Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-mlc7c Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-14T22:20:42.909Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Maternal speech to three-month-old infants in the United States and Japan*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 February 2009

Suero Toda*
Affiliation:
National Institute of Child Health and Human Development
Alan Fogel
Affiliation:
University of Utah
Masatoshi Kawai
Affiliation:
Nagoya University
*
NIH, Building 31, Room B2–B15, 9000 Rockville Pike, Bethesda, MD 20892, USA

Abstract

An American-Japanese comparison of maternal speech to 3-month-old infants is presented. Mother-infant dyads were videotaped in the laboratory, and the maternal speech was analysed by function and syntactic form. US mothers were more information-oriented than were Japanese mothers; they also used more question forms, especially yes/no questions. Japanese mothers were affect-oriented, and they used more nonsense, onomatopoeic sounds, baby talk, and babies' names. The differences between countries in maternal speech addressed to 3-month-olds appear to reflect characteristic culture-specific communicative styles as well as beliefs and values related to childrearing.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1990

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

*

Portions of this paper were presented at the Southeastern Conference on Human Development, 1988. The research was supported by a Visiting Fellowship at the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development to the first author. It was also funded by a Fulbright Senior Research Fellowship awarded to the second author, by the Department of Educational Psychology of Nagoya University, and by a Hatch Act grant from the Purdue University Agricultural Experiment Station. The authors wish to thank the coders, assistants, and subjects who participated in this study. We are grateful to E. Nwokah for her assistance in coding maternal speech. We thank M. Bornstein for comments.

References

REFERENCES

Azuma, H. (1986). Why study child development in Japan? In Stevenson, H., Azuma, H. & Hakuta, K. (eds), Child development and education in Japan. New York: Freeman.Google Scholar
Blount, B. (1972). Parental speech and language acquisition: some Luo and Samoan examples. Anthropological Linguistics 14. 119–30.Google Scholar
Blount, B. (1977). Ethnography and caretaker-child interaction. In Snow, C. & Ferguson, C. (eds). Talking to children: language input and acquisition. New York: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Bornstein, M., Azuma, H., Tamis-Lemonda, C. & Ogino, M. (1990). Mother and infant activity and interaction in Japan and in the United States. I. A comparative macroanalysis of naturalistic exchanges. International Journal of Behavioral Development.Google Scholar
Bornstein, M., Miyake, K. & Tamis-LeMonda, C. (1986). A cross-national study of mother and infant activities and interactions: some preliminary comparisons between Japan and the United States. Annual Report of the Research and Clinical Center for Child Development 1–12. Sapporo, Japan: University of Hokkaido Press.Google Scholar
Brazelton, T. B., Koslowski, B. & Main, M. (1974). The origins of reciprocity. In Lewis, M. & Rosenblum, L. (eds), The effect of the infant on its caregiver. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Brown, R. (1977). Introduction. In Snow, C. & Ferguson, C. (eds), Talking to children: language input and acquisition. New York: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Caudill, W. (1973). The influence of social structure and culture on human behavior in modern Japan. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 157. 240–57.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caudill, W. & Weinstein, H. (1969). Maternal care and infant behavior in Japan and America. Psychiatry 32. 1243.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chew, J. J. (1969). The structure of Japanese baby talk. Newsletter of the Association of Japanese Teachers 6. 417.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clancy, P. (1986). The acquisition of communicative style in Japanese. In Schieffelin, B. & Ochs, B. (eds). Language socialization across cultures. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Conroy, M., Hess, R., Azuma, H. & Kashiwagi, K. (1980). Maternal strategies for regulating children's behavior: Japanese and American families. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 11. 153–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Della Corte, M., Benedict, H. & Klein, D. (1983). The relationship of pragmatic dimensions of mothers' speech to the referential-expressive distinction. Journal of Child Language 10. 3543.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ferguson, C. (1977). Baby talk as a simplified register. In Snow, C. & Ferguson, C. (eds), Talking to children: language input and acquisition. New York: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Fisher, J. (1970). Linguistic socialization: Japan and the United States. In Hill, R. & Konig, R. (eds), Families in east and west: socialization process and kinship ties. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Fogel, A., Toda, S. & Kawai, M. (1988). Mother-infant face-to-face interaction in Japan and the United States: a laboratory comparison using 3-month-old infants. Developmental Psychology 24. 398406.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Folger, J. & Chapman, R. (1978). A pragmatic analysis of spontaneous imitations. Journal of Child Language 5. 2538.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kakuta, K. (1986). The search for cross-linguistic invariants and variation in language development. In Stevenson, H., Azuma, H. & Hakuta, K. (eds), Child development and education in Japan. New York: Freeman.Google Scholar
Kaye, K. (1980). Why we don't talk ‘baby talk’ to babies. Journal of Child Language 7. 489507.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keller, H. & Scholmerich, A. (1987). Infant vocalizations and parental rections during the first four months of life. Developmental Psychology 23. 62–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kojima, H. (1986 a). Childrearing concepts as a belief-value system of the society and the individual. In Stevenson, H., Azuma, H. & Hakuta, K. (eds), Child development and education in Japan. New York: Freeman.Google Scholar
Kojima, H. (1986 b). Japanese concepts of child development from the mid-17th to mid-19th century. International Journal of Behavioral Development 9. 315–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuno, S. (1986). The Japanese language. In Stevenson, H., Azuma, H. & Hakuta, K. (eds), Child development and education in Japan. New York: Freeman.Google Scholar
Morikawa, H., Shand, N. & Kosawa, Y. (1988). Maternal speech to prelingual infants in Japan and the United States: relationships among functions, forms and referents. Journal of Child Language 15. 237–56.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morsbach, H. (1973). Aspects of nonverbal communication in Japan. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 157. 262–77.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murray, L. & Trevarthen, C. (1986). The infant's role in mother-infant communications. Journal of Child Language 13. 1529.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Newport, E., Gleitman, H. & Gleitman, L. (1977). Mother, I'd rather do it myself: some effects and non-effects of maternal speech style. In Snow, C. & Ferguson, C. (eds), Talking to children. New York: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Otaki, M., Durrett, M., Richards, P., Nyquist, L. & Pennebaker, J. (1986). Maternal and infant behavior in Japan and America: a partial replication. Journal of Cross-cultural Psychology 17. 251–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Papousek, M., Papousek, H. & Bornstein, M. (1985). The naturalistic vocal environment of young infants: on the significance of homogeneity and variability in parental speech. In Field, T. & Fox, N. (eds), Social perception in infants. Norwood, N.J.: Ablex.Google Scholar
Penman, R., Cross, T., Milgrom-Friedman, J. & Meares, R. (1983). Mothers' speech to prelingual infants: a pragmatic analysis. Journal of Child Language 10. 1734.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Phillips, J. (1973). Syntax and vocabulary of mothers' speech to young children: age and sex comparisons. Child Development 44. 182–5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rondal, J. (1985). Adult-child interaction and the process of language acquisition. New York: Praeger.Google Scholar
Sachs, J. (1977). The adaptive significance of linguistic input to prelinguistic infants. In Snow, C. & Ferguson, C. (eds), Talking to children. New York: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Sengoku, T. (1983). Mother-child relationship in Japan and the United States through behavioral observation. The Journal of Perinatal Medicine 13. Supplement, 501–4 (in Japanese).Google Scholar
Shand, N. & Kosawa, Y. (1985). Japanese and American behavior types at three months: infants and infant-mother dyads. Infant Behavior and Development 8. 225–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snow, C. (1977 a). Mothers' speech research: from input to interaction. In Snow, C. & Ferguson, C. (eds), Talking to children. New York: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Snow, C. (1977 b). The development of conversation between mothers and babies. Journal of Child Language 4. 122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snow, C., Blauw, A. & Roosmalen, G. (1979). Talking and playing with babies: the role of ideologies of child-rearing. In Bullowa, M. (ed.), Before speech: the beginning of interpersonal communication. New York: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Stern, D. N., Spieker, S. & MacKain, K. (1982). Intonation contours as signals in maternal speech to pre-linguistic infants. Developmental Psychology 18. 727–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stevenson, M., Ver Hoeve, J., Roach, M. & Leavitt, L. (1986). The beginning of conversation: early patterns of mother-infant vocal responsiveness. Infant Behavior and Development 9. 423–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Woods, A., Fletcher, P. & Hughes, A. (1986). Statistics in language studies. Cambridge: C.U.P.CrossRefGoogle Scholar