vieira, Santos and Pinzon2000) found that active travel benefits were significant only for the physical domains (e.g., body pain, functional capacity) and only in men (Pucci et al., Reference Pucci, Reis, Rech and Hallal2012). Overall, cross-sectional studies have produced mixed results. Four reported a negative (Jurakić et al., Reference Jurakić, Pedišić and Greblo2010; Gómez et al., Reference Gómez, Moreno, Gómez, Carvajal and Parra2013) or a non-significant association (Cerin et al., Reference Cerin, Leslie, Sugiyama and Owen2009; Scarabottolo et al., Reference Scarabottolo, Cyrino, Nakamura, Tebar, Canhin, Gobbo and Christofaro2019) using a version of the SF-36.

Affect

The 11 studies identified used 11 measures to operationalise the outcome affect. Findings based on cross-sectional designs are mixed. Two reported a positive correlation between active travel and positive affect (Gatersleben and Uzzell, Reference Gatersleben and Uzzell2007; Lira and Paez, Reference Lira and Paez2021). Another confirmed these results only for walking (Rasciute and Downward, Reference Rasciute and Downward2010) and two more for cycling (Lancée et al., Reference Lancée, Veenhoven and Burger2017; Kaplan et al., Reference Kaplan, Wrzesinska and Prato2019). We also found cross-sectional evidence of a negative (Office for National Statistics, 2014) and non-significant (Morris and Guerra, Reference Morris and Guerra2015; Brainard et al., Reference Brainard, Cooke, Lane and Salter2019) relationship between travelling actively and being happy. The experimental literature refers to an RCT measuring active travel as walking (steps/day) (Baker et al., Reference Baker, Gray, Wright, Fitzsimons, Nimmo, Lowry and Mutrie2008) and a quasi-experimental study investigating e-bike use (Page and Nilsson, Reference Page and Nilsson2017). They used PANAS (Watson et al., Reference Watson, Clark and Tellegen1988) and diary analysis to measure affect. Despite the different methodologies, both reported an increase in positive affect in active travellers and not passive ones. The only longitudinal study we found confirmed this result by measuring active travel with the smartphone app Daynamica and affect on the Travel Mood Scale (Glasgow et al., Reference Glasgow, Scott Geller, Le and Hankey2018, Reference Glasgow, Le, Scott Geller, Fan and Hankey2019).

Eudaimonia

Although the five studies identified aimed to investigate eudaimonia as the “sense of purpose and meaning in life”, they all used different constructs and instruments, such as structured questionnaires (e.g., Flourishing Scale) or ad hoc statements. One cross-sectional study highlighted a significant negative relationship (Office for National Statistics, 2014), and another reported the same result only for cycling (Kaplan et al., Reference Kaplan, Wrzesinska and Prato2019). Two more cross-sectional designs showed a non-significant relationship (Brainard et al., Reference Brainard, Cooke, Lane and Salter2019; Liu et al., Reference Liu, Chen and Cao2021). A quasi-experimental analysis confirmed this result; however, their intervention was based on AM only as e-bike use (Page and Nilsson, Reference Page and Nilsson2017). The most recent study (Liu et al., Reference Liu, Chen and Cao2021) demonstrated that AM and eudaimonia are indirectly related since both significantly correlate with hedonic well-being (positive and negative affect and life satisfaction).

Life satisfaction

This outcome has been investigated using single-item instruments like, “Overall, how satisfied are you with your life?” One cross-sectional study showed that people actively travelling were less satisfied with their life (Office for National Statistics, 2014). Another confirmed this result but only for people younger than 50 (Sener and Lee, Reference Sener and Lee2017). Two more reported the opposite, so active travel was positively related to life satisfaction (McCarthy and Habib, Reference McCarthy and Habib2018; Wang et al., Reference Wang, Yin and Shao2021), in one case significant only for walking (Chng et al., Reference Chng, White, Abraham and Skippon2016). Two other analyses recorded a non-significant relationship between the factors (Brainard et al., Reference Brainard, Cooke, Lane and Salter2019; Clark et al., Reference Clark, Chatterjee, Martin and Davis2020). Together these results suggest that the context in which active travel is chosen shapes how it is perceived. The only (quasi-)experimental evidence shows that changing from public transport to active travel improved people’s life satisfaction (Jacob et al., Reference Jacob, Munford, Rice and Roberts2021).

Travel satisfaction

The search found six cross-sectional studies concerning travel satisfaction and active commuting. The results are consistent and encouraging. While the oldest article used specific aspects of satisfaction (St-Louis et al., Reference St-Louis, Manaugh, Van Lierop, Student and El-Geneidy2014) to operationalise travel satisfaction, the other five studies used some variant of the Satisfaction with Travel Scale (Friman et al., Reference Friman, Gärling, Ettema and Olsson2017; Smith, Reference Smith2017; Singleton, Reference Singleton2019; Fan et al., Reference Fan, Chen and Zhang2021; Liu et al., Reference Liu, Chen and Cao2021). These studies showed a significant positive correlation between walking and cycling and travel satisfaction.

Stress

Four cross-sectional studies explored the link between transport modes and stress levels. The oldest reported a non-significant association between active travelling and stress, claiming the “healthy commuter effect” mentioned above (Hansson et al., Reference Hansson, Mattisson, Bjork, Ostergren and Jakobsson2011). The same measure (a single-item “Do you feel stressed in your everyday life?”) was used by a later cross-sectional study (Mattisson et al., Reference Mattisson, Idris, Cromley, Håkansson, Östergren and Jakobsson2018), while two more (Avila-Palencia et al., Reference Avila-Palencia, de Nazelle, Cole-Hunter, Donaire-Gonzalez, Jerrett, Rodriguez and Nieuwenhuijsen2017; Sattler et al., Reference Sattler, Färber, Traußnig, Köberl, Paier, Dietz and van Poppel2020) using a structured questionnaire (Perceived Stress Scale, PSS-4) (Cohen et al., Reference Cohen, Kamarck and Mermelstein1983) to reveal an inverse relationship between active travel and stress levels. Finally, Avila-Palencia et al. (Reference Avila-Palencia, Int Panis, Dons, Gaupp-Berghausen, Raser, Götschi, Gerike, Brand, de Nazelle, Orjuela, Anaya-Boig, Stigell, Kahlmeier, Iacorossi and Nieuwenhuijsen2018) obtained the same result, although significant only for cycling, through a longitudinal study design using the PSS-4.

Depressive symptoms

Although the present scoping review includes only empirical studies, the results of a recent review investigating the relationship between active commuting and depression among adults (Marques et al., Reference Marques, Peralta, Henriques-Neto, Frasquilho, Gouveira and Gomez-Baya2020) are worth mentioning. Firstly, no experimental evidence was found. Five of the seven identified studies reported no significant relationship. The one with a longitudinal design reported decreased depressive symptoms after changing from inactive to active mode (Knott et al., Reference Knott, Panter, Foley and Ogilvie2018). We identified two cross-sectional studies not included in this review. Bergland et al. (Reference Bergland, Thorsen and Loland2010) studied outdoor walking ability or outdoor mobility and found no significant relationship with depression levels. However, Muñiz and others lately reported a negative relationship between these variables (Muñiz et al., Reference Muñiz, Rodríguez and Suárez2021). In addition, a longitudinal simulation by Yang et al. (Reference Yang, Langellier, Stankov, Purtle, Nelson and Diez Roux2019) using agent-based modelling (ABM) (Nianogo and Arah, Reference Nianogo and Arah2015) predicted almost no effect of walking on depression prevalence. Each study measured the outcome by applying different approaches.

Anxiety

One cross-sectional study considered only walking as the active travel mode and measured anxiety using the Hopkins Symptoms Check-List (HSCL) (Parloff et al., Reference Parloff, Kelman and Frank1954); it found no significant relationship (Bergland et al., Reference Bergland, Thorsen and Loland2010). In 2014, the ONS (UK) analysed the data from a survey asking, “Overall, how anxious did you feel yesterday?” (Office for National Statistics, 2014). Commuters were found to be more anxious than non-commuters, and those who actively travelled to places, especially for longer than 15 min, reported worse anxiety levels. On the other hand, a similar but more recent analysis found no association between anxiety levels and active commuting (Brainard et al., Reference Brainard, Cooke, Lane and Salter2019).

Social support and loneliness

The literature about social support and active travel is minimal and focused on older adults. Two studies explored social contact with friends and family and loneliness levels. They use the same instrument (UCLA Loneliness Scale; Version 3) (Austin, Reference Austin1983; Russell, Reference Russell1996). A cross-sectional design examined the mobility of a sample of older adults (74–98 years old) and found that “disrupted engagement with others” due to reduced mobility (stopped driving a car and the inadequacy of transportation) was associated with fewer current relationships which also translated in higher loneliness feelings (Smith, Reference Smith2012). The participants stated that “reaching others” would be their primary strategy to recover those contacts. The longitudinal study (Avila-Palencia et al., Reference Avila-Palencia, Int Panis, Dons, Gaupp-Berghausen, Raser, Götschi, Gerike, Brand, de Nazelle, Orjuela, Anaya-Boig, Stigell, Kahlmeier, Iacorossi and Nieuwenhuijsen2018) considered unimodal and multimodal travelling (e.g., public transport and walking) and confirmed these findings. Incorporating walking into multimodal trips was associated with higher social contacts, and cycling in multimodal trips was related to fewer feelings of loneliness.

Self-efficacy

All included studies were cross-sectional and typically assumed that self-efficacy was a determinant of transport mode choices, not a result. Two papers studied outdoor mobility in the elderly and active travel in adults linked to self-efficacy using structured scales (Bergland et al., Reference Bergland, Thorsen and Loland2010; Molina-García et al., Reference Molina-García, Castillo and Sallis2010). Cross-sectional evidence was also gained from implementing a public bicycle share program (BIXI – Bicycle-taXI, Canada) designed to increase people’s intention and self-efficacy to use the bike as a means of transport (Bélanger-Gravel et al., Reference Bélanger-Gravel, Gauvin, Fuller and Drouin2016). They measured self-efficacy with ad hoc statements, and the program led to an increase in active travel and self-efficacy. The most recent evidence (Brainard et al., Reference Brainard, Cooke, Lane and Salter2019) showed a significant positive relationship only for walking. Overall, the results are modest and hard to interpret.

Sleep

Evidence regarding sleep patterns and AM is limited to cross-sectional studies. One found no significant results (Hansson et al., Reference Hansson, Mattisson, Bjork, Ostergren and Jakobsson2011). Two were published in 2018 and used data from public health surveys but operationalised sleep behaviour differently. For example, Foley et al. (Reference Foley, Dumuid, Atkin, Olds and Ogilvie2018) used self-reported sleeping time (minutes/day) records, while Vancampfort et al. (Reference Vancampfort, Smith, Stubbs, Swinnen, Firth, Schuch and Koyanagi2018) assessed sleep through a single item about sleeping issues (falling asleep, waking up frequently or too early in the morning). The former found that active travellers reported shorter sleep duration, yet, the latter found that people actively travelling scored better sleep.

Vitality/exhaustion

The evidence about this outcome is mainly cross-sectional. Avila-Palencia et al. (Reference Avila-Palencia, Int Panis, Dons, Gaupp-Berghausen, Raser, Götschi, Gerike, Brand, de Nazelle, Orjuela, Anaya-Boig, Stigell, Kahlmeier, Iacorossi and Nieuwenhuijsen2018) identified a significant positive relationship between cycling and walking as a means of transport and vitality scores. Clark et al. (Reference Clark, Chatterjee, Martin and Davis2020) claimed that walking to work was associated with reduced strain, thus, better vitality. Nevertheless, low vitality (exhaustion) has shown no significant relationship with active travel (Hansson et al., Reference Hansson, Mattisson, Bjork, Ostergren and Jakobsson2011; Mattisson et al., Reference Mattisson, Idris, Cromley, Håkansson, Östergren and Jakobsson2018). The inconsistency might likely be due to the methodology and construct definition differences.

Self-reported health

Self-reported health has mainly been investigated via single-item questions like “How is your health in general?” (Rasciute and Downward, Reference Rasciute and Downward2010) or “How do you judge your general state of health?” (Bergland et al., Reference Bergland, Thorsen and Loland2010). In other cases, authors used physical health scales, such as the Physical Component Score of the SF-36 (PCS). Researchers have also measured people’s health status as the sum of outcomes such as sickness and absence from work (Hansson et al., Reference Hansson, Mattisson, Bjork, Ostergren and Jakobsson2011; Mytton et al., Reference Mytton, Panter and Ogilvie2016), long-term illness, walking difficulties and obesity (BMI) (Mattisson et al., Reference Mattisson, Idris, Cromley, Håkansson, Östergren and Jakobsson2018). Considering the established beneficial influence of transport physical activity on physical health (Marques et al., Reference Marques, Peralta, Henriques-Neto, Frasquilho, Gouveira and Gomez-Baya2020; Alattar et al., Reference Alattar, Cottrill and Beecroft2021), it was no surprise that active travel modes improved objective and subjective health measures. Objectively, walking and cycling reduced sickness absence days, illness levels, walking difficulties and obesity scores (Mattisson et al., Reference Mattisson, Idris, Cromley, Håkansson, Östergren and Jakobsson2018). Subjectively, cycling and walking were associated with better self-perceived health compared to car driving or taking the bus (Avila-Palencia et al., Reference Avila-Palencia, Int Panis, Dons, Gaupp-Berghausen, Raser, Götschi, Gerike, Brand, de Nazelle, Orjuela, Anaya-Boig, Stigell, Kahlmeier, Iacorossi and Nieuwenhuijsen2018; Clark et al., Reference Clark, Chatterjee, Martin and Davis2020). These studies, investigating mental and physical aspects of health, confirmed the positive relationship between these components of overall health (Rasciute and Downward, Reference Rasciute and Downward2010; Mason et al., Reference Mason, Curl and Kearns2016; Sener and Lee, Reference Sener and Lee2017; Mattisson et al., Reference Mattisson, Idris, Cromley, Håkansson, Östergren and Jakobsson2018). Five cross-sectional designs claimed a positive relationship between AM and self-reported health, four using specific questions (Rasciute and Downward, Reference Rasciute and Downward2010; Humphreys et al., Reference Humphreys, Goodman and Ogilvie2013; Mason et al., Reference Mason, Curl and Kearns2016; Mattisson et al., Reference Mattisson, Idris, Cromley, Håkansson, Östergren and Jakobsson2018; Muñiz et al., Reference Muñiz, Rodríguez and Suárez2021). Two more confirmed these results: walking (Bergland et al., Reference Bergland, Thorsen and Loland2010) and cycling (Clark et al., Reference Clark, Chatterjee, Martin and Davis2020). Other cross-sectional findings described a non-significant relationship (Bergland et al., Reference Bergland, Thorsen and Loland2010; Hansson et al., Reference Hansson, Mattisson, Bjork, Ostergren and Jakobsson2011; Mason et al., Reference Mason, Curl and Kearns2016; Sener and Lee, Reference Sener and Lee2017).

The longitudinal and quasi-experimental evidence indicated a positive relationship between better self-reported health and walking and cycling (Avila-Palencia et al., Reference Avila-Palencia, Int Panis, Dons, Gaupp-Berghausen, Raser, Götschi, Gerike, Brand, de Nazelle, Orjuela, Anaya-Boig, Stigell, Kahlmeier, Iacorossi and Nieuwenhuijsen2018), maintaining cycling over time (Mytton et al., Reference Mytton, Panter and Ogilvie2016), and changing from passive to active transport modes. In conclusion, self-reported health appears to have a stronger significant relationship with active travel compared to mental health outcomes, such as self-efficacy, self-esteem, depression and anxiety (Bergland et al., Reference Bergland, Thorsen and Loland2010), stress and vitality (Mattisson et al., Reference Mattisson, Idris, Cromley, Håkansson, Östergren and Jakobsson2018) and overall mental health (Hansson et al., Reference Hansson, Mattisson, Bjork, Ostergren and Jakobsson2011) and mental well-being (Humphreys et al., Reference Humphreys, Goodman and Ogilvie2013).

Discussion

To our knowledge, this review is the first to examine a vast range of mental health outcomes related to “AM” in healthy adult populations. Moreover, it highlights promising evidence of the positive effects that AM might have on mental health, both in terms of prevention and interventions to reduce symptoms.

In this section, we attempt to summarise areas where methodologies and outcomes seem to agree and where there is a divergence or a lack of relevant investigations.

Key findings for mental health outcomes

Regarding the number of studies, mental health, quality of life, and the affective component of subjective well-being have received the most extensive attention. These, followed by self-reported health, also appeared as the most promising regarding a favourable relationship with active travel modes, confirmed by the experimental or quasi-experimental investigations. Although evidence regarding travel satisfaction, life satisfaction, stress, loneliness and social support, and self-efficacy was limited, results were encouraging from a health promotion perspective. Eudaimonia, depressive symptoms, anxiety, sleep, and vitality had limited or mixed findings. No studies about self-esteem and resilience could be found.

Points of consensus and gaps in the literature

We grouped these into five areas.

What is active mobility?

Given the differing backgrounds and study designs, a consensus definition is complex. This problem is discussed further in section “AM, the exposure factor”. Study differences must be interpreted based on the included and excluded behaviours. For example, public transport represents a contentious factor since some studies implicitly discount the walking associated with travel to bus stops or train stations (Neumeier et al., Reference Neumeier, Loidl, Reich, Fernandez La Puente de Battre, Kissel, Templin, Schmied, Niebauer and Niederseer2020). Furthermore, the trip purpose or domain might be crucial. Studies have reported that leisure-time walking and cycling consistently correlate with the quality of life, whereas transport physical activity does not (Rasciute and Downward, Reference Rasciute and Downward2010; Pucci et al., Reference Pucci, Reis, Rech and Hallal2012). Finally, some authors explicitly investigated walking or cycling (Mutrie et al., Reference Mutrie, Carney, Blamey, Crawford, Aitchison and Whitelaw2002; De Geus et al., Reference De Geus, Van Hoof, Aerts and Meeusen2008); others found significant results only for one active travel modality and not the other, without further exploration to explain these findings (Mytton et al., Reference Mytton, Panter and Ogilvie2016; Avila-Palencia et al., Reference Avila-Palencia, Int Panis, Dons, Gaupp-Berghausen, Raser, Götschi, Gerike, Brand, de Nazelle, Orjuela, Anaya-Boig, Stigell, Kahlmeier, Iacorossi and Nieuwenhuijsen2018).

Does the quantity matter?

Not only mode and purpose but also duration and distance could impact the beneficial health effects of AM (Singleton, Reference Singleton2019). As a form of physical activity, frequency, duration, and intensity might mediate or moderate AM’s health benefits (Ekkekakis et al., Reference Ekkekakis, Parfitt and Petruzzello2011; Chan et al., Reference Chan, Liu, Liang, Deng, Wu and Yan2019). While it is not yet clear if a “dose–response” relationship exists in the results, authors have found that for each additional commuting minute, especially for trips longer than 10–15 min, people using active travel modes (and public transport) tend to report lower eudaimonia, life satisfaction, travel satisfaction, happiness, and overall mental health and higher strain and anxiety feelings (Office for National Statistics, 2014; St-Louis et al., Reference St-Louis, Manaugh, Van Lierop, Student and El-Geneidy2014; Singleton, Reference Singleton2019; Clark et al., Reference Clark, Chatterjee, Martin and Davis2020; Wang et al., Reference Wang, Yin and Shao2021). Recently, a study on travel satisfaction confirmed these results (De Vos et al., Reference De Vos, Le and Kroesen2022). Compared to commute mode, commute duration showed a stronger association with the outcome and appeared to moderate the relationship with the exposure. While walking and cycling usually refer to shorter trips (less than 20 min), public transport is used for more than 30 min commutes. This time frames likely explain the resulting satisfaction levels.

Still, commuting time did not relate to the quality of life (Neumeier et al., Reference Neumeier, Loidl, Reich, Fernandez La Puente de Battre, Kissel, Templin, Schmied, Niebauer and Niederseer2020), eudaimonia (Page and Nilsson, Reference Page and Nilsson2017) and life satisfaction (Clark et al., Reference Clark, Chatterjee, Martin and Davis2020), as reported by experimental and quasi-experimental evidence and more recent cross-sectional designs.

Does the context matter (where and when)?

Scientists have demonstrated that walking through green and blue areas (natural environments) (Glasgow et al., Reference Glasgow, Le, Scott Geller, Fan and Hankey2019) provides advantages in terms of positive mood and overall mental health (Zijlema et al., Reference Zijlema, Avila-Palencia, Triguero-Mas, Gidlow, Maas, Kruize, Andrusaityte, Grazuleviciene and Nieuwenhuijsen2018; Wicks et al., Reference Wicks, Barton, Orbell and Andrews2022). However, when investigating commuting, authors rarely specify where the active travel occurred or within what type of settings. The surrounding environment might represent an obstacle or an advantage to the beneficial effect of walking and cycling on mental health, well-being and mood (Glasgow et al., Reference Glasgow, Le, Scott Geller, Fan and Hankey2019). Opposite to the consistent positive effect on well-being shown by walking, cycling’s benefits could be influenced by “disutility” connected to environmental barriers (Rasciute and Downward, Reference Rasciute and Downward2010). Public bicycle stations around the home/work/study addresses, levels of greenness and “bikeability/walkability”, and the presence and length of sidewalks are all examples of external characteristics influencing active travel behaviour (Avila-Palencia et al., Reference Avila-Palencia, de Nazelle, Cole-Hunter, Donaire-Gonzalez, Jerrett, Rodriguez and Nieuwenhuijsen2017; Glasgow et al., Reference Glasgow, Le, Scott Geller, Fan and Hankey2019).

Regardless of the transport mode, contextual characteristics might directly impact stress levels (Avila-Palencia et al., Reference Avila-Palencia, de Nazelle, Cole-Hunter, Donaire-Gonzalez, Jerrett, Rodriguez and Nieuwenhuijsen2017). The season or the hour of the day could also be considered “objective external characteristics”. A study in Canada observed that people tend to use active commuting in spring/summer and switch to “public transit” in winter. Also, those actively travelling in harsh weather conditions showed significantly lower travel satisfaction. Lastly, they noticed that people working during regular hours were more satisfied than those who travelled to work during irregular hours, possibly due to a lack of adequate public services during those hours (St-Louis et al., Reference St-Louis, Manaugh, Van Lierop, Student and El-Geneidy2014).

Do personal characteristics matter?

Individual and demographic characteristics are potentially confounding variables in the link between AM and mental health. However, there is no consensus as to which are most influential. Such characteristics include sex, age, education, employment status, monthly income, ethnicity, marital/relationship status, household (cohabiting status; the number of children), and baseline health (operationalised as weight, BMI, and physical activity levels).

Age and sex have significantly impacted the use of AM modes and their relationship with health benefits. Younger people, particularly men, travel more frequently by walking and cycling (Bergland et al., Reference Bergland, Thorsen and Loland2010; Scarabottolo et al., Reference Scarabottolo, Tebar, Gobbo, Ohara, Ferreira, Canhin, da and Christofaro2022), albeit not in all cases (Cobbold et al., Reference Cobbold, Standen, Shepherd, Greaves and Crane2022). Age also appeared to moderate active travel effects on life satisfaction, emotional well-being and travel satisfaction, so people older than 50 opting for active travel had poorer mental health scores (Friman et al., Reference Friman, Gärling, Ettema and Olsson2017; Sener and Lee, Reference Sener and Lee2017). Regarding differences between men and women, the results are equivocal. Studies have shown that commuting on foot or by bike significantly benefits mental health and quality of life only in men (Ohta et al., Reference Ohta, Mizoue, Mishima and Ikeda2007; Pucci et al., Reference Pucci, Reis, Rech and Hallal2012). However, little experimental and quasi-experimental evidence claimed that changing from passive to active travelling improves mental health in both men and women (higher vitality and overall mental health), but physical health significantly increased in women and decreased in men (De Geus et al., Reference De Geus, Van Hoof, Aerts and Meeusen2008; Jacob et al., Reference Jacob, Munford, Rice and Roberts2021). Also, changing from active to passive modes meant poorer physical health in both sexes but better mental health in women (Jacob et al., Reference Jacob, Munford, Rice and Roberts2021). Other than age and sex, social disparities might have a role in influencing quality-of-life improvements due to AM (Gómez et al., Reference Gómez, Moreno, Gómez, Carvajal and Parra2013). For instance, it was noted that in low-income countries, people might be forced to walk or cycle as a means of transport because of their poverty conditions and associated feelings of discomfort (Pucci et al., Reference Pucci, Reis, Rech and Hallal2012; Lira and Paez, Reference Lira and Paez2021). Nevertheless, the moderating role of these factors remains unclear and only potential since the results of their influence have not been widely confirmed (Sattler et al., Reference Sattler, Färber, Traußnig, Köberl, Paier, Dietz and van Poppel2020).

Does study methodology matter?

Most studies reviewed were cross-sectional, and many used national population survey data. However, it is difficult to identify any element of the study design on which consensus was achieved. Indeed, different kinds of studies were considered suitable for examining different aspects of the question. Still, the only experimental evidence identified dates back almost 20 years ago. The interest in this phenomenon spans the fields of environmental studies, transportation studies, economics, public health and exercise science. Thus, the instruments and terminology employed, as valid and reliable as they may be, are frequently not very specific, which can create concerns in comparing the results. Furthermore, most information about active commuting and mental health was self-reported. However, authors have demonstrated that self-report and objective measures might have the same validity in reporting AM characteristics (Gebel et al., Reference Gebel, Bauman and Owen2009; Laeremans et al., Reference Laeremans, Dons, Avila-Palencia, Carrasco-Turigas, Orjuela, Anaya, Brand, Cole-Hunter, De Nazelle, Götschi, Kahlmeier, Nieuwenhuijsen, Standaert, De Boever and Int Panis2017). Focusing on mental health and quality of life outcomes in adults resulted in neglecting other vital aspects of adult people’s health like cognitive and neurological function, the former considered predominantly in school-aged populations. Together with self-efficacy, and social support, self-esteem and resilience are considered mental health “protective” factors (Keyes, Reference Keyes2002, Reference Keyes2005, Reference Keyes2007). Resilience is widely accepted as a vital psychological aspect of healthy functioning from an individual and public health perspective (Kelly et al., Reference Kelly, Williamson, Niven, Hunter, Mutrie and Richards2018). Despite its significance, active travel research has wholly ignored these aspects.

Despite well-validated instruments to measure specific mental health outcomes exist, their absence in the current literature is a lacuna that future studies in this field should address.

Limitations of the present study

The search method used by this review to identify studies was tightly focused on English language studies with AM as the exposure and mental health as the outcomes and healthy adults as the sample, so important literature may have been excluded where, for example, the distinction between walking and cycling as a means of transport and as a leisure-time activity was not completely clarified, where the population was younger or older, or the reports were written in other languages.

In order to limit the scope of an already complex review, we have not attempted to untangle the possibly contrasting contributions of mixed-mode travel to the outcomes of interest and have avoided studying the effects of partially active modes, including electric bicycles and scooters.

The studies included in this review used a variety of research designs and methodologies. Most employed cross-sectional analyses, precluding causal inferences about the relationship between AM and mental health.

Most included studies focused on single ethnical-cultural-social populations (USA, Belgium, New Zealand, Japan, older adults, or workers), meaning their results are only partially comparable and might not be valid for other populations.

Lastly, we did not attempt to account for publication bias due to the absence of unpublished studies with no or incoherent conclusions.

Future perspective: A unified conceptual and empirical framework

Based on a review of the current state of the literature, we believe that more prospective experimental studies are needed, using specific instruments to operationalise mental health outcomes. For example, the healthy commuter effect discussed above might be crucial to investigate through the experimental approach, producing new AM unbiased research. In line with the health promotion and precision health approach (Ryan et al., Reference Ryan, Viana, Sellak, Gondalia and O’Callaghan2021), it is necessary to understand what factors might change AM’s advantageous effect on people’s health. To this end, it would be helpful to better define AM as a physical activity (in frequency, intensity and duration) that would allow a more objective comparison of different modes, such as walking, cycling, e-bike use or even wheelchair use and skateboarding. Additionally, critical environmental factors, such as air pollution or green areas, are more accessible information nowadays using lightweight, portable instruments to track people’s transport routes. However, there remain many open questions. To what extent is journey purpose influential? Are health benefits due to an additive or synergic relationship between moderators and exposure? Are there groups of people who are more susceptible? Active travel has a potentially significant and bidirectional relationship with social support and loneliness that has been little investigated. Does active travel increase social connection and reduces loneliness? We suggest a unified and shared framework with precise terminology, properly operationalised variables and specific measurement tools to assist a comparative evaluation of the studies already published and the design of new research in this area. Although a similar task has been attempted recently (Götschi et al., Reference Götschi, de Nazelle, Brand, Gerike, Alasya, Anaya, Avila-Palencia, Banister, Bartana, Benvenuti, Boschetti, Brand, Buekers, Carniel, Carrasco Turigas, Castro, Cianfano, Clark, Cole-Hunter and Zeuschner2017), the authors aimed to identify the determinants of AM behaviours more than their health benefits and mental health was scarcely considered. Still, the current literature is highly mixed.

Taken at face value, the results presented in this study suggest that in many cases different cohorts respond differently to the experience of AM; policymakers should carefully consider the intended targets of any intervention as responses may not be uniformly positive. Specific recommendations can only be given when solid evidence has been gathered to answer the questions posed above.

Conclusions

This review lists outcomes related to mental health that have received scientific attention when studying AM’s effects on health. Although limited and mainly cross-sectional, this evidence has suggested that AM, defined as walking and cycling to get to and from places, provides significant benefits in terms of mental health. A few experimental designs have mainly investigated the quality of life. Moreover, since the millennium, authors have broadened their interest in examining overall mental health, positive and negative affect and self-reported health. More recently, cross-sectional studies have also focused on other important individual and public health outcomes like stress, life and travel satisfaction, loneliness and social support. Also, depressive and anxiety symptoms and sleep quality have been surprisingly neglected, considering their global health-related burden at an individual and societal level. Moreover, eudaimonia, self-efficacy, self-esteem, and resilience, which might all have a role in this subject, are barely considered. Finally, as findings in different populations are hardly comparable, we described the approaches used in the studies included. We hypothesise that active travel modes and mental health benefits have been explored from a non-strictly psychological scientific perspective, leading to inconsistencies in methodologies, outcome definitions and instruments and omitting essential health factors.

We hope this review will motivate new and diverse approaches to research how AM and mental health interact, aiming to promote sustainable lifestyles and improve public health and personal well-being.

Open peer review

To view the open peer review materials for this article, please visit http://doi.org/10.1017/gmh.2023.74.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/gmh.2023.74.

Data availability statement

The authors confirm that the data supporting the findings of this study are available within the article (and/or its Supplementary Materials).

Author contribution

The authors equally contributed to the conceptualisation of the study. L.S. and S.M.M. conceived the scoping review design. L.S. and D.N.M. planned the methodology. L.S. conducted data acquisition and data extraction, and D.N.M. completed it with additional numerical values. L.S. performed narrative analysis, interpretation and reporting. D.N.M. performed the numerical analyses and graphical illustration. D.N.M. and A.T. revised the drafts. Finally, D.N.M. and L.S. edited the interim versions of this manuscript. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Financial support

This research received no specific grant from any funding agency in the public, commercial or not-for-profit sectors.

Competing interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

Alattar, MA, Cottrill, C and Beecroft, M (2021) Sources and applications of emerging active travel data: A review of the literature. Sustainability (Switzerland) 13(13), 7006. https://doi.org/10.3390/su13137006.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
American Psychiatric Association (2013) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. Washington, DC: APA. https://doi.org/10.1176/APPI.BOOKS.9780890425596.Google Scholar
American Psychological Association (2022) Anxiety. Available at https://www.apa.org/topics/anxiety (accessed 17 February 2023).Google Scholar
Anable, J and Gatersleben, B (2005) All work and no play? The role of instrumental and affective factors in work and leisure journeys by different travel modes. Transportation Research Part A: Policy and Practice 39(2–3), 163181. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.TRA.2004.09.008.Google Scholar
APA Dictionary of Psychology (n.d.) Social support. Available at https://dictionary.apa.org/social-support (accessed 23 February 2023).Google Scholar
Arksey, H and O’Malley, L (2005) Scoping studies: Towards a methodological framework. International Journal of Social Research Methodology: Theory and Practice 8(1), 1932. https://doi.org/10.1080/1364557032000119616.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Austin, BA (1983) Factorial structure of the UCLA Loneliness Scale. Psychological Reports 53(3 I), 883889. https://doi.org/10.2466/PR0.1983.53.3.883.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Avila-Palencia, I, de Nazelle, A, Cole-Hunter, T, Donaire-Gonzalez, D, Jerrett, M, Rodriguez, DA and Nieuwenhuijsen, MJ (2017) The relationship between bicycle commuting and perceived stress: A cross-sectional study. BMJ Open 7(6), e013542. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmjopen-2016-013542.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Avila-Palencia, I, Int Panis, L, Dons, E, Gaupp-Berghausen, M, Raser, E, Götschi, T, Gerike, R, Brand, C, de Nazelle, A, Orjuela, JP, Anaya-Boig, E, Stigell, E, Kahlmeier, S, Iacorossi, F and Nieuwenhuijsen, MJ (2018) The effects of transport mode use on self-perceived health, mental health, and social contact measures: A cross-sectional and longitudinal study. Environment International 120, 199206. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.envint.2018.08.002.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baker, G, Gray, SR, Wright, A, Fitzsimons, C, Nimmo, M, Lowry, R and Mutrie, N (2008) The effect of a pedometer-based community walking intervention ‘walking for wellbeing in the west’ on physical activity levels and health outcomes: A 12-week randomized controlled trial. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity 5, 44. https://doi.org/10.1186/1479-5868-5-44.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bandura, A (1977) Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psychological Review 84(2), 191215. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.84.2.191.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bandura, A (1986) Social foundations of thought and action. In Social Foundations of Thought and Action: A Social Cognitive Theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall, pp. 2328.Google Scholar
Bauman, S (2012) The importance of self-esteem in learning and behavior in children with exceptionalities and the role magic tricks may play in improving self-esteem and in motivating learning. HIM 1990–2015. Available at https://stars.library.ucf.edu/honorstheses1990-2015/1344.Google Scholar
Bélanger-Gravel, A, Gauvin, L, Fuller, D and Drouin, L (2016) Association of implementation of a public bicycle share program with intention and self-efficacy: The moderating role of socioeconomic status. Journal of Health Psychology 21(6), 944953. https://doi.org/10.1177/1359105314542820.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benoit, K, Watanabe, K, Wang, H, Nulty, P, Obeng, A, Müller, S and Matsuo, A (2018) Quanteda: An R package for the quantitative analysis of textual data. Journal of Open Source Software 3(30), 774. https://doi.org/10.21105/joss.00774.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bergland, A, Thorsen, K and Loland, NW (2010) The relationship between coping, self-esteem and health on outdoor walking ability among older adults in Norway. Ageing and Society 30(6), 949963. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0144686X1000022X.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berwick, DM, Murphy, JM, Goldman, PA, Ware, JE, Barsky, AJ and Weinstein, MC (1991) Performance of a five-item mental health screening test. Medical Care 29(2), 169176.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bos, I, Jacobs, L, Nawrot, TS, de Geus, B, Torfs, R, Int Panis, L, Degraeuwe, B and Meeusen, R (2011) No exercise-induced increase in serum BDNF after cycling near a major traffic road. Neuroscience Letters 500(2), 129132. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2011.06.019.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowling, A (2001) Measuring Disease. Buckingham: Open University Press.Google Scholar
Brainard, J, Cooke, R, Lane, K and Salter, C (2019) Age, sex and other correlates with active travel walking and cycling in England: Analysis of responses to the active lives survey 2016/17. Preventive Medicine 123, 225231. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ypmed.2019.03.043.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cerin, E, Leslie, E, Sugiyama, T and Owen, N (2009) Associations of multiple physical activity domains with mental well-being. Mental Health and Physical Activity 2(2), 5564. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.mhpa.2009.09.004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chan, JSY, Liu, G, Liang, D, Deng, K, Wu, J and Yan, JH (2019) Special issue–therapeutic benefits of physical activity for mood: A systematic review on the effects of exercise intensity, duration, and modality. Journal of Psychology: Interdisciplinary and Applied 153(1), 102125. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223980.2018.1470487.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Childs, E and de Wit, H (2014) Regular exercise is associated with emotional resilience to acute stress in healthy adults. Frontiers in Physiology 5, 161. https://doi.org/10.3389/FPHYS.2014.00161/BIBTEX.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chng, S, White, M, Abraham, C and Skippon, S (2016) Commuting and wellbeing in London: The roles of commute mode and local public transport connectivity. Preventive Medicine 88, 182188. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ypmed.2016.04.014.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, B, Chatterjee, K, Martin, A and Davis, A (2020) How commuting affects subjective wellbeing. Transportation 47(6), 27772805. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11116-019-09983-9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cobbold, A, Standen, C, Shepherd, L, Greaves, S and Crane, M (2022) Multimodal trips, quality of life and wellbeing: An exploratory analysis. Journal of Transport and Health 24, 101330. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jth.2022.101330.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, S, Kamarck, T and Mermelstein, R (1983) A global measure of perceived stress. Journal of Health and Social Behavior 24(4), 385396. https://doi.org/10.2307/2136404.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Geus, B, Van Hoof, E, Aerts, I and Meeusen, R (2008) Cycling to work: Influence on indexes of health in untrained men and women in Flanders. Coronary heart disease and quality of life. Scandinavian Journal of Medicine and Science in Sports 18(4), 498510. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0838.2007.00729.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Jong Gierveld, J and van Tilburg, TG (2016) Social isolation and loneliness. In Encyclopedia of Mental Health: Second Edition. Amsterdam: Elsevier, pp. 175178. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-397045-9.00118-X.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Vos, J, Le, HTK and Kroesen, M (2022) Does commute duration attenuate the effect of travel mode choice on commute satisfaction? Travel Behaviour and Society 28, 1321. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tbs.2022.02.004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Diener, E and Emmons, RA (1984) The independence of positive and negative affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 47(5), 11051117. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.47.5.1105.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diener, E, Suh, EM, Lucas, RE and Smith, HL (1999) Subjective well-being: Three decades of progress. Psychological Blletin 125(2), 276.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ekkekakis, P, Parfitt, G and Petruzzello, SJ (2011) The pleasure and displeasure people feel when they exercise at different intensities: Decennial update and progress towards a tripartite rationale for exercise intensity prescription. Sports Medicine (Auckland, N.Z.) 41(8), 641. https://doi.org/10.2165/11590680-000000000-00000.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ettema, D, Gärling, T, Eriksson, L, Friman, M, Olsson, LE and Fujii, S (2011) Satisfaction with travel and subjective well-being: Development and test of a measurement tool. Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour 14(3), 167175. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.TRF.2010.11.002.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ettema, D, Gärling, T, Olsson, LE and Friman, M (2010) Out-of-home activities, daily travel, and subjective well-being. Transportation Research Part A: Policy and Practice 44(9), 723732. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tra.2010.07.005.Google Scholar
FAIR Principles (n.d.) GO FAIR. Available at https://www.go-fair.org/fair-principles/ (accessed 27 May 2023).Google Scholar
Fan, A, Chen, X and Zhang, X (2021) What factors contribute to higher travel happiness? Evidence from Beijing, China. Journal of Advanced Transportation 2021, 8861841. https://doi.org/10.1155/2021/8861841.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fleck, MP, Louzada, S, Xavier, M, Chachamovich, E, Vieira, G, Santos, L and Pinzon, V (2000) Aplicação da versão em português do instrumento abreviado de avaliação da quali-dade de Vida ‘WHOQOL-bref’ application of the Portuguese version of the abbreviated instrument of quality life WHOQOL-bref. Revista de Saúde Pública 32(2), 178183.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Foley, L, Dumuid, D, Atkin, AJ, Olds, T and Ogilvie, D (2018) Patterns of health behaviour associated with active travel: A compositional data analysis. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity 15(1), 26. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12966-018-0662-8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fornell, C, Johnson, MD, Anderson, EW, Cha, J and Bryant, BE (1996) The American customer satisfaction index: Nature, purpose, and findings. Journal of Marketing 60(4), 718. https://doi.org/10.1177/002224299606000403.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Friman, M, Fujii, S, Ettema, D, Gärling, T and Olsson, LE (2013) Psychometric analysis of the satisfaction with travel scale. Transportation Research Part A: Policy and Practice 48, 132145. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tra.2012.10.012.Google Scholar
Friman, M, Gärling, T, Ettema, D and Olsson, LE (2017) How does travel affect emotional well-being and life satisfaction? Transportation Research Part A: Policy and Practice 106, 170180. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tra.2017.09.024.Google Scholar
Galderisi, S, Heinz, A, Kastrup, M, Beezhold, J and Sartorius, N (2015) Toward a new definition of mental health. World Psychiatry 14(2), 231233. https://doi.org/10.1002/wps.20231.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gatersleben, B and Uzzell, D (2007) Affective appraisals of the daily commute: Comparing perceptions of drivers, cyclists, walkers, and users of public transport. Environment and Behavior 39(3), 416431. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013916506294032.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gebel, K, Bauman, A and Owen, N (2009) Correlates of non-concordance between perceived and objective measures of walkability. Annals of Behavioral Medicine 37(2), 228238. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12160-009-9098-3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gerike, R, De Nazelle, A, Nieuwenhuijsen, M, Panis, LI, Anaya, E, Avila-Palencia, I, Boschetti, F, Brand, C, Cole-Hunter, T, Dons, E, Eriksson, U, Gaupp-Berghausen, M, Kahlmeier, S, Laeremans, M, Mueller, N, Orjuela, JP, Racioppi, F, Raser, E, Rojas-Rueda, D and Götschi, T (2016) Physical activity through sustainable transport approaches (PASTA): A study protocol for a multicentre project. BMJ Open 6(1), e009924. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmjopen-2015-009924.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glasgow, TE, Le, HTK, Scott Geller, E, Fan, Y and Hankey, S (2019) How transport modes, the built and natural environments, and activities influence mood: A GPS smartphone app study. Journal of Environmental Psychology 66, 101345. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2019.101345.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glasgow, TE, Scott Geller, E, Le, B and Hankey, S (2018) Travel mood scale: Development and validation of a survey to measure mood during transportation. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.trf.2018.09.014.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldberg, DP (1972) The Detection of Psychiatric Illness by Questionnaire: A Technique for the Identification and Assessment of Non-psychotic Psychiatric Illness. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Available at https://psycnet.apa.org/record/1973-23456-000.Google Scholar
Gómez, LF, Moreno, J, Gómez, OL, Carvajal, R and Parra, DC (2013) Physical activity and health-related quality of life among adult women in Cali, Colombia: A cross-sectional study. Quality of Life Research 22(9), 23512358. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11136-013-0378-9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Götschi, T, de Nazelle, A, Brand, C, Gerike, R, Alasya, B, Anaya, E, Avila-Palencia, I, Banister, D, Bartana, I, Benvenuti, F, Boschetti, F, Brand, C, Buekers, J, Carniel, L, Carrasco Turigas, G, Castro, A, Cianfano, M, Clark, A, Cole-Hunter, T and Zeuschner, V (2017) Towards a comprehensive conceptual framework of active travel behavior: A review and synthesis of published frameworks. Current Environmental Health Reports 4(3), 286295. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40572-017-0149-9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gottholmseder, G, Nowotny, K, Pruckner, GJ and Theurl, E (2009) Stress perception and commuting. Health Economics 18(5), 559576. https://doi.org/10.1002/HEC.1389.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gupta, S (2022) Why fuzzy definitions are a problem in the social sciences. ScienceNews, October 26. Available at https://www.sciencenews.org/article/social-sciences-definitions-terms-research-measurment.Google Scholar
Hansson, E, Mattisson, K, Bjork, J, Ostergren, P-O and Jakobsson, K (2011) Relationship between commuting and health outcomes in a cross-sectional population survey in southern Sweden. BMC Public Health 11, 834. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2458-11-834.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harrison, S, Jones, HE, Martin, RM, Lewis, SJ and Higgins, JPT (2017) The albatross plot: A novel graphical tool for presenting results of diversely reported studies in a systematic review. Research Synthesis Methods 8(3), 281289. https://doi.org/10.1002/jrsm.1239.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Humphreys, DK, Goodman, A and Ogilvie, D (2013) Associations between active commuting and physical and mental wellbeing. Preventive Medicine 57(2), 135139. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ypmed.2013.04.008.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jacob, N, Munford, L, Rice, N and Roberts, J (2021) Does commuting mode choice impact health? Health Economics (United Kingdom) 30(2), 207230. https://doi.org/10.1002/hec.4184.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jurakić, D, Pedišić, Ž and Greblo, Z (2010) Physical activity in different domains and health-related quality of life: A population-based study. Quality of Life Research 19(9), 13031309. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11136-010-9705-6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaplan, S, Wrzesinska, DK and Prato, CG (2019) Psychosocial benefits and positive mood related to habitual bicycle use. Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour 64, 342352. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.trf.2019.05.018.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karimi, M and Brazier, J (2016) Health, health-related quality of life, and quality of life: What is the difference? PharmacoEconomics 34(7), 645649. https://doi.org/10.1007/S40273-016-0389-9/METRICS.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Katsarou, AL, Triposkiadis, F and Panagiotakos, D (2013) Perceived stress and vascular disease: Where are we now? Angiology 64(7), 529534. https://doi.org/10.1177/0003319712458963.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kelly, P, Williamson, C, Niven, AG, Hunter, R, Mutrie, N and Richards, J (2018) Walking on sunshine: Scoping review of the evidence for walking and mental health. British Journal of Sports Medicine 52(12), 800806. https://doi.org/10.1136/bjsports-2017-098827.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keyes, CLM (2002) The mental health continuum: From languishing to flourishing in life. Journal of Health and Social Behavior 43(2), 207222. https://doi.org/10.2307/3090197.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keyes, CLM (2005) Mental illness and/or mental health? Investigating axioms of the complete state model of health. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 73(3), 539548. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.73.3.539.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keyes, CLM (2007) Promoting and protecting mental health as flourishing: A complementary strategy for improving National Mental Health. American Psychologist 62(2), 95108. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.62.2.95.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Knott, CS, Panter, J, Foley, L and Ogilvie, D (2018) Changes in the mode of travel to work and the severity of depressive symptoms: A longitudinal analysis of UK biobank. Preventive Medicine 112, 6169. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ypmed.2018.03.018.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kroesen, M and De Vos, J (2020) Does active travel make people healthier, or are healthy people more inclined to travel actively? Journal of Transport and Health 16, 100844. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jth.2020.100844.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laeremans, M, Dons, E, Avila-Palencia, I, Carrasco-Turigas, G, Orjuela, JP, Anaya, E, Brand, C, Cole-Hunter, T, De Nazelle, A, Götschi, T, Kahlmeier, S, Nieuwenhuijsen, M, Standaert, A, De Boever, P and Int Panis, L (2017) Physical activity and sedentary behaviour in daily life: A comparative analysis of the global physical activity questionnaire (GPAQ) and the SenseWear armband. PLoS One 12(5), e0177765. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0177765.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lampinen, P, Heikkinen, RL, Kauppinen, M and Heikkinen, E (2006) Activity as a predictor of mental well-being among older adults. Aging and Mental Health 10(5), 454466. https://doi.org/10.1080/13607860600640962.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lancée, S, Veenhoven, R and Burger, M (2017) Mood during commute in the Netherlands: What way of travel feels best for what kind of people? Transportation Research Part A: Policy and Practice 104, 195208. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tra.2017.04.025.Google Scholar
Lira, BM and Paez, A (2021) Do drivers dream of walking? An investigation of travel mode dissonance from the perspective of affective values. Journal of Transport and Health 20, 101015. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jth.2021.101015.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liu, Q, Chen, CL and Cao, M (2021) Exploring the relationship between the commuting experience and hedonic and eudaimonic well-being. Transportation Research Part D: Transport and Environment 99, 103026. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.trd.2021.103026.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marques, A, Peralta, M, Henriques-Neto, D, Frasquilho, D, Gouveira, ER and Gomez-Baya, D (2020) Active commuting and depression symptoms in adults: A systematic review. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 17(3), 1041. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17031041.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martin, A, Goryakin, Y and Suhrcke, M (2014) Does active commuting improve psychological wellbeing? Longitudinal evidence from eighteen waves of the British household panel survey. Preventive Medicine 69, 296303. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ypmed.2014.08.023.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marttila, J and Nupponen, R (2000) Health enhancing physical activity as perceived in interviews based on the theory of planned behaviour. Psychology & Health 15(5), 593608. https://doi.org/10.1080/08870440008405473.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Masi, CM, Chen, H-Y, Hawkley, LC and Cacioppo, JT (2011) A meta-analysis of interventions to reduce loneliness. Personality and Social Psychology Review 15(3), 219266. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868310377394.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mason, P, Curl, A and Kearns, A (2016) Domains and levels of physical activity are linked to adult mental health and wellbeing in deprived neighbourhoods: A cross-sectional study. Mental Health and Physical Activity 11, 1928. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.mhpa.2016.07.001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mattisson, K, Idris, AO, Cromley, E, Håkansson, C, Östergren, P-O and Jakobsson, K (2018) Modelling the association between health indicators and commute mode choice: A cross-sectional study in southern Sweden. Journal of Transport & Health 11, 110121. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jth.2018.10.012.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCarthy, S and Habib, MA (2018) Investigation of life satisfaction, travel, built environment and attitudes. Journal of Transport and Health 11, 1524. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jth.2018.09.007.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McMahan, EA and Estes, D (2011) Measuring lay conceptions of well-being: The beliefs about well-being scale. Journal of Happiness Studies 12(2), 267287. https://doi.org/10.1007/S10902-010-9194-X.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moher, D, Liberati, A, Tetzlaff, J, Altman, DG and Group, P (2009) Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: The PRISMA statement. Annals of Internal Medicine 151, 264269. https://doi.org/10.7717/peerj.15039/fig-1.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Molina-García, J, Castillo, I and Sallis, JF (2010) Psychosocial and environmental correlates of active commuting for university students. Preventive Medicine: An International Journal Devoted to Practice and Theory 51(2), 136138. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ypmed.2010.05.009.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morris, EA and Guerra, E (2015) Mood and mode: Does how we travel affect how we feel? Transportation 42(1), 2543. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11116-014-9521-x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Muñiz, C, Rodríguez, P and Suárez, MJ (2021) An insight into physical activity across domains: Implications for depression and perceived health. European Sport Management Quarterly 23, 508. https://doi.org/10.1080/16184742.2021.1890801.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Murphy, B, Herrman, H, Hawthorne, G, Pinzone, T and Evert, H (2000) Australiano WHOQoL instruments: User’s manual and interpretation guide.Google Scholar
Mutrie, N, Carney, C, Blamey, A, Crawford, F, Aitchison, T and Whitelaw, A (2002) ‘Walk in to work out’: A randomised controlled trial of a self help intervention to promote active commuting. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health 56(6), 407412. https://doi.org/10.1136/jech.56.6.407.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mutrie, N, Cerney, C, Blamey, A, Whitelaw, A, Crawford, F and Aitchison, T (2000) Part ii: Interdisciplinary. Journal of Sports Sciences 18(1), 1221. https://doi.org/10.1080/026404100365252.Google Scholar
Mytton, OT, Panter, J and Ogilvie, D (2016) Longitudinal associations of active commuting with wellbeing and sickness absence. Preventive Medicine 84, 1926. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ypmed.2015.12.010.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, KL, Davis, JE and Corbett, CF (2022) Sleep quality: An evolutionary concept analysis. Nursing Forum 57(1), 144151. https://doi.org/10.1111/NUF.12659.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neumeier, LM, Loidl, M, Reich, B, Fernandez La Puente de Battre, MD, Kissel, CK, Templin, C, Schmied, C, Niebauer, J and Niederseer, D (2020) Effects of active commuting on health-related quality of life and sickness-related absence. Scandinavian Journal of Medicine & Science in Sports 30( Suppl 1), 3140. https://doi.org/10.1111/sms.13667.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nianogo, RA and Arah, OA (2015) Agent-based modeling of noncommunicable diseases: A systematic review. American Journal of Public Health 105(3), e20. https://doi.org/10.2105/AJPH.2014.302426.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
OCEBM Levels of Evidence Working Group (2011) The Oxford levels of evidence 2 (Web Page). Available at https://www.cebm.ox.ac.uk/resources/levels-of-evidence/ocebm-levels-of-evidence (accessed 22 January 2023).Google Scholar
Ogilvie, D, Bull, F, Powell, J, Cooper, AR, Brand, C, Mutrie, N, Preston, J and Rutter, H (2011) An applied ecological framework for evaluating infrastructure to promote walking and cycling: The iconnect study. American Journal of Public Health 101(3), 473481. https://doi.org/10.2105/AJPH.2010.198002.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ogilvie, D, Griffin, S, Jones, A, Mackett, R, Guell, C, Panter, J, Jones, N, Cohn, S, Yang, L and Chapman, C (2010) Commuting and health in Cambridge: A study of a ‘natural experiment’ in the provision of new transport infrastructure. BMC Public Health 10, 703. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2458-10-703.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ohta, M, Mizoue, T, Mishima, N and Ikeda, M (2007) Effect of the physical activities in leisure time and commuting to work on mental health. Journal of Occupational Health 49(1), 4652. https://doi.org/10.1539/joh.49.46.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olsson, LE, Gärling, T, Ettema, D, Friman, M and Fujii, S (2013) Happiness and satisfaction with work commute. Social Indicators Research 111(1), 255263. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11205-012-0003-2.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Page, NC and Nilsson, VO (2017) Active commuting: Workplace health promotion for improved employee well-being and organizational behavior. Frontiers in Psychology 7, 1994. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01994.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Panter, JR and Jones, A (2010) Attitudes and the environment as determinants of active travel in adults: What do and don’t we know? Journal of Physical Activity and Health 7, 551561.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parloff, MB, Kelman, HC and Frank, JD (1954) Comfort, effectiveness, and self-awareness as criteria of improvement in psychotherapy. The American Journal of Psychiatry 111(5), 343352. https://doi.org/10.1176/AJP.111.5.343.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paudel, S, Owen, AJ and Smith, BJ (2021) Exploration of physical activity barriers and facilitators among adults in Kathmandu, Nepal. Qualitative Health Research 31(6), 11831195. https://doi.org/10.1177/1049732321993096.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pearlin, LI and Schooler, C (1978) The structure of coping. Journal of Health and Social Behavior 19(1), 221. https://doi.org/10.2307/2136319.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pearson, JE (1986) The definition and measurement of social support. Journal of Counseling & Development 64(6), 390395. https://doi.org/10.1002/j.1556-6676.1986.tb01144.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Physical Activity Guidelines Advisory Committee (2018) 2018 Physical Activity Guidelines Advisory Committee Scientific Report. Washington, DC: US Department of Health and Human Services.Google Scholar
Position Statement: Addressing Social Isolation and Loneliness and the Power of Human Connection (2022) Available at https://www.gilc.global/ (accessed 27 March 2023).Google Scholar
Pucci, G, Reis, RS, Rech, CR and Hallal, PC (2012) Quality of life and physical activity among adults: Population-based study in Brazilian adults. Quality of Life Research 21(9), 15371543. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11136-011-0083-5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rasciute, S and Downward, P (2010) Health or happiness? What is the impact of physical activity on the individual? Kyklos 63(2), 256270.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Redding, CA, Mundorf, N, Kobayashi, H, Brick, L, Horiuchi, S, Paiva, AL and Prochaska, JO (2014) Sustainable transportation stage of change, decisional balance, and self-efficacy scale development and validation in two university samples. International Journal of Environmental Health Research 25(3), 241253. https://doi.org/10.1080/09603123.2014.938025.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosenberg, M (1965) Rosenberg self-esteem scale. Journal of Religion and Health 61, 52.Google Scholar
Rothbaum, F, Weisz, JR and Snyder, SS (1982) Changing the world and changing the self: A two-process model of perceived control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 42(1), 537. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.42.1.5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rovniak, LS, Sallis, JF, Saelens, BE, Frank, LD, Marshall, SJ, Norman, GJ, Conway, TL, Cain, KL and Hovell, MF (2010) Adults’ physical activity patterns across life domains: Cluster analysis with replication. Health Psychology 29(5), 496505. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020428.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Russell, DW (1996) UCLA loneliness scale (version 3): Reliability, validity, and factor structure. Journal of Personality Assessment 66(1), 2040.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Russell, JA and Lanius, UF (1984) Adaptation level and the affective appraisal of environments. Journal of Environmental Psychology 4(2), 119135. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0272-4944(84)80029-8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ryan, JC, Viana, JN, Sellak, H, Gondalia, S and O’Callaghan, N (2021) Defining precision health: A scoping review protocol. BMJ Open 11(2), e044663. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmjopen-2020-044663.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ryff, CD (1989) Happiness is everything, or is it? Explorations on the meaning of psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 57(6), 10691081. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.57.6.1069.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sattler, MC, Färber, T, Traußnig, K, Köberl, G, Paier, C, Dietz, P and van Poppel, MNM (2020) Cross-sectional association between active commuting and perceived commuting stress in Austrian adults: Results from the HOTway study. Mental Health and Physical Activity 19, 100356. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.mhpa.2020.100356.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scarabottolo, CC, Cyrino, ES, Nakamura, PM, Tebar, WR, Canhin, D, Gobbo, LA and Christofaro, DGD (2019) Relationship of different domains of physical activity practice with health-related quality of life among community-dwelling older people: A cross-sectional study. BMJ Open 9(6), e027751. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmjopen-2018-027751.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scarabottolo, CC, Tebar, WR, Gobbo, LA, Ohara, D, Ferreira, AD, Canhin, D, da, S and Christofaro, DGD (2022) Analysis of different domains of physical activity with health-related quality of life in adults: 2-year cohort. Health and Quality of Life Outcomes 20, 19. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12955-022-01981-3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scott, AJ, Webb, TL, Martyn-St James, M, Rowse, G and Weich, S (2021) Improving sleep quality leads to better mental health: A meta-analysis of randomised controlled trials. Sleep Medicine Reviews 60, 101556. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.SMRV.2021.101556.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sener, IN and Lee, RJ (2017) Active travel behavior in a border region of Texas and New Mexico: Motivators, deterrents, and characteristics. Journal of Physical Activity and Health 14(8), 636645. https://doi.org/10.1123/jpah.2016-0503.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Singleton, PA (2018) How useful is travel-based multitasking? Evidence from commuters in Portland, Oregon. Transportation Research Record 2672(50), 1122. https://doi.org/10.1177/0361198118776151.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Singleton, PA (2019) Walking (and cycling) to well-being: Modal and other determinants of subjective well-being during the commute. Travel Behaviour and Society 16, 249261. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tbs.2018.02.005.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, JM (2012) Toward a better understanding of loneliness in community-dwelling older adults. Journal of Psychology: Interdisciplinary and Applied 146(3), 293311. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223980.2011.602132.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, O (2017) Commute well-being differences by mode: Evidence from Portland, Oregon, USA. Journal of Transport and Health 4, 246254. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jth.2016.08.005.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Southwick, SM, Bonanno, GA, Masten, AS, Panter-Brick, C and Yehuda, R (2014) Resilience definitions, theory, and challenges: Interdisciplinary perspectives. European Journal of Psychotraumatology 5(1), 25338. https://doi.org/10.3402/EJPT.V5.25338.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
St-Louis, E, Manaugh, K, Van Lierop, D, Student, P and El-Geneidy, A (2014) The happy commuter: A comparison of commuter satisfaction across modes. Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour 26, 160170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tannenbaum, C, Lexchin, J, Tamblyn, R and Romans, S (2009) Indicators for measuring mental health: Towards better surveillance. Healthcare Policy 5(2), e177. https://doi.org/10.12927/hcpol.2013.21180.Google ScholarPubMed
The EuroQoL Group (1990) EuroQol—A new facility for the measurement of health-related quality of life. Health Policy 16(3), 199208. https://doi.org/10.1016/0168-8510(90)90421-9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
The Whoqol Group (1998) The World Health Organization quality of life assessment (WHOQOL): Development and general psychometric properties. Social Science & Medicine 46(12), 15691585.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tin Tin, S, Woodward, A, Thornley, S and Ameratunga, S (2009) Cycling and walking to work in New Zealand, 1991–2006: Regional and individual differences, and pointers to effective interventions. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity 6, 64. https://doi.org/10.1186/1479-5868-6-64.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Dyck, D, Cardon, G, Deforche, B, Giles-Corti, B, Sallis, JF, Owen, N and De Bourdeaudhuij, I (2011) Environmental and psychosocial correlates of accelerometer-assessed and self-reported physical activity in Belgian adults. International Journal of Behavioral Medicine 18(3), 235245. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12529-010-9127-4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vancampfort, D, Smith, L, Stubbs, B, Swinnen, N, Firth, J, Schuch, FB and Koyanagi, A (2018) Associations between active travel and physical multi-morbidity in six low- and middle-income countries among community-dwelling older adults: A cross-sectional study. PLoS One 13(8), e0203277. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0203277.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, X, Yin, C and Shao, C (2021) Relationships among haze pollution, commuting behavior and life satisfaction: A quasi-longitudinal analysis. Transportation Research Part D: Transport and Environment 92, 102723. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.trd.2021.102723.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ware, JE, Kosinski, M and Keller, SD (1996) A 12-item short-form health survey: Construction of scales and preliminary tests of reliability and validity. Medical Care 34(3), 220233.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ware, JE and Sherbourne, CD (1992) The MOS 36-item short-form health survey (SF-36): I. Conceptual Framework and Item Selection. Care 30(6), 473483.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Watson, D, Clark, LA and Tellegen, A (1988) Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 54(6), 10631070. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wicks, C, Barton, J, Orbell, S and Andrews, L (2022) Psychological benefits of outdoor physical activity in natural versus urban environments: A systematic review and meta-analysis of experimental studies. Applied Psychology: Health and Well-Being 14(3), 10371061. https://doi.org/10.1111/aphw.12353.Google ScholarPubMed
Wilson, K, Elliott, SJ, Eyles, JD and Keller-Olaman, SJ (2007) Factors affecting change over time in self-reported health. Canadian Journal of Public Health 98(2), 154158. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03404329/METRICS.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
World Health Organization (2022) Mental health. Available at https://www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/mental-health-strengthening-our-response (accessed 21 March 2023).Google Scholar
World Health Organization (2023) Stress. Available at https://www.who.int/news-room/questions-and-answers/item/stress (accessed 24 February 2023).Google Scholar
Yang, Y, Langellier, BA, Stankov, I, Purtle, J, Nelson, KL and Diez Roux, AV (2019) Examining the possible impact of daily transport on depression among older adults using an agent-based model. Aging & Mental Health 23(6), 743751. https://doi.org/10.1080/13607863.2018.1450832.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zijlema, WL, Avila-Palencia, I, Triguero-Mas, M, Gidlow, C, Maas, J, Kruize, H, Andrusaityte, S, Grazuleviciene, R and Nieuwenhuijsen, MJ (2018) Active commuting through natural environments is associated with better mental health: Results from the PHENOTYPE project. Environment International 121, 721727. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.envint.2018.10.002.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Definitions of the included mental health outcomes

Figure 1

Figure 1. PRISMA 2020 flow diagram for new systematic reviews which included searches of databases and registers only.

Figure 2

Table 2. Summary of study characteristics and participants, ordered by study design and year of publication

Figure 3

Figure 2. Word clouds summarising the most common study-specific phrases found in articles included in the review are categorised as (a) related to outcomes or (b) related to covariates. The size of the text is proportional to the relative number of occurrences of the phrase.

Figure 4

Figure 3. The study distribution in the review is projected onto the world map (left panel), while European studies are shown in the enlargement (right panel). The size of the symbols is related to the number of participants. The location of each symbol is centred on the centre of the city, region or country from which participants were drawn.

Figure 5

Figure 4. Albatross plots mapping the imputed effect size of each study for which data were available regarding differences in outcome between study participants selecting active and motorised mobility for transport needs in daily life. Curved lines join points with equal effect sizes (labelled 0.1, 0.25, 0.5). The x-axis indicates the reported p-value. Points to the right of the origin (null p-value) show a benefit for active mobility, and those to the left show a benefit for motorised transport. The y-axis shows the number of participants on a logarithmic scale. Points are positioned conservatively when only the relationship to a significance threshold is reported (i.e., p < 0.05 is treated as p = 0.05). Results are divided between four panels for easier visualisation, grouping outcomes by alphabetical order. Symbol shapes indicate study design (legend in the bottom right-hand panel). Labels indicate the first three letters of the first author’s name and the 2-digit year of publication.

Figure 6

Table 3. Summary of findings relating to the effect of active mobility on aspects of mental health, ordered by mental health variable, study design, and year of publication

Supplementary material: File

Scrivano et al. supplementary material 1
Download undefined(File)
File 76.7 KB
Supplementary material: File

Scrivano et al. supplementary material 2
Download undefined(File)
File 27.3 KB
Supplementary material: File

Scrivano et al. supplementary material 3
Download undefined(File)
File 82.5 KB
Supplementary material: File

Scrivano et al. supplementary material 4
Download undefined(File)
File 194.9 KB

Author comment: Active mobility and mental health: A scoping review towards a healthier world — R0/PR1

Comments

31/05/2023

To the attention of Global Mental Health Editor-in-Chief

Dear Sir/Madam,

We wish to submit a research manuscript entitled “Active Mobility and Mental Health: A Scoping Review towards a Healthier and Greener World” for publishing in your esteemed journal.

First of its kind, this comprehensive review examines the existing literature on active transportation modes' benefits in terms of mental health outcomes. The extended time range and the number of variables covered explain the extensive length of this work.

From the perspective of health promotion and sustainable development, we selected the Global Mental Health journal because the literature includes many countries worldwide and because the potential impact of the present paper wishes to support international and interdisciplinary collaborations.

All of this manuscript’s authors (Luana Scrivano, Alessia Tessari, Samuele Maria Marcora, and David Neil Manners) agreed to submit the manuscript to the journal Global Mental Health. We also decided to transfer the copyright from the authors to the journal. The manuscript has been prepared per the journal’s guidelines and checked for language correction.

We confirm this work is original, and the manuscript is not currently considered for publication elsewhere.

Please address all the correspondence on this manuscript to me at luana.scrivano2@unibo.it.

Thanking you

Sincerely

Luana Scrivano

Review: Active mobility and mental health: A scoping review towards a healthier world — R0/PR2

Conflict of interest statement

Reviewer declares none.

Comments

Overview:

On the whole I found this coherent and a good literature overview, structured well enough for people who are only interested in one sub topic to find the information they wanted, although there are plenty of minor issues and one puzzle.

I don’t know how novel this is, but it does a good job of being transparent and rigorous and a decent overview. Also strongly agree with conclusions, that RCTs would be useful and more definitive. The 4 figures in main text are interesting. There is a problem with Figures 4 (or 5?) – 10, which is my only major comment.

MAJOR

I don’t see Figures 4-10, as described in this sentence

“Finally, the implicit effect sizes of statistically assessed outcomes are shown in Figures 4 to 10.”

Figure 4 seem to be something else. Figures 5-10 aren’t discussed again. So that all is confusing.

MINOR:

Progressing in abstract should be Progress. I also think a better word might be ‘improvement’ or simply ‘links to’

Impacts, “related” on pg 3 line 15: should be ‘relevant’

There are minor issues like missing spaces between narrative text and references(like so)

I appreciate the clear description of how the search phrases were developed.

“The extracted were used to describe characteristics”

Needs a noun, maybe should be

The extracted data were used to describe characteristics

/or/

The extractions were used to describe characteristics

cross-sectional case-control studies : these are 2 different study designs. Should that be “cross-sectional or case-control studies”

similarly, should put a comma to make phrase

(non-randomized controlled, cohort or longitudinal follow-up studies)

Is data plural or singular in this article? Line 55 pg 6

The albatross plots were an interesting way to visualise the findings. On balance I think I liked them, showed difference between RCTs & X sectional quite effectively.

Eudaimonia is an uncommon word in English; would help to define it before page 10, at 1st use ideally. It gets defined again a few more times after pg 10, probably not all necessary.

I’m not sure the authors did enough to explain that people who use public transport inherently walk more; to get to and from the bus. Indeed, having a bus pass is a good predictor of higher physical activity. That’s why public transport use gets mixed up with calculating time spent in active mobility. Increasingly popular Use of hire electric scooters & bicycles is going to complicate the picture, too.

I wouldn’t be as confident as authors about the cause and effect relationship of AT-AC-AM & good mental health; maybe the relationship runs the other way, good MH helps make AT-AC-AM easier. We need more RCTs, especially some more recent ones!

OTHER

Someone is doing an IPD meta-analysis (American PhD student, I think) about active travel and health outcomes, you may want to check syst-review protocol registries to see what they are doing, their progress.

Could incorporate wider environmental and social benefits of more people not moving around urban environments in high pollution vehicles, and lower risk of traffic accidents (although that may not be proven, and is complicated by things like high speed rental electric scooters).

Age-sex relationship: may be changing. What people did in past (social norms then) may not predict patterns in future; long distance female cyclists have become more common, for instance.

Review: Active mobility and mental health: A scoping review towards a healthier world — R0/PR3

Conflict of interest statement

Reviewer declares none.

Comments

PREVIEW

Active Mobility and Mental Health: A Scoping Review towards a Healthier and Greener World

SUMMARY

The topic of this scoping review is highly relevant and needed. I’m sure once it will be published, a lot of researchers would use it as a starting point to define future research directions. So, really happy to see this!

My main concerns are the over-promising Introduction, the lack of specificity in Methods, and a missing deeper Discussion in limitations and implications of the review. Find some suggestions in my comments below.

GENERAL COMMENTS

- Title & Introduction: the list of aims seems really ambitious and I’m not sure all of them are achieved (e.g., I haven’t seen any framework as a result of this review). The same happens with the second part of the title (‘A Scoping Review towards a Healthier and Greener World’). The review is focused on mental health and well-being and does not have that much emphasis on green space (assuming that the word ‘greener’ in the title refers to green space). I think the authors would guide better the readers avoiding over-promises in the title and the introduction of the paper. I suggest to re-write the title and the aims part of the Introduction being a bit more accurate and aligned with what is actually presented in the paper.

- Search strategy:

o it would be good to know the date when each source was last searched or consulted and if any filters and limits were used in the search.

o Could the authors be a bit more specific in the methods they used to decide whether a study met the inclusion criteria? Did they used a form? The text says “The same author (LS) conducted the full-text assessment, and the inclusion of potential studies was agreed upon by consensus with the others (DNM, AT, SMM).” If I’m understanding this properly, only one person read the papers, how the agreement about the inclusion of studies was done then?

- Study quality and risk of bias: the authors mention the ‘Oxford Centre for Evidence-Based Medicine classification’ but it is not very clear to me how this classification was used. I suggest to re-write the paragraph being more specific and clearer about if this classification was the actual tool used to assess the quality and risk of bias and if so, how it was used (e.g. it was used by one reviewer, there was any agreement process with other reviewers, etc).

- Descriptive analytical synthesis: it would be good if the authors could extend a bit more on the explanation of the ‘descriptive-analytical’ method for those readers who are not familiar with this term. Any literature reference would be helpful too.

- Mapping of terminology: it seems the authors decided to adopt ‘active mobility’ as the term they prefer to use. I suggest including a statement somewhere in the text, maybe this section, in which they explain their election and why they made it.

- Limitations: I miss a deeper discussion of the limitations of the evidence included in the review. What are the implications of the limitations identified (e.g., Mostly cross-sectional studies, self-reported methods)? The authors also need to discuss limitations of the review processes used.

- Future perspective: this section mainly discusses implications for future research. Any implication of the results for practice and policy?

SPECIFIC COMMENTS

- Appendix B uses a lot of acronyms in titles (e.g. MH, AM). They might seem self-explanatory, but I would recommend using the full word(s). If the authors prefer not to, make sure the acronyms are properly detailed in the document.

Recommendation: Active mobility and mental health: A scoping review towards a healthier world — R0/PR4

Comments

No accompanying comment.

Decision: Active mobility and mental health: A scoping review towards a healthier world — R0/PR5

Comments

No accompanying comment.

Author comment: Active mobility and mental health: A scoping review towards a healthier world — R1/PR6

Comments

31/05/2023

To the attention of Global Mental Health Editor-in-Chief

Dear Sir/Madam,

We wish to submit a research manuscript entitled “Active Mobility and Mental Health: A Scoping Review towards a Healthier and Greener World” for publishing in your esteemed journal.

First of its kind, this comprehensive review examines the existing literature on active transportation modes' benefits in terms of mental health outcomes. The extended time range and the number of variables covered explain the extensive length of this work.

From the perspective of health promotion and sustainable development, we selected the Global Mental Health journal because the literature includes many countries worldwide and because the potential impact of the present paper wishes to support international and interdisciplinary collaborations.

We declare that all this manuscript’s authors (Luana Scrivano, Alessia Tessari, Samuele Maria Marcora, and David Neil Manners) agreed to submit the manuscript to the journal Global Mental Health. We also agreed to transfer the copyright from the authors to the journal. The manuscript has been prepared per the journal’s guidelines and checked for language correction.

We confirm this work is original, and the manuscript is not currently considered for publication elsewhere.

Please address all the correspondence on this manuscript to me at luana.scrivano2@unibo.it.

Thanking you

Sincerely

Luana Scrivano

Review: Active mobility and mental health: A scoping review towards a healthier world — R1/PR7

Conflict of interest statement

Reviewer declares none.

Comments

Satisfactory, thanks.

Review: Active mobility and mental health: A scoping review towards a healthier world — R1/PR8

Conflict of interest statement

Reviewer declares none.

Comments

I am pretty happy with how the authors have addressed my comments. Willing to see it published! All the best.

Recommendation: Active mobility and mental health: A scoping review towards a healthier world — R1/PR9

Comments

No accompanying comment.

Decision: Active mobility and mental health: A scoping review towards a healthier world — R1/PR10

Comments

No accompanying comment.