Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-g7gxr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-14T04:21:01.316Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Cognitive bias in professional hand hygiene and feedback: A national online-survey on overconfidence in Germany

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 July 2019

Stefan Bushuven*
Affiliation:
Institute for Anesthesiology, Intensive Care, Emergency Medicine and Pain Therapy, Hegau-Bodensee Hospital, Singen, Germany Institute for Hospital Hygiene and Infection Prevention, Hegau-Bodensee Hospital, Radolfzell, Germany Institute for Didactics and Educational Research in Medicine, Clinic of the University Munich, LMU Munich, Germany
Marc Weidenbusch
Affiliation:
Institute for Didactics and Educational Research in Medicine, Clinic of the University Munich, LMU Munich, Germany
Stefan Mohr
Affiliation:
Clinic for Anesthesiology of the University Hospital of Heidelberg, Heidelberg, Germany
Achilles Delis
Affiliation:
Clinic and Policlinic for Anesthesiology and operative Intensive Care, University Hospital of Bonn, Bonn, Germany
Martin R. Fischer
Affiliation:
Institute for Didactics and Educational Research in Medicine, Clinic of the University Munich, LMU Munich, Germany
Jana Juenger
Affiliation:
Institute for Medical and Pharmaceutical Examinations, Mainz, Germany
Markus Dettenkofer
Affiliation:
Institute for Hospital Hygiene and Infection Prevention, Hegau-Bodensee Hospital, Radolfzell, Germany
*
Author for correspondence: Dr Stefan Bushuven, DESA, EDIC, Institute for Anesthesiology, Intensive Care, Emergency Medicine and Pain Therapy, Hegau-Bodensee Hospital Singen, 78224 Singen, Germany. Email: S.Bushuven@gmx.de
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

To evaluate learning motivation barriers in infection control and feedback competences, we conducted a national online survey in Germany. Among 767 healthcare workers, overconfidence effects could be detected independent from age, gender, profession, education, and hospital-size. The identified effects may impair learning motivation relevant for supervisors and educators in infection control.

Type
Concise Communication
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Society for Healthcare Epidemiology of America 2019

Training in hand hygiene and professional feedback are core elements of infection prevention.Reference Schwappach and Gehring1 According to the World Health Organization (WHO), adherence among healthcare providers to recommended hand hygiene procedures ranges from 5% to 89%, with an overall average of 38.7%. To provide these competencies and intensify multiprofessional collaboration, regular training in hygiene proficiencies and team dynamics is necessary.2 Unfortunately, motivation to participate in these programs is low among healthcare workers such as medical students.Reference Qasmi, Mahmood Shah, Wakil and Pirzada3 As a consequence, hospitals use extrinsic motivation to ensure participation. Unfortunately, participation alone does not guarantee learning, and learning does not necessarily ensure good behavior in everyday life.

In a multicenter study, we identified an overplacement effect, a subtype of the overconfidence effect, as a possible barrier to incentive motivationReference Mubeen and Reid4 to attend to infection prevention and communication programs.Reference Bushuven, Juenger, Moeltner and Dettenkofer5 Overconfidence effects are impressive, popular and common heuristic errors found in multiple situations.Reference Dunning, Heath and Suls6, Reference Moore and Healy7 The effect is divided into 3 subeffectsReference Moore and Healy7:

  1. 1. Absolute overconfidence (overestimation): the belief that one is better than is quantifiable.

  2. 2. Relative overconfidence (overplacement): the belief that one is better than the median of a group.

  3. 3. Overprecision – the excessive belief concerning the correctness of one’s own assessment.

In the previously reported regional study on overconfidence in infection prevention in a communal healthcare association,Reference Bushuven, Juenger, Moeltner and Dettenkofer5 selection bias was one of the main limiting factors. Therefore, we tested our hypothesis in a nationwide group of participants. This article reports the results of our approach and adds more insight into heuristic influence on learning motivation in infection prevention.

Methods

The cross-sectional anonymous online-study was conducted over 12 months from 2017 to 2018. We used a previously tested a 6-point Likert questionnaire (Subjective Assessment of Training in Infection Prevention Skills version 2) with 61 items. This questionnaire was checked in a test phase for reliability and validity using factor analysis and varimax rotation. Items consisted of short statements like “I am well educated in providing feedback.” Answers were chosen from a list of 0 “completely disagree” to 5 “completely agree.”

After consultation with the institutional review board (State Medical Chamber Baden-Wuerttemberg, Stuttgart), the questionnaire was provided by e-mail by Enuvo GmbH Zuerich. Internet protocol (IP) addresses were blinded to the investigators. Participant recruitment was accomplished using announcements in social media (eg, Facebook, LinkedIn); using different Internet forums of medical professionals; and by e-mail to hospital hygiene departments, medical academies, and risk managers. Furthermore, members of the QuMIK (Quality and Management in Hospitals) network of Baden-Wuerttemberg were requested by e-mail to forward the survey link.

A statistical analysis of the completed forms was conducted with Excel software (Microsoft, Redmond, WA) and XLSTAT software (Addinsoft SARL, NY) using Mann-Whitney U, Friedman, and Wilcoxon tests. Significance was set at 0.05, with adjustments for multiple testing. A correlation analysis was conducted with Spearman rank correlation. Because of the anticipated selection bias for this study, the central questions were clustered at the beginning of the survey for a “completer/dropout” comparison. Subgroup analyses focused on age, sex, profession, education level, and workplace.

Results

Participants

Of 767 participants in >20 professions, 421 completed the survey. Of these, 63.9% were female. Most completers (45.6%) were 26–40 years old, and 15.9% were undergraduates. Professions of participants included physicians (36.6%), nurses (22.6%), paramedics (11.6%), and surgical nurses (11.2%). Also, 74.5% were employed in a hospital and 59.1% worked at tertiary-care hospitals.

Main results

Participants assessed their own level of education and adherence to clinical hand hygiene protocols to be better than those of students, colleagues of the same profession, and supervisors (Table 1 and Fig. 1). Effect sizes were moderate (Dz = 0.3–0.5) to strong (Dz > 0.5) for these findings. Recognition of indications of hand hygiene showed deficits for completers with “better than average” self-assessments: 15.1% did not recognize the need for hand hygiene before connecting a urinary catheter to a collection bag. Moreover, 21.5% of participants failed to identify the preparation of oral medication as an aseptic procedure, and 1.5% of participants failed to do so before vascular access. For ‘weaker’ indications, half of the participants believing themselves to have an above-average education found no indication for hand hygiene after using computer keyboards or after food consumption.

Table 1. Statistical Analysis of Selected Questionnaire Times with Mean, Standard Deviation (SD), Significance (P) and Intraindividual Effect Size (Cohen’s Dz) with Low (<0.3), Moderate (0.3–0.5) and Strong (>0.5) Effects

Fig. 1. Results of the assessment of one’s own and other’s education in hand hygiene and feedback competences. Assessment of one’s own skills was significantly higher in all item-comparisons.

Self-assessment correlated with subjective belief in participants’ own protocol adherence (P < .0001; r = −0.38). Participants with high self-assessments reported having skipped only a few hand hygiene occasions in the prior work week. Only 2.9% of the participants chose the realistic number of >10 per week. In contrast, 34.3% reported 100% compliance without errors or omissions.

High self-assessment in hand-hygiene significantly positively correlated with ratings of one’s own proficiency (r = 0.14–0.20) and the assessment of colleagues in the same profession (r = 0.12–0.20).

The ability to identify WHO criteria 1–3 showed no significant correlation with correct identification and self-assessment of education in hand hygiene. The only exception was the removal of contaminated clothes, which showed a weak positive correlation (P = .004; r = 0.14).

Self-assessment of providing feedback correlated positively with that for members of one’s own profession (r = 0.21–0.25) but not of other professions. Concerning feedback reception proficiency, a weak positive correlation between self-assessment and ratings of supervisors was detected (r = 0.10).

Regarding feedback provision and reception, half of the participants who rated themselves as having an above-average education reported low adherence to classical feedback rules. Reported corrective intervention when participants witnessed someone else skipping a hand hygiene occasion was either low or unrealistic: Only 8.6% of participants were realistic in answering that they omitted >10 feedback situationsReference Schwappach and Gehring1 per week. On the contrary, 39.7% reported having spoken up after every skipped occasion in their prior work week.

Subgroup analysis

Gender

There were no differences with respect to gender in assessment of one’s own or of others’ compliance and education. Women ranked the hand hygiene compliance of supervisors (P = .023) and feedback skills of students to be better (P = .005) than male participants did.

Age

We detected significant differences between participants below and above 40 years of age: Younger medical professionals rated themselves as better educated in feedback reception skills than did older participants (P < .001). Furthermore, younger persons rated students to be better educated in hygiene (P = .004) and to have better feedback reception skills (P = .002).

Professions

Four main professions were represented: physicians, nurses, paramedics and surgical nurses. Between them, no differences in their answers could be detected.

Students

Students (n = 67) showed higher ratings for their own capabilities in hand hygiene and feedback than colleagues and supervisors (P < .05). There was no correlation between the years of education and self-assessment or ratings of others (P > .05).

Workplace

No differences between participants working in primary, secondary, tertiary or university hospitals or in out-of-hospital settings were detected.

Completers versus dropouts

A comparison of completers versus dropouts showed no significant difference in the assessments of others (P > .05). For self-estimation items, completers rated themselves better than dropouts did (P = .028). Dropouts who terminated the survey early (directly after self-assessment, items 1–5) rated themselves inferior to those who dropped out later (P = .002; r = 0.13).

Discussion

To our knowledge, this is the first investigation in Germany to test for absolute and relative overconfidence in hand hygiene and feedback competence in interprofessional and intersectoral healthcare workers.

Our results show overestimation of one’s own education in hand hygiene and feedback: despite high self-assessment, participants failed to identify relevant WHO indications for hand hygiene. They were not able to rate the realistic occurrence of skipped occasions, and they showed relevant gaps in knowledge and behavior for feedback techniques.Reference Ramani and Krackov8

Second, we detected overplacement consistent with prior studies,Reference Bushuven, Juenger, Moeltner and Dettenkofer5Reference Moore and Healy7 namely that participants rated themselves to be of above-average education and proficiency. Overplacement was higher in the national survey than in the data from the regional study.Reference Bushuven, Juenger, Moeltner and Dettenkofer5

Neither overplacement nor overestimation in students and supervisors were dependent on age, working place, profession, or education level. Previous studies have shown differences along gender lines in assessment of one’s own skills, with women tending to rate themselves lower than men (although both are overconfident).Reference Blanch, Hall, Roter and Frankel9 In this study, this tendency was not detected.

These effects, together with the low tendency to speak up in case of erroneous or skipped hand disinfections, indicates that risks in infections prevention and medical education may affect patient safety. First, the detection of overestimation indicates that hand hygiene and feedback are mistakenly perceived to be “easy” skills. Challenging skills normally generate underestimation.Reference Moore and Healy7 Trainings in subjectively easy abilities may therefore be considered time-consuming and a waste of resources. Training motivation mainly depends on 3 aspects: value, self-efficacy, and controllability.Reference Pelaccia and Viau10 With a subjectively high level of education, the value of further training may be estimated to be low, especially if ‘others’ are perceived to be in need of more in-depth education.

Second, both effects emerge early in education and are independent of gender, age, profession and workplace. To compensate for overconfidence and collaboration impairment, educators could use interprofessional teaching formats to address all professionals, regardless of their educational level. Due to the high resistance of overconfidence, classical learning formats are known to fail to compensate for it. This indicates the need for reflexive and individual training.

Third, witnessed errors in hygienic hand disinfection were ignored, although feedback training was perceived to be satisfactory. This finding is consistent with results reported by Schwappach et al.Reference Schwappach and Gehring1 Although reflection by feedback is a core aspect of learning, feedback may be perceived to be negative and even harmful in the presence of overconfidence. With overconfidence developing in early stages of professional education, it seems to be advisable to integrate feedback and self-reflection as early as possible in medical education of infection prevention.

In addition, the missing correlation between self-assessment and quality of practice shows that self-assessment in infection control may be flawed and therefore unsuitable for the measurement of realistic training demands.

This study has several limitations including selection bias, which is a typical limitation of all surveys. Although the selection bias never can be ruled out completely, this study combined with the preceding workReference Bushuven, Juenger, Moeltner and Dettenkofer5 and findings in other settingsReference Dunning, Heath and Suls6 increase credibility and generalizability of the hypothesis. The comparison of drop-outs and completers as a surrogate parameter for “interest” or “motivation” is questionable because one’s attitude toward completing a survey is not completely related to interest but may also be linked to other social factors (eg, politeness).

In conclusion, our results show relative and absolute overconfidence for infection prevention among healthcare providers. The effects were independent of age, profession, educational level, working place and gender. Self-assessment did not correlate with actual education in hand hygiene and feedback. Although we found some limitations, flawed self-assessment and overconfidence in infection prevention competences should be considered relevant elements in the development and design of training and curricula for healthcare providers.

Author ORCIDs

Stefan Bushuven 0000-0001-6272-0714

Acknowledgments

We thank Dr. Alexandra Patin, PhD for proofreading the manuscript.

Financial support

No financial support was provided relevant to this article.

Conflicts of interest

All authors report no conflicts of interest relevant to this article.

References

Schwappach, DL, Gehring, K. Silence that can be dangerous: a vignette study to assess healthcare professionals’ likelihood of speaking up about safety concerns. PLoS One 2014;9(8):e104720.10.1371/journal.pone.0104720CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
WHO Guidelines on Hand Hygiene in Health Care: First Global Patient Safety Challenge Clean Care Is Safer Care. Geneva: World Health Organization; 2009.Google Scholar
Qasmi, SA, Mahmood Shah, SM, Wakil, HYI, Pirzada, S. Guiding hand hygiene interventions among future healthcare workers: implications of knowledge, attitudes, and social influences. Am J Infect Control 2018;46:10261031.10.1016/j.ajic.2018.02.019CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mubeen, S, Reid, N. The measurement of motivation with science students. Eur J Educ Res 2014;3:129144.10.12973/eu-jer.3.3.129CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bushuven, S, Juenger, J, Moeltner, A, Dettenkofer, M. Overconfidence in infection control proficiency. Am J Infect Control 2019;47:545550.10.1016/j.ajic.2018.10.022CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunning, D, Heath, C, Suls, JM. Flawed self-assessment: implications forhealth, education and the workplace. Psych Sci Public Interest 2004;5: 24769–106.10.1111/j.1529-1006.2004.00018.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moore, DA, Healy, PJ. The trouble with overconfidence. Psychol Rev 2008;115:502517.10.1037/0033-295X.115.2.502CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ramani, S, Krackov, SK. Twelve tips for giving feedback effectively in the clinical environment. Med Teach 2012;34:787791.10.3109/0142159X.2012.684916CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blanch, DC, Hall, JA, Roter, DL, Frankel, RM. Medical student gender andissues of confidence. Patient Educ Couns 2008;72:374381.10.1016/j.pec.2008.05.021CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pelaccia, T, Viau, R. Motivation in medical education. Med Teach 2017;39:136140.10.1080/0142159X.2016.1248924CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Statistical Analysis of Selected Questionnaire Times with Mean, Standard Deviation (SD), Significance (P) and Intraindividual Effect Size (Cohen’s Dz) with Low (<0.3), Moderate (0.3–0.5) and Strong (>0.5) Effects

Figure 1

Fig. 1. Results of the assessment of one’s own and other’s education in hand hygiene and feedback competences. Assessment of one’s own skills was significantly higher in all item-comparisons.