Crossref Citations
This article has been cited by the following publications. This list is generated based on data provided by
Crossref.
Brown, Gordon D. A.
Sharkey, Amanda J. C.
and
Brown, Gillian
1987.
Factors affecting the success of referential communication.
Journal of Psycholinguistic Research,
Vol. 16,
Issue. 6,
p.
535.
Bredart, Serge
1987.
Is the quantity maxim strategy child-specific?.
Journal of Child Language,
Vol. 14,
Issue. 1,
p.
175.
Pynte, Joel
Girotto, Vittorio
and
Baccino, Thierry
1991.
Children's communicative abilities revisited: verbal versus perceptual disambiguating strategies in referential communication.
Journal of Child Language,
Vol. 18,
Issue. 1,
p.
191.
Surian, Luca
1991.
Do children exploit the Maxim of Antecedent in order to interpret ambiguous descriptions?.
Journal of Child Language,
Vol. 18,
Issue. 2,
p.
451.
Eskritt, Michelle
Whalen, Juanita
and
Lee, Kang
2008.
Preschoolers can recognize violations of the Gricean maxims.
British Journal of Developmental Psychology,
Vol. 26,
Issue. 3,
p.
435.
Katsos, Napoleon
and
Bishop, Dorothy V.M.
2011.
Pragmatic tolerance: Implications for the acquisition of informativeness and implicature.
Cognition,
Vol. 120,
Issue. 1,
p.
67.
KRONMÜLLER, EDMUNDO
MORISSEAU, TIFFANY
and
NOVECK, IRA A.
2014.
Show me the pragmatic contribution: a developmental investigation of contrastive inference.
Journal of Child Language,
Vol. 41,
Issue. 5,
p.
985.
FOPPOLO, Francesca
MAZZAGGIO, Greta
PANZERI, Francesca
and
SURIAN, Luca
2021.
Scalar and ad-hoc pragmatic inferences in children: guess which one is easier.
Journal of Child Language,
Vol. 48,
Issue. 2,
p.
350.