Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-mkpzs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-27T09:27:23.810Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Attention and word learning in toddlers who are late talkers

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 July 2015

MICHELLE MACROY-HIGGINS*
Affiliation:
Hunter College, City University of New York
ELIZABETH A. MONTEMARANO
Affiliation:
New York City Department of Education
*
[*]Address for correspondence: Michelle MacRoy-Higgins, Hunter College, CUNY, Department of Speech-Language Pathology and Audiology, 425 East 25th Street, New York, NY 10010. tel: 212-481-4358; fax: 212-481-4458; e-mail: mmacroy@hunter.cuny.edu

Abstract

The purpose of this study was to examine attention allocation in toddlers who were late talkers and toddlers with typical language development while they were engaged in a word-learning task in order to determine if differences exist. Two-year-olds who were late talkers (11) and typically developing toddlers (11) were taught twelve novel pseudo-words for unfamiliar objects over ten training sessions. The toddlers' attention allocation during the word-learning sessions was measured as well as their comprehension of the newly learned words. Late talkers showed reduced attention allocation to objects during word-training sessions, and also comprehended fewer of the novel words than toddlers with typical language development. Attention allocation was found to be a stronger predictor of word learning as compared to cognition and auditory comprehension. Reduced attention allocation may contribute to the early lexical delay characteristic in late talkers.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2015 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Bavin, E. L. & Bretherton, L. (2013). The early language in Victoria study. In Rescorla, L. & Dale, P. S. (eds), Late talkers: language development, interventions, and outcomes (pp. 321). Baltimore, MD: Brookes Publishing Co. Google Scholar
Bayley, N. (2006). Bayley scales of infant development, 3rd ed. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Borovsky, A., Elman, J. L. & Fernald, A. (2012). Knowing a lot for one's age: vocabulary skill and not age is associated with anticipatory incremental sentence interpretation in children and adults. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 112(4), 417–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carpenter, M., Nagell, K. & Tomasello, M. (1998). Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. Monographs for the Society of Research in Child Development 63(4), 1143.Google Scholar
Dixon, W. E. Jr. & Salley, B. J. (2007). Shhh! We're trying to concentrate: attention and environmental distractors in novel word learning. Journal of Genetic Psychology 167(4), 393414.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dixon, W. E. Jr., Salley, B. J. & Clements, A. D. (2006). Temperament, distraction, and learning in toddlerhood. Infant Behavior & Development 29, 342–57.Google Scholar
Dixon, W. E. Jr. & Shore, C. (1997). Temperamental predictors of linguistic style during multiword acquisition. Infant Behavior and Development 20, 99103.Google Scholar
Dixon, W. E. Jr. & Smith, P. H. (2000). Links between early temperament and language acquisition. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly 46, 417–40.Google Scholar
Ellis Weismer, S. & Evans, J. L. (2002). The role of processing limitations in early identification of specific language impairment. Topics in Language Disorders 3, 1529.Google Scholar
Fasolo, M., Majorano, M. & D'Odorico, L. (2008). Babbling and first words in children with slow expressive development. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics 22(2), 8394.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fenson, L., Marchman, V. A., Thal, D., Dale, P. S., Reznick, P. & Bates, E. (2007). MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory, 2nd ed. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co.Google Scholar
Fernald, A. & Marchman, V. A. (2012). Individual differences in lexical processing at 18 months predict vocabulary growth in typically-developing and late-talking toddlers. Child Development 83, 203222.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fernald, A., Perfors, A. & Marchman, V. A. (2006). Picking up speed in understanding: speech processing efficiency and vocabulary growth across the 2nd year. Developmental Psychology 42(1), 98116.Google Scholar
Irwin, J. R., Carter, A. S. & Briggs-Gowan, M. J. (2002). The social–emotional development of late-talking toddlers. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 41(11), 1324–32.Google Scholar
Kannass, K. N. & Oakes, L. M. (2008). The development of attention and its relations to language in infancy and toddlerhood. Journal of Cognition and Development 9(2), 222–46.Google Scholar
Leonard, L. B. (1998). Children with specific language impairment. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google ScholarPubMed
Leonard, L. B., Schwartz, R. G., Swanson, L. A. & Frome Loeb, D. M. (1987). Some conditions that promote unusual phonological behavior in children. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics 1, 2334.Google Scholar
MacRoy-Higgins, M., Schwartz, R. G., Shafer, V. L. & Marton, K. (2013). Influence of phonotactic probability / neighbourhood density on lexical learning in late talkers. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders 48(2), 188–99.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mundy, P., Delgado, C., Block, J., Pomares, Y., VanHecke, A. V. & Parlade, M. V. (2007). Individual differences and the development of joint attention in infancy. Child Development 78, 938–54.Google Scholar
Mundy, P., Fox, N. & Card, J. (2003). EEG coherence, JA and language development in the second year. Developmental Science 6(1), 4854.Google Scholar
Oakes, L. M., Madole, K. L. & Cohen, L. B. (1991). Infant habituation and categorization of real objects. Cognitive Development 6, 377392.Google Scholar
Oller, D. K., Eilers, R. E., Neal, A. R. & Schwartz, H. K. (1999). Precursors to speech in infancy: the prediction of speech and language disorders. Journal of Communication Disorders 32, 223245.Google Scholar
Paul, R. (1993). Outcomes of early expressive language delay. Journal of Childhood Communication Disorders 15, 714.Google Scholar
Paul, R. & Shiffer, M. (1991). Communicative initiations in normal and late-talking toddlers. Applied Psycholinguistics 12, 419–31.Google Scholar
Plunkett, K. (1997) Theories of early language acquisition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 1(4), 146–53.Google Scholar
Rescorla, L. (1989). The language development survey: a screening tool for delayed language in toddlers. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders 54, 587–99.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rescorla, L. (2009). Age 17 language and reading outcomes in late-talking toddlers: support for a dimensional perspective on language delay. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 52, 1630.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ruff, H. A. (1986). Components of attention during infants’ manipulative exploration. Child Development 57(1), 105–14.Google Scholar
Schwartz, R. G. & Leonard, L. B. (1985). Lexical imitation and acquisition in language-impaired children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders 50, 141–9.Google Scholar
Smith, L. B. & Yu, C. (2008). Infants rapidly learn word–referent mappings via cross-situational statistics. Cognition 106(3), 1558–68.Google Scholar
Stoel-Gammon, C. (2011). Relationships between lexical and phonological development in young children. Journal of Child Language 38, 134.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Storkel, H. L., 2001, Learning new words: phonotactic probability in language development. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 44, 1321–37.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (1995). Joint attention as social cognition. In Moore, C. & Dunham, P.J. (eds), Joint attention: its origins and role in development (pp. 103130). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Vuksanovic, J. & Bjekic, J. (2013). Developmental relationship between language and joint attention in late talkers. Research in Developmental Disabilities 34, 2360–8.Google Scholar
Wetherby, A. M., Yonclas, D. G. & Bryan, A. A. (1989). Communicative profiles of preschool children with handicaps: implications for early identification. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders 54, 148–58.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yu, C. & Ballard, D. H. (2007). A unified model of early word learning: integrating statistical and social cues. Neurocomputing 70, 2149–65.Google Scholar
Yu, C. & Smith, L. B. (2011). What you learn is what you see: using eye movements to study infant cross-situational word learning. Developmental Science 16(2), 165–80.Google Scholar
Yu, C. & Smith, L. B. (2012). Embodied attention and word learning by toddlers. Cognition 125, 244–62.Google Scholar
Zimmerman, I. L., Steiner, V. G. & Pond, R. E. (2002). The Preschool Language Scale, 4th ed. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar