Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gbm5v Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-27T07:44:10.951Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Caregivers' suffix frequencies and suffix acquisition by language impaired, late talking, and typically developing children*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  13 December 2011

ANNE S. WARLAUMONT*
Affiliation:
University of Memphis
LINDA JARMULOWICZ
Affiliation:
University of Memphis

Abstract

Acquisition of regular inflectional suffixes is an integral part of grammatical development in English and delayed acquisition of certain inflectional suffixes is a hallmark of language impairment. We investigate the relationship between input frequency and grammatical suffix acquisition, analyzing 217 transcripts of mother–child (ages 1 ; 11–6 ; 9) conversations from the CHILDES database. Maternal suffix frequency correlates with previously reported rank orders of acquisition and with child suffix frequency. Percentages of children using a suffix are consistent with frequencies in caregiver speech. Although late talkers acquire suffixes later than typically developing children, order of acquisition is similar across populations. Furthermore, the third person singular and past tense verb suffixes, weaknesses for children with language impairment, are less frequent in caregiver speech than the plural noun suffix, a relative strength in language impairment. Similar findings hold across typical, SLI and late talker populations, suggesting that frequency plays a role in suffix acquisition.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2011

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

This work was supported by a Department of Energy Computational Science Graduate Fellowship awarded to the first author. We thank Sarah Lowder, Rick Dale, the research teams and participants who contributed data for the Conti-Ramsden and Weismer CHILDES corpora, the CHILDES project, the R project, and the anonymous reviewers of this article.

References

REFERENCES

Brown, R. (1973). A first language: The early stages. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bybee, J. (1995). Regular morphology and the lexicon. Language and Cognitive Processes 10, 425–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, E. V. (1993). The lexicon in acquisition. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Conti-Ramsden, G. & Dykins, J. (1991). Mother–child interactions with language-impaired children and their siblings. British Journal of Disorders of Communication 26, 337–54.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Conti-Ramsden, G., Hutcheson, G. D. & Grove, J. (1995). Contingency and breakdown: Children with SLI and their conversations with mothers and fathers. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 38, 1290–302.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Conti-Ramsden, G. & Jones, M. (1997). Verb use in specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 40, 1298–313.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Creutz, M. & Lagus, K. (2005). Inducing the morphological lexicon of a natural language from unannotated text. In Honkela, T., Könönen, V., Pöllä, M. & Simula, O. (eds), Proceedings of AKRR'05, International and Interdisciplinary Conference on Adaptive Knowledge Representation and Reasoning, 106113.Google Scholar
Dale, P. S. & Fenson, L. (1996). Lexical development norms for young children. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers 28, 125–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dallal, G. E. (2001). Identifying similar groups. Retrieved from: www.jerrydallal.com/LHSP/similar_prog.htm.Google Scholar
Demberg, V. (2007). A language-independent unsupervised model for morphological segmentation. In Proceedings of the 45th Annual Meeting of the Association of Computational Linguistics, 920–27.Google Scholar
de Villiers, J. G. & de Villiers, P. A. (1973). A cross-sectional study of the acquisition of grammatical morphemes in child speech. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 2, 267–78.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ellis Weismer, S. & Hesketh, L. J. (1996). Lexical learning by children with specific language impairment: Effects of linguistic input presented at varying speaking rates. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 39, 177–90.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fey, M. E., Long, S. H. & Finestack, L. H. (2003). Ten principles of grammar facilitation for children with specific language impairments. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology 12, 315.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Girolametto, L., Bonifacio, S., Visini, C., Weitzman, E., Zocconi, E. & Steig Pearce, P. (2002). Mother–child interactions in Canada and Italy: Linguistic responsiveness to late-talking toddlers. International Journal of Communication Disorders 37(2), 153–71.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Girolametto, L., Wiigs, M., Smyth, R., Weitzman, E. & Steig Pearce, P. (2001). Children with a history of expressive vocabulary delay: Outcomes at 5 years of age. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology 10, 358–69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodman, J. C., Dale, P. S. & Li, P. (2008). Does frequency count? Parental input and the acquisition of vocabulary. Journal of Child Language 35, 515–31.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gray, S. (2003). Word-learning by preschoolers with specific language impairment: What predicts success? Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 46, 5667.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Graziano-King, J. & Cairns, H. S. (2005). Acquisition of English comparative adjectives. Journal of Child Language 32(2), 345–73.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hammer, C. S., Tomblin, J. B., Zhang, X. & Weiss, A. L. (2001). Relationship between parenting behaviours and specific language impairment in children. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders 36, 185205.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoff, E. & Naigles, L. (2002). How children use input to acquire a lexicon. Child Development 73, 418–33.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jarmulowicz, L. (2002). English derivational suffix frequency and children's stress judgments. Brain and Language 81, 192204.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jarmulowicz, L., Taran, V. L. & Hay, S. E. (2008). Lexical frequency and third-graders' stress accuracy in derived English word production. Applied Psycholinguistics 29, 213–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kouri, T. (2005). Lexical training through modeling and elicitation procedures with late talkers who have specific language impairment and developmental delays. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 48, 157–71.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leonard, L. B. (1998). Children with specific language impairment. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google ScholarPubMed
Leonard, L. B. & Finneran, D. (2003). Grammatical morpheme effects on MLU. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 46, 878–88.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leonard, L. B., Nippold, M. A., Kail, R. & Hale, C. A. (1983). Picture naming in language-impaired children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 26, 609615.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacWhinney, B. (2000). The CHILDES Project: Tools for analyzing talk, 3rd edn.Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Marchman, V. A., Wulfeck, B. & Ellis Weismer, S. E. (1999). Morphological productivity in children with normal language and SLI: A study of the English past tense. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 42, 206219.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marshall, C. R. & van der Lely, H. K. J. (2007). Derivational morphology in children with grammatical specific language impairment. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics 21(2), 7191.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, J. (1981). Assessing language production in children. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.Google Scholar
Moerk, E. L. (1980). Relationships between parental input frequencies and children's language acquisition – a reanalysis of Brown's data. Journal of Child Language 7, 105118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moyle, M. J., Ellis Weismer, S., Evans, J. L. & Lindstrom, M. J. (2007). Longitudinal relationships between lexical and grammatical development in typical and late-talking children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 50, 508528.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Naigles, L. R. & Hoff-Ginsberg, E. (1998). Why are some verbs learned before other verbs? Effects of input frequency and structure on children's early verb use. Journal of Child Language 25, 95120.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nicoladis, E., Palmer, A. & Marentette, P. (2007). The role of type and token frequency in using past tense morphemes correctly. Developmental Science 10, 237–54.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oetting, J. B. & Rice, M. L. (1993). Plural acquisition in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 36, 1236–48.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
R Development Core Team (2009). R: A language and environment for statistical computing [Computer software]. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Retrieved from: www.R-project.org.Google Scholar
Rescorla, L., Dahlsgaard, K. & Roberts, J. (2000). Late-talking toddlers: MLU and IPSyn outcomes at 3 ; 0 and 4 ; 0. Journal of Child Language 27(3), 643–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rescorla, L. & Roberts, J. (2002). Nominal versus verbal morpheme use in late talkers at ages 3 and 4. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 45, 1219–31.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rice, M. L. & Bode, J. V. (1993). GAPS in the verb lexicons of children with specific language impairment. First Language 13, 113–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rice, M. L., Oetting, J. B., Marquis, J., Bode, J. & Pae, S. (1994). Frequency of input effects on word comprehension of children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 37, 106122.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rice, M. L., Wexler, K. & Hershberger, S. (1998). Tense over time: The longitudinal course of tense acquisition in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 41, 1412–31.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rice, M. L., Wexler, K., Marquis, J. & Hershberger, S. (2000). Acquisition of irregular past tense by children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 43, 1126–45.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Riches, N. G., Faragher, B. & Conti-Ramsden, G. (2006). Verb schema use and input dependence in 5-year-old children with Specific Language Impairment (SLI). International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders 41, 117–35.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Riches, N. G., Tomasello, M. & Conti-Ramsden, G. (2005). Verb learning in children with SLI: Frequency and spacing effects. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 48, 1397–411.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwartz, R. G. & Terrell, B. Y. (1983). The role of input frequency in lexical acquisition. Journal of Child Language 10, 5764.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Storkel, H. L. (2001). Learning new words: Phonotactic probability in language development. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 44, 1321–37.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tardif, T., Shatz, M. & Naigles, L. (1997). Caregiver speech and children's use of nouns versus verbs: A comparison of English, Italian, and Mandarin. Journal of Child Language 24, 535–65.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thordardottir, E. T. & Ellis Weismer, S. (2001). High-frequency verbs and verb diversity in the spontaneous speech of school-age children with specific language impairment. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders 36, 221–44.Google ScholarPubMed
Tomasello, M. (2005). Constructing a language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vasilyeva, M., Huttenlocher, J. & Waterfall, H. (2006). Effects of language intervention on syntactic skill levels in preschoolers. Developmental Psychology 42, 164–74.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed