Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-mlc7c Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-14T23:13:56.907Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Child language research: building on the past, looking to the future

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 September 2008

Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Editorial
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1994

References

REFERENCES

Aksu-Koç, A. (1988). The acquisition of aspect and modality. The case of past reference in Turkish. Cambridge: C.U.P.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Atkinson, M. (1992). Children's syntax: an introduction to principles and parameters theory. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Bates, E., Bretherton, I. & Snyder, L. (1980). From first words to grammar: indimdual differences and dissociable mechanisms. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Bavin, E. L. & Shopen, T. A. (1985). Children's acquisition of Warlpiri: comprehension of transitive sentences. Journal of Child Language 12, 597610.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1981). Lectures on government and binding. Dordrecht: Foris.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1986). Barriers. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Clancy, P.(1989). Form and function in the acquisition of Korean wh- questions. Journal of Child Language 16, 323–47.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Demuth, K. (1990). Subject, topic and Sesotho passive. Journal of Child Language 17, 6784.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dromi, E. & Berman, R. A. (1982). A morphemic measure of early language development: data from modern Hebrew. Journal of Child Language 9, 403–24.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fodor, J. A. (1983). The modularity of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hickey, T. (1991). Mean length of utterance and the acquisition of Irish. Journal of Child Language 18, 553–69.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1986). Some fundamental aspects of language development after age 5. In Fletcher, P. & Garman, M. (eds), Language acquisition (2nd edn). Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Klima, E. S. & Bellugi, U. (1966). Syntactic regularities in the speech of children. In Lyons, J. & Wales, R. J. (eds), Psycholinguistic Papers. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.Google Scholar
Lieven, E. V. M., Fine, J. M. & Dresner Barnes, H. (1992). Individual differences in early vocabulary development: redefining the referential-expressive distinction. Journal of Child Language 19, 287310.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McClelland, J. L. & Rumelhart, D. E. (eds) (1986). Parallel distributed processing: explorations in the microstructure of cognition. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (1991). The CHILDES project: computational tools for analyzing talk. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. & Bates, E. (1989). The crosslinguistic study of sentence processing. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Maratsos, M., Kuczaj, S., Fox, D. & Chalkley, M. A. (1979). Some empirical studies in the acquisition of transformational relations: passives, negatives and the past tense. In Collins, W. A. (ed.), Children's language and communications. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Menig-Peterson, C. L. (1975). The modification of communicative behavior in preschoolaged children as a function of the listener's perspective. Child Development 46, 1015–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moon, C., Bever, T. G. & Fifer, W. P. (1992). Canonical and non-canonical syllable discrimination by two-day-old infants. Journal of Child Language 19, 117.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, K. (1973). Structure and strategy in learning to talk. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 38, No. 149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, K. (1981). Individual differences in language development: implications for development and language. Developmental Psychology 17, 170–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perner, J. & Leekam, S. R. (1986). Belief and quantity: three-year-olds' adaptation to listeners' knowledge. Journal of Child Language 13, 305–15.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peterson, C. & Dodsworth, P. (1991). A longitudinal analysis of young children's cohesion and noun specification in narratives. Journal of Child Language 18, 397415.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pine, J. M. & Lieven, E. V. M. (1990). Referential style at thirteen months: why age-defined cross-sectional measures are inappropriate for the study of strategy differences in early language development. Journal of Child Language 17, 625–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pinker, S. (1984). Language learnability and language development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Pinker, S. (1989). Learnability and cognition: the acquisition of argument structure. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Reinhart, T. (1983). Anaphora and semantic interpretation. London: Croom Helm.Google Scholar
Richards, B. & Robinson, P. (1993). Environmental correlates of child copula verb growth. Journal of Child Language 20, 343–62.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shatz, M., Hoff-Ginsberg, E. & Maclver, D. (1989). Induction and the acquisition of English auxiliaries: the effects of differentially enriched input. Journal of Child Language 16, 141–60.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Slobin, D. I. (ed.) (1985). The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition. Vols I & II. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Slobin, D. I. (ed.) (1993). The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition. Vol. III. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Snow, C. (1986). Conversations with children. In Fletcher, P. & Garman, M. (eds), Language acquisition (2nd edn). Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Sperber, D. & Wilson, D. (1986). Relevance: communication and cognition. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Vaidyanathan, R. (1991). Development of forms and functions of negatives in the early stages of language acquisition: a study in Tamil. Journal of Child Language 18, 5166.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weist, R. M., Wysocka, H. & Lyytinen, P. (1991). A crosslinguistic perspective on the development of temporal Systems. Journal of Child Language 18, 6792.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed