Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-q99xh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-27T09:54:48.333Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The defective tense hypothesis: on the emergence of tense and aspect in child Polish*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 September 2008

Richard M. Weist
Affiliation:
State University of New York
Hanna Wysocka
Affiliation:
Adam Mickiewicz University
Katarzyna Witkowska-Stadnik
Affiliation:
Adam Mickiewicz University
Ewa Buczowska
Affiliation:
Adam Mickiewicz University
Emilia Konieczna
Affiliation:
Adam Mickiewicz University

Abstract

Longitudinal and cross-sectional designs were combined in this analysis of the evolution of children's capacity to represent deictic relationships. The longitudinal component contained the naturalistic observation of three relatively young children (1; 7–1; 9) and three somewhat older children (2; 0–2; 2). These children were tape-recorded in caretaker–child interactions. The analysis of the corpora from these children revealed: (1) imperfective activity verb phrases in the past tense, (2) telic verb phrases in the past tense used independently of resulting states, (3) moderately remote past references, and (4) deictic future references. The cross-sectional component contained an experiment in which elicitation procedures were used to obtain past and future references to atelic and telic situations. Nine 2½- and nine 3½-year-old children were tested. Generally high levels of performance reinforced the outcome of the longitudinal analysis.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1984

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

[*]

This research was supported by the National Science Foundation, No. BNS 8121133, the Kościuszko Foundation, SUNY College at Fredonia, and the Polish Academy of Science. We would like to thank Zofia Baranowska, Ewa Domżalska and Jolanta Stawicka for their contributions to the project, and Dan Slobin for drawing our attention to Gvozdev's observations. Address for correspondence: Dept. of Psychology, SUNY College at Fredonia, Fredonia, New York 14063.

References

REFERENCES

Aksu, A. A. (1978). Aspect and modality in the child's acquisition of the Turkish past tense. Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, University of California, Berkeley.Google Scholar
Antinucci, F. & Miller, R. (1976). How children talk about what happened. JChLang 3. 167–89.Google Scholar
Bloom, L., Lifter, K. & Hafitz, J. (1980). Semantics of verbs and the development of verb inflection in child language. Lg 56. 386412.Google Scholar
Bronckart, J. P. & Sinclair, H. (1973). Time, tense, and aspect. Cognition 2. 107–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, R. (1973). A first language: the early stages. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clancy, P. M., Jacobsen, T. & Silva, N. (1976). The acquisition of conjunctions: a cross linguistic study. PRCLD 12. 7180.Google Scholar
Clark, E. V. (1970). How young children describe events in time. In d'Arcais, G. B. Flores & Levelt, W. J. M. (eds), Advances in psycholinguistics. Amsterdam: North-Holland.Google Scholar
Clark, E. V. (in press). Acquisition of Romance, with special reference to French. In Slobin, D. I.(ed.), The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition. Hillside, N.J.: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Cochrane, N. J. (1977). Verbal aspect and the semantic classification of verbs in Serbo-Croatian. Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, University of Texas.Google Scholar
Coker, P. L. (1978). Syntactic and semantic factors in the acquisition of before and after. JChLang 5. 261–77.Google Scholar
Comrie, B. (1976). Aspect: an introduction to the study of verbal aspect and related problems. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Cromer, R. F. (1968). The development of temporal reference during the acquisition of language. Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, Harvard University.Google Scholar
Paolo, M. Di & Smith, C. S. (1978). Cognitive and linguistic factors in the acquisition of temporal and aspectual expressions. In French, P. (ed.), The development of meaning. Tokyo: Bunka Hyoron.Google Scholar
Dowty, D. (1979). Word meaning and Montague Grammar. Dordrecht: Reidel.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eisenberg, A. R. (in prep.). Languge development in cultural perspective. Ph.D. dissertation, University of California, Berkeley.Google Scholar
Erbaugh, M. S. (1982). Coming to order: natural selection and the origin of syntax in the Mandarin-speaking child. Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, University of California, Berkeley.Google Scholar
Ferrell, J. O. (1951). The meaning of perfective aspect in Russian. Word 7. 104–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flavell, J. H. (1963). The developmental psychology of Jean Piaget. Princeton, N.J.: Van Nostrand.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fletcher, P. (1979). The development of the verb phrase. In Fletcher, P. & Garman, M. (eds), Language acquisition. Cambridge: C.U.P.Google Scholar
Fletcher, P. (1981). Description and explanation in the acquisition of verb forms. JChLang 8. 93108.Google ScholarPubMed
Gvozdev, A. N. (1961). Voprosy izucheniya detskoy rechi. Moscow: Izd-vo Akademii Pedagogicheskikh Nauk RSFSR.Google Scholar
Harner, L. (1981). Children talk about time and aspect of actions. ChDev 52. 498506.Google Scholar
Miller, J. E. (1970). Stative verbs in Russian. FL 6. 488504.Google Scholar
Radulović, L. (1975). Acquisition of language: studies of Dubrovnik children. Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, University of California, Berkeley.Google Scholar
Rispoli, M. (1981). The emergence of verb and adjective tense–aspect inflections in Japanese. Unpublished Master's thesis, University of Pennsylvania.Google Scholar
Schenker, A. M. (1973). Beginning Polish. New Haven: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Sinclair, J. (1971). Sensorimotor action patterns as a condition for the acquisition of syntax. In Huxley, R. & Ingram, E. (eds), Language acquisition: models and methods. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Smith, C. S. (1980). The acquisition of time talk: relations between child and adult grammars. JChLang 7. 263–78.Google ScholarPubMed
Smoczyńska, M. (1978). Wczesne stadia rozwoju skladni w mowie dziecka. (The early phase of development of syntax in the speech of children). Dissertation, Uniwersytet Jagielloński, Kraków.Google Scholar
Smoczyńska, M. (in press). Acquisition of Polish. In Slobin, D. I. (ed.), The cross-linguistic study of language acquisition. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Stephany, U. (1981). Verbal grammar in Modern Greek early child language. In Dale, P. S. & Ingram, D. (eds), Child language: an international perspective. Baltimore: University Park Press.Google Scholar
Stephany, U. (in prep.). Aspekt, Tempus, und Modalität.Google Scholar
Szagun, G. (1979). The development of spontaneous reference to past and future: a crosslinguistic study. Institut für Psychologie, Technische Universität Berlin.Google Scholar
Toivainen, J. (1980). Inflectional affixes used by Finnish-speaking children aged 1–3 years. Helsinki: Suomalaisen Kirjallisuuden Seura.Google Scholar
Tokarski, J. (1973). Fleksja Polska (Polish flection). Warszawa: Państwowe Wydawnictwo Naukowe.Google Scholar
Vendler, Z. (1967). Verbs and times. In Vendler, Z., Linguistics in philosophy. Ithaca, New York: Cornell University Press.Google Scholar
Weist, R. M. (1982). Temporal systems in child language: a crosslinguistic study. Paper presented at the 15th Annual Meeting of Societas Linguistica Europaea, Athens.Google Scholar
Weist, R. M. (1983). Prefix versus suffix information processing in the comprehension of tense and aspect. JChLang 10. 8596.Google ScholarPubMed
Zarębina, M. (1964). Ksztaltowanie się systemu językotvego dziecka (The formation of the language system of a child). Kraków: Wydawnictwo Polskiej Akademii Nauk.Google Scholar