Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-8ctnn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-27T07:43:01.212Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Caregivers provide more labeling responses to infants' pointing than to infants' object-directed vocalizations*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  13 June 2014

ZHEN WU
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Iowa
JULIE GROS-LOUIS*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Iowa
*
Address for correspondence: Julie Gros-Louis, Department of Psychology, University of Iowa, E11, Seashore Hall, Iowa City IA, 52242. e-mail: julie-gros-louis@uiowa.edu

Abstract

Existing studies have observed a robust relationship between infants' pointing gestures and language outcomes. By contrast, infants' overall vocal production is not related to language outcomes. One possible explanation for the association between pointing and language is that pointing gestures, as compared to vocalizations, may elicit more verbal responses from social partners that are facilitative for language learning. To test this, we observed forty-seven infants aged 1;0 during free play with their mothers and fathers separately to compare parents' verbal responses to infants' pointing gestures and object-directed vocalizations. Results showed that, compared to object-directed vocalizations, infants' pointing elicited more verbal responses from parents, particularly object labels. Moreover, mothers were more likely than fathers to provide labels. These results may help explain why pointing is associated with indices of language acquisition, but the production of vocalizations is not.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2014 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

First, we would like to thank the infants and families who participated. Also, thanks to Elizabeth Alpert, Jenny Anderson, Sarah Bartlett, Emma Bulzoni, Johanna Burdinie, Ahhyun Cheong, Amber Dewey, Kiley Dewhurst, Kara Eberhardt, Alex Einfeldt, Jennifer Hand, Ayumi Irie, Ruri Kim, Kristin Langhammer, Katie McDonald, Jessica Mucha, Elena Newton, Heather Pollock, Fanya Sun, Kelsey Torgerson, Nicole Vorce, Elizabeth Watson, and Qiuting Zhou for help in participant recruitment, testing, coding, and/or data organization. Lastly, thanks to Larissa Samuelson for helpful suggestions on the manuscript.

References

REFERENCES

Adamson, L. B., Bakeman, R., Smith, C. B. & Walters, A. S. (1987). Adults' interpretation of infants' acts. Developmental Psychology 23, 383–7.Google Scholar
Bates, E. (1979). The emergence of symbols: cognition and communication in infancy. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bates, E., Camaioni, L. & Volterra, V. (1975). The acquisition of performatives prior to speech. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly 21, 205–26.Google Scholar
Bates, E., O'Connell, B. & Shore, C. (1987). Language and communication in infancy. In Osofsky, J. D. (ed.), Handbook of infant development, 2nd ed., 149203. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Begus, K. & Southgate, V. (2012). Infant pointing serves an interrogative function. Developmental Science 15, 611–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blake, J., Vitale, G., Osborne, P. & Olshansky, E. (2005). A cross-cultural comparison of communicative gestures in human infants during the transition to language. Gesture 5, 201–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bornstein, M. H., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Hahn, C.-S. & Haynes, O. M. (2008). Maternal responsiveness to young children at three ages: longitudinal analysis of a multidimensional, modular, and specific parenting construct. Developmental Psychology 44, 867–74.Google Scholar
Bornstein, M. H., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S. & Haynes, O. M. (1999). First words in the second year: continuity, stability, and models of concurrent and predictive correspondence in vocabulary and verbal responsiveness across age and context. Infant Behavior and Development 22, 6585.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brooks, R. & Meltzoff, A. N. (2008). Infant gaze following and pointing predict accelerated vocabulary growth through two years of age: a longitudinal, growth curve modeling study. Journal of Child Language 35, 207–20.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Butterworth, G. (2003). Pointing is the royal road to language for babies. In Kita, S. (ed.), Pointing: where language, culture, and cognition meet, 933. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Butterworth, G. & Morissette, P. (1996). Onset of pointing and the acquisition of language in infancy. Journal of Reproductive and Infant Psychology 14, 219–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cabrera, N. J., Shannon, J. D. & Tamis-LeMonda, C. (2007). Fathers' influence on their children's cognitive and emotional development: from toddlers to pre-K. Applied Development Science 11, 208–13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Camaioni, L., Castelli, M. C., Longobardi, E. & Volterra, V. (1991). A parent report instrument for early language assessment. First Language 11, 345–58.Google Scholar
Carpenter, M., Nagell, K. & Tomasello, M. (1998). Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 255(63), 1143.Google Scholar
Clark, R. (1978). The transition from action to gesture. In Lock, A. (ed.), Action, gesture and symbol: the emergence of language, 231–57. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Colonnesi, C., Geert Jan, J. M., Stams, G. J. J. M., Koster, I. & Noom, M. J. (2010). The relation between pointing and language development: a meta-analysis. Developmental Review 30, 352–66.Google Scholar
Desrochers, S., Morissette, P. & Ricard, M. (1995). Two perspectives on pointing in infancy. In Moore, C. & Dunham, P. J. (eds), Joint attention: its origins and role in development, 85101. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Elias, G., Meadows, D. & Bain, J. D. (2003). Agreement between mothers and fatherson the identification of infant communicative acts: use of a randomization procedure. Infancy 4, 219–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gleason, J. B. (1975). Fathers and other strangers: men's speech to young children. In Data, D. P. (ed.), Developmental psycholinguistics: theory and application, 289–97. Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press.Google Scholar
Goldfield, B. A. & Reznick, J. S. (1990). Early lexical acquisition: rate, content, and the vocabulary spurt. Journal of Child Language 17, 171–83.Google Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S. (2007). Pointing sets the stage for learning language and creating language. Child Development 78, 741–5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldin-Meadow, S., Goodrich, W., Sauer, E. & Iverson, J. (2007). Young children use their hands to tell their mothers what to say. Developmental Science 10, 778–85.Google Scholar
Goldstein, M. H. & Schwade, J. A. (2010). From birds to words: perception of structure in social interactions guides vocal development and language learning. In Blumberg, M. S., Freeman, J. H. & Robinson, S. R. (eds), The Oxford handbook of developmental behavioral neuroscience, 708–29. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Goldstein, M. H., Schwade, J. A., Briesch, J. & Syal, S. (2010). Learning while babbling: prelinguistic object-directed vocalizations indicate a readiness to learn. Infancy 15, 362–91.Google Scholar
Golinkoff, R. & Ames, G. (1979). A comparison of fathers' and mothers' speech with their children. Child Development 50, 2832.Google Scholar
Goodman, J. C., Dale, P. S. & Li, P. (2008). Does frequency count? Parental input and the acquisition of vocabulary. Journal of Child Language 35, 515–31.Google Scholar
Gros-Louis, J., West, M. J., Goldstein, M. H. & King, A. P. (2006). Mothers provide differential feedback to infants' prelinguistic sounds. International Journal of Behavioral Development 30, 509–16.Google Scholar
Harding, C. G. & Golinkoff, R. M. (1979). The origins of intentional vocalizations in prelinguistic infants. Child Development 50, 3340.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hayes, A. F. & Krippendorff, K. (2007). Answering the call for a standard reliability measure for coding data. Communication Methods and Measures 1, 7789.Google Scholar
Hiadek, E. & Edwards, H. (1984). A comparative analysis of mother–father speech in the naturalistic home environment. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 13, 321–32.Google Scholar
Huttenlocher, J., Haight, W., Bryk, A., Seltzer, M. & Lyons, T. (1991). Early vocabulary growth: relation to language input and gender. Developmental Psychology 27, 236–48.Google Scholar
Kishimoto, T., Shizawa, Y., Yasuda, J., Hinobayashi, T. & Minami, T. (2007). Do pointing gestures by infants provoke comments from adults? Infant Behavior & Development 30, 562–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lamb, M. E. (2010). How do fathers influence children's development? Let me count the ways. In Lamb, M. E. (ed.), The role of the father in child development, 126. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Lausberg, H. & Sloetjes, H. (2009). Coding gestural behavior with the NEUROGES-ELAN system. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments & Computers 41, 841–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
LeBarton, E. A. S., Goldin-Meadow, S. & Raudenbush, S. (in press). Experimentally-induced increases in early gesture lead to increases in spoken vocabulary. Journal of Cognition and Development, online doi:10.1080/15248372.2013.858041.Google Scholar
Leung, E. H. & Rheingold, H. L. (1981). Development of pointing as a social gesture. Developmental Psychology 17, 215–20.Google Scholar
Liszkowski, U. (2008). Before L1: a differentiated perspective on infant gestures. Gesture 8, 180–96.Google Scholar
Liszkowski, U. (2011). Three lines in the emergence of prelinguistic communication and social cognition. Journal of Cognitive Education and Psychology 10, 3244.Google Scholar
Liszkowski, U., Albrecht, K., Carpenter, M. & Tomasello, M. (2008). infants' visual and auditory communication when a partner is or is not visually attending. Infant Behavior and Development 31, 157–67.Google Scholar
Liszkowski, U., Carpenter, M., Henning, A., Striano, T. & Tomasello, M. (2004). Twelve-month-olds point to share attention and interest. Developmental Science 7, 297307.Google Scholar
Liszkowski, U., Carpenter, M. & Tomasello, M. (2007). Reference and attitude in infant pointing. Journal of Child Language 34, 120.Google Scholar
Masur, E. F. (1982). Mothers' responses to infants' object-related gestures: influences on lexical development. Journal of Child Language 9, 2330.Google Scholar
Masur, E. F. & Gleason, J. B. (1980). Parent–child interaction and the acquisition of lexical information during play. Developmental Psychology 16, 404–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Majorano, M., Rainieri, C. & Corsano, P. (2013). Parents' child-directed communication and child language development: a longitudinal study with Italian toddlers. Journal of Child Language 40, 836–59.Google Scholar
McLaughlin, B., White, D., McDevitt, T. & Raskin, R. (1983). Mothers' and fathers' speech to their young children: Similar or different? Journal of Child Language 32, 6391.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. (1988). Constraints on word learning? Cognitive Development 3(3), 221–46.Google Scholar
Olson, J. & Masur, E. F. (2011). infants' gestures influence mothers' provision of object, action and internal state labels. Journal of Child Language 38, 1028–54.Google Scholar
Olson, J. & Masur, E. F. (2013). Mothers respond differently to infants' gestural versus nongestural communicative bids. First Language 33, 372–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paavola, L., Kunnari, S. & Moilanen, I. (2005). Maternal responsiveness and infant intentional communication: implications for the early communicative and linguistic development. Child: Care, Health and Development 31, 727–35.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paavola, L., Kunnari, S., Moilanen, I. & Lehtihalmes, M. (2005). The functions of maternal verbal responses to prelinguistic infants as predictors of early communicative and linguistic development. First Language 25, 173–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pan, B. A., Rowe, M. L., Singer, J. D. & Snow, C. E. (2005). Maternal correlates of growth in toddler vocabulary production in low-income families. Child Development 76, 763–82.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pancsofar, N. & Vernon-Feagans, L. (2006). Mother and father language input to young children: contributions to later language development. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 27, 571–87.Google Scholar
Pancsofar, N. & Vernon-Feagans, L. (2010). Fathers' early contributions to children's language development in families from low-income rural communities. Early Childhood Research Quarterly 25, 450–63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pleck, J. H. (2010). Paternal involvement: revised conceptualization and theoretical linkages with child outcomes. In Lamb, M. E. (ed.), The role of the father in child development, 5893. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Puccini, D., Hassemer, M., Salomo, D. & Liszkowski, U. (2010). The type of shared activity shapes caregiver and infant communication. Gesture 10, 279–96.Google Scholar
Rollins, P. R. (2003). Caregivers' contingent comments to 9-month-old infants: relationships with later language. Applied Psycholinguistics 24, 221–34.Google Scholar
Rondal, J. A. (1980). Fathers' and mothers' speech in early language development. Journal of Child Language 7, 353–69.Google Scholar
Rowe, M. L., Coker, D. & Pan, B. A. (2004). A comparison of fathers' and mothers' talk to toddlers in low-income families. Social Development 13(2), 278–91.Google Scholar
Rowe, M. L. & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2009). Early gesture selectively predicts later language learning. Developmental Science 12, 182–7.Google Scholar
Rowe, M. L., Özçalışkan, Ş. & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2008). Learning words by hand: gesture's role in predicting vocabulary development. First Language 28, 182–99.Google Scholar
Shannon, J., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., London, K. & Cabrera, N. (2002). Beyond rough and tumble: low-income fathers' interactions and children's cognitive development at 24 months. Parenting: Science and Practice 2, 77104.Google Scholar
Southgate, V., Van Maanen, C. & Csibra, G. (2007). Infant pointing: Communication to cooperate or communication to learn? Child Development 78, 735–40.Google Scholar
Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Baumwell, L. & Cristofaro, T. (2012). Parent–child conversations during play. First Language 32, 413–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Bornstein, M. H. & Baumwell, L. (2001). Maternal responsiveness and children's achievement of language milestones. Child Development 72, 748–67.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tamis-LeMonda, C., Shannon, J., Cabrera, N. & Lamb, M. (2004). Fathers and mothers at play with their 2- and 3-year-olds: contributions to language and cognitive development. Child Development 75, 1806–20.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2008). Origins of human communication. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M. & Liszkowski, U. (2007). A new look at infant pointing. Child Development 78, 705–22.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M., Conti-Ramsden, G. & Ewert, B. (1990). Young children's conversations with their mothers and fathers: differences in breakdown and repair. Journal of Child Language 17, 115–30.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. & Farrar, M. J. (1986). Joint attention and early language. Child Development 57, 1454–63.Google Scholar
Vygotsky, L. S. (1986). Thought and Language, revised ed. (Ed. Kozulin, A.). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Werner, H. & Kaplan, B. (1964). Symbol formation: an organismic developmental approach to language & the expression of thought. New York: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Yoder, P. J. & Warren, S. F. (1998). Maternal responsivity predicts the prelinguistic communication intervention that facilitates generalized intentional communication. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 41, 1207–19.Google Scholar