Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-8bhkd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-10T12:25:52.563Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

An event-semantic account of spatial modification in the acquisition of Mandarin

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  15 February 2018

Xiangjun DENG*
Affiliation:
Research Centre for Language and Cognition, School of Foreign Languages, Shenzhen University, Nanhai Ave 3688, Shenzhen, Guangdong Province, P.R. China, 518060
Virginia YIP
Affiliation:
University of Cambridge–Chinese University of Hong Kong Joint Laboratory for Bilingualism Childhood Bilingualism Research Centre, Chinese University of Hong Kong The Chinese University of Hong Kong–Peking University–University System of Taiwan Joint Research Centre for Language and Human Complexity Department of Linguistics and Modern Languages, Chinese University of Hong Kong
*
*Corresponding author. E-mail: dengxj98@gmail.com

Abstract

This study investigates Mandarin-speaking children's knowledge of event semantics in interpreting spatial modifiers with zai ‘at’ after a posture verb or before a placement verb. The event-semantic principles investigated include subevent modification (Parsons, 1990) and aspect shift (Fong, 1997). We conducted an experimental study using modified forced choice, video choice, and elicited production techniques with five groups of children (two- to six-year-olds) and an adult control group. Three-year-olds were sensitive to the ambiguity of zai-PPs with placement verbs and posture verbs, suggesting guidance from principles of aspect shift and subevent modification. On the other hand, distributional properties of the input play a role in acquiring the interpretation and word order of zai: e.g., four-year-olds significantly differed from adults in accepting non-target V-zai sentences, as some verb classes can take postverbal prepositional phrases with zai while others cannot in adult usage.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2018 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bach, E. (1986). The algebra of events. Linguistics and Philosophy, 9, 516.Google Scholar
Cheung, S. L. (1991). The acquisition of locative constructions by Cantonese children. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Stanford University.Google Scholar
Chierchia, G., Crain, S., Guasti, M. T., Gualmini, A., & Meroni, L. (2001). The acquisition of disjunction: evidence for a grammatical view of scalar implicatures. In Do, A. H.-J., Domínguez, L., & Johansen, A. (Eds.), BUCLD 25 proceedings (pp. 157–68). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Cinque, G. (2010). Mapping spatial PPs: an introduction. In Cinque, G. & Rizzi, L. (Eds.), Mapping spatial PPs: the cartography of syntactic structures (Vol. 6) (pp. 325). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Crain, S. (1991). Language acquisition in the absence of experience. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 14, 597650.Google Scholar
Crain, S. (2008). The interpretation of disjunction in Universal Grammar. Language and Speech, 51(1/2), 151–69.Google Scholar
Davidson, D. (1967). The logical form of action sentences. In Rescher, N. (Ed.), The logic of decision and action (pp. 8195). University of Pittsburgh Press.Google Scholar
Demuth, K., Machobane, M., & Moloi, F. (2003). Rules and construction effects in learning the argument structure of verbs. Journal of Child Language, 30, 797821.Google Scholar
Deng, X. (2010). The acquisition of the resultative verb compound in Mandarin Chinese. Unpublished MPhil thesis, The Chinese University of Hong Kong.Google Scholar
Deng, X. (2014). Space, events and language acquisition in Mandarin. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, The Chinese University of Hong Kong.Google Scholar
Deng, X., & Yip, V. (2015). The linguistic encoding of space in child Mandarin: a corpus-based study. Linguistics, 53(5), 1079–112.Google Scholar
Dowty, D. (1979). Word meaning and Montague Grammar: the semantics of verbs and times in Generative Semantics and in Montague's PTQ. Dordrecht: Reidel.Google Scholar
Fan, J. (1982). Lun jieci duanyu ‘zai + chusuo’ [On prepositional phrase ‘zai + location’]. Yuyan Yanjiu [Language Research], 2, 7186.Google Scholar
Fong, V. (1997). The order of things: what directional locatives denote. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Stanford University.Google Scholar
Gleitman, L., & Newport, E. (1995). The invention of language by children: environmental and biological influences on the acquisition of language. In Gleitman, L. & Liberman, M. (Eds.), Invitation to cognitive science (Vol. 1) (pp. 124). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Hsieh, M.-L. (2010). Post-verbal locative/directional phrases in child Mandarin: a longitudinal study. In Wilder, C. & Åfarli, T. A. (Eds.), Chinese matters: from grammar to first and second language acquisition (pp. 111–30). Trondheim: Tapir Academic Press.Google Scholar
Huang, J. C.-T. (1994). More on Chinese word order and parametric theory. In Lust, B., Suñer, M., & Whitman, J. (Eds.), Syntactic theory and first language acquisition: heads, projections, and learnability (pp. 1535). Hillsdale, NJ: L. Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Jakobson, R. (1968). Child language, aphasia and phonological universals. The Hague / Paris: Mouton.Google Scholar
Klein, W., Li, P., & Hendriks, H. (2000). Aspect and assertion in Mandarin Chinese. Natural Language& Linguistic Theory, 18, 723–70.Google Scholar
Li, C. N., & Thompson, S. A. (1981). Mandarin Chinese: a functional reference grammar. Berkeley: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Liu, F.-H. (2009). Aspect and the postverbal zai phrase in Mandarin Chinese. In Xing, J. Z. (Ed.), Studies of Chinese linguistics: functional approaches (pp. 103–29). Hong Kong University Press.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (2000). The CHILDES project: tools for analyzing talk, 3rd ed. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
McDaniel, D., & Cairns, H. S. (1996). Eliciting judgments of grammaticality and reference. In McDaniel, D., Mckee, C., & Cairns, H. S. (Eds.), Methods for assessing children's syntax (pp. 233–54). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Mulder, R., & Sybesma, R. (1992). Chinese is a VO language. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, 10(3), 439–76.Google Scholar
Nam, S. (2000). A typology of locatives and event composition in English. Language Research, 36(4), 689714.Google Scholar
Parsons, T. (1990). Events in the semantics of English: a study in subatomic semantics. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Peyraube, A. (1994). On the history of Chinese locative prepositions. Zhongguo Jingnei Yuyan Ji Yuyanxue [Chinese Languages and Linguistics], 2, 361–87.Google Scholar
Pinker, S. (1984). Language learnability and language development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Pustejovsky, J. (1991). The syntax of event structure. Cognition, 41, 4781.Google Scholar
Pustejovsky, J. (1995). The generative lexicon. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Rothstein, S. D. (2004). Structuring events: a study in the semantics of lexical aspect. Malden, MA: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Slobin, D. I. (1997a). The universal, the typological, and the particular in acquisition. In Slobin, D. I. (Ed.), Crosslinguistic study of language acquisition (Vol. 5) (pp. 140). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Slobin, D. I. (1997b). The origins of grammaticizable notions: beyond the individual mind. In Slobin, D. I. (Ed.), Crosslinguistic study of language acquisition (Vol. 5) (pp. 265324). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Smith, C. S. (1991). The parameter of aspect. Dordrecht / Boston: Kluwer.Google Scholar
Snyder, W., & Stromswold, K. (1997). The structure and acquisition of English dative constructions. Linguistic Inquiry, 28(2), 281317.Google Scholar
Tai, J. H.-Y. (1975). On two functions of place adverbials in Mandarin Chinese. Journal of Chinese Linguistics, 3, 154–79.Google Scholar
Tai, J. H-Y. (1985). Temporal sequence and Chinese word order. In Haiman, J. (Ed.), Iconicity in syntax (pp. 4972). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Tardif, T. (1993). Adult-to-child speech and language acquisition in Mandarin Chinese. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Yale University.Google Scholar
Teng, S.-H. (1975). On location and movement in Chinese. Gengo Kenkyu, 67, 3057.Google Scholar
Thornton, R. (1996). Elicited production. In McDaniel, D., Mckee, C., & Cairns, H. S. (Eds.), Methods for assessing children's syntax (pp. 77102). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2000a). Do young children have adult syntactic competence? Cognition, 74, 209–53.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2000b). The item-based nature of children's early syntactic development. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(4), 156–63.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2009). The usage-based theory of language acquisition. In Bavin, E. (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of child language (pp. 6988). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Vendler, Z. (1957). Verbs and times. Philosophical Review, 66(2), 143–60.Google Scholar
Verkuyl, H. J. (1972). On the compositional nature of the aspects. Dordrecht: Reidel.Google Scholar
Wang, H. (1957). Shuo ‘zai’ [On zai]. Zhongguo Yuwen [Studies of the Chinese Language], 2, 25–6.Google Scholar
Wang, H. (1980). Zai shuoshuo ‘zai’ [A revisit to zai]. Yuyan Jiaoxue yu Yanjiu [Language Teaching and Research], 3, 25–9.Google Scholar
Yang, C. D. (2002). Knowledge and learning in natural language. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Yang, C. D. (2004). Universal Grammar, statistics or both. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(10), 451–6.Google Scholar
Yip, V., & Matthews, S. (2007). The bilingual child: early development and language contact. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Zhou, W. (2011). Xiandai hanyu jieci xide yanjiu [Research on the acquisition of prepositions in modern Chinese]. Beijing: Shijie Tushu Chubanshe.Google Scholar