Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-hc48f Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-28T20:42:26.542Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Developing spatial localization abilities and children's interpretation of where*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  16 April 2008

ELENA NICOLADIS*
Affiliation:
University of Alberta
EDWARD H CORNELL
Affiliation:
University of Alberta
MELISSA GATES
Affiliation:
University of Alberta
*
Address for correspondence: Elena Nicoladis, University of Alberta, P-217 Biological Sciences Building, Edmonton AB T6G 2E9, Canada. Tel: (780) 492-0124. Fax: (780) 492-1768. e-mail: elenan@ualberta.ca

Abstract

Two-year-old children often start asking questions with where. In this study we test whether children understand where to mean route or absolute location and whether the size of the space or elevation made a difference. Previous research has documented developmental changes over the preschool years in children's non-verbal spatial reasoning. Forty-eight children between two and five years of age were interviewed. We asked them to point in response to where questions about an object, rooms on the same floor and on a different floor. All children pointed to the location of the hidden objects. The youngest children pointed to the route to rooms while the oldest children were more likely to point to the location of rooms. With age, the children gradually used more spatial location terms than deictic terms in response to where. These results suggest that children's meaning of where initially differs for different sized spaces and developmental changes reflect non-verbal cognition.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2008

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

*

Natural Science and Engineering Research Council grants to all three authors contributed to funding the research. Elaine Gredanius, Gloria Leung and James Singleton all helped with the testing of the children.

References

Bloom, L., Merkin, S. & Wootten, J. (1982). Wh-questions: Linguistic factors that contribute to the sequence of acquisition. Child Development 53, 1084–92.Google Scholar
Bloom, L., Rocissano, L. & Hood, L. (1976). Adult–child discourse: Developmental interaction between information processing and linguistic knowledge. Cognitive Psychology 8, 521–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Booth, A. E. & Waxman, S. (2002). Object names and object functions serve as cues to categories for infants. Developmental Psychology 38, 948–57.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bowerman, M. (1982). Reorganizational processes in lexical and syntactic development. In Wanner, E. & Gleitman, L. R. (eds) Language acquisition: The state of the art, 319–46. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bowerman, M. (1996). Learning how to structure space for language: A crosslinguistic perspective. In Bloom, P., Peterson, M. A., Nadel, L. & Garrett, M. F. (eds) Language and space, 385436. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowerman, M. (1999). Learning how to structure space for language: A crosslinguistic perspective. In Bloom, P., Peterson, M., Nadel, L. & Garrett, M. (eds) Cognitive development and spatial language, 383435. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Brown, R. (1973). A first language: The early stages. London: George Allen & Unwin Ltd.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Butterworth, G. (1991). The ontogeny and phylogeny of joint visual attention. In Whiten, A. (ed.) Natural theories of mind: Evolution, development and simulation of everyday mindreading, 223–32. Cambridge, MA: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Choi, S. & Bowerman, M. (1991). Learning to express motion events in English and Korean: The influence of language-specific lexicalization patterns. Cognition 41, 83121.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Conning, A. M. & Byrne, R. W. (1984). Pointing to preschool children's spatial competence: A study in natural settings. Journal of Environmental Psychology 4, 165–75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cornell, E. H. & Heth, C. D. (2004). Memories of travel: Dead reckoning within the cognitive map. In Allen, G. (ed.) Human spatial memory: Remembering where, 191215. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Craton, L. G., Elicker, J., Plumert, J. M. & Pick, H. L. Jr. (1990). Children's use of frames of reference in communication of spatial location. Child Development 61, 1528–43.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Cat, C. (2000). Structure building and the acquisition of dislocations in child French. In Howell, C., Fish, S. A. & Keith-Lucas, T. (eds) Proceedings of the 24th annual Boston University Conference on Language Development, 242–52. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
DeLoache, J. S. (1987). Young children's understanding of models. In Fivush, R. & Hudson, J. A. (eds) Knowing and remembering in young children, 94126. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
De Vega, M. & Rodrigo, M. J. (2001). Updating spatial layouts mediated by pointing and labelling under physical and imaginary rotation. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology 13, 369–93.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Vega, M., Rodrigo, M. J., Ato, M., Dehn, D. M. & Barquero, B. (2002). How nouns and prepositions fit together: An exploration of the semantics of locative sentences. Discourse Processes 34, 117–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ervin-Tripp, S. (1970). Discourse agreement: How children answer questions. In Hayes, J. R. (ed.) Cognition and the development of language, 79106. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Finegan, E. (2004). Language: Its structure and use. London: Thomson Wadsworth.Google Scholar
Franco, F. & Gagliano, A. (2001). Toddlers' pointing when joint attention is obstructed. First Language 21, 289321.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Franklin, N. & Tversky, B. (1990). Searching imagined environments. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 119, 6376.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gauvain, M. (1993). The development of spatial thinking in everyday activity. Developmental Review 13, 92121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gopnik, A. (1988). Conceptual and semantic development as theory change. Mind and Language 3, 197217.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hazen, N. L., Lockman, J. J. & Pick, H. L. Jr. (1978). The development of children's representations of large-scale environments. Child Development 49, 623–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heft, H. (1996). The ecological approach to navigation: A Gibsonian perspective. In Portugali, J. (ed.) The construction of cognitive maps, 105–32. Dordrect: Kluwer Academic Publishers.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnston, J. R. (1988). Children's verbal representation of spatial location. In Stiles-Davis, J., Kritchevsky, M. and Bellugi, U. (eds) Spatial cognition, brain bases and development, 196205. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Kannass, K. N., Plumert, J. M., McDermott, J., Moore, B. & Durich, N. (2004). The role of the physical context in supporting young children's use of spatiotemporal organization in recall. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 22, 5982.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Learmonth, A. E., Newcombe, N. S. & Huttenlocher, J. (2001). Toddler's use of metric information and landmarks to reorient. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 80, 225–44.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lehnung, M. H., Haaland, V. Ø., Pohl, J. & Leplow, B. (2001). Compass- versus finger-pointing tasks: The influence of different methods on age-related orientation performance in children. Journal of Environmental Psychology 21, 283–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lieven, E., Behrens, H., Speares, J. & Tomasello, M. (2003). Early syntactic creativity: A usage-based approach. Journal of Child Language 30, 333–70.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Masur, E. (1983). Gestural development, dual-directional signalling, and the transition to words. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 12, 93109.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCune-Nicolich, L. (1981). The cognitive bases of relational words in the single word period. Journal of Child Language 8, 1534.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Montello, D. R. & Pick, H. L. (1993). Integrating knowledge of vertically aligned large-scale spaces. Environment and Behavior 25, 457–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Newcombe, N., Huttenlocher, J., Drummey, A. B. & Wiley, J. G. (1998). The development of spatial location coding: Place learning and dead reckoning in the second and third years. Cognitive Development 13, 185200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Newcombe, N. S. & Learmonth, A. (1999). Change and continuity in early spatial development: Claiming the ‘radical middle’. Infant Behavior and Development 22, 457–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nicoladis, E. (2001). The use of “where” questions. Unpublished manuscript, University of Alberta.Google Scholar
Nicoladis, E. (2002). Some gestures develop in conjunction with spoken language development and others don't: Evidence from bilingual preschoolers. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior 26, 241–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nicoladis, E., Mayberry, R. & Genesee, F. (1999). Gesture and early bilingual development. Developmental Psychology 35, 514–26.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pick, H. L. Jr. & Lockman, J. J. (1982). Development of spatial cognition in children. In Baird, J. C. & Lutkas, A. D. (eds) Mind, child, architecture, 4863. Hanover, NH: University Press of New England.Google Scholar
Plumert, J. M. & Nichols-Whitehead, P. (1996). Parental scaffolding of young children's spatial communication. Developmental Psychology 32, 523–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Plumert, J. M., Pick, H. L. Jr., Marks, R. A., Kintsch, A. S. & Wegesin, D. (1994). Locating objects and communicating about locations: Differences in children's searches and direction-giving. Developmental Psychology 30, 443–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Plumert, J. M., Spalding, T. L. & Nichols-Whitehead, P. (2001). Preferences for ascending and descending hierarchical organization in spatial communication. Memory and Cognition 29, 274–84.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rice, S. (1999). Patterns of acquisition in the emerging mental lexicon: The case of to and for in English. Brain and Language 68, 268–76.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Siegel, A. W. & White, S. H. (1975). The development of spatial representations of large scale environments. In Reese, H. W. (ed.) Advances in child development and behavior, Vol. 10, 955. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Taylor, H. A. (2000). A view of space through language. In Kitchin, R. & Freundshuh, S. (eds) Cognitive mapping: Past, present and future, 177–96. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Uttal, D. H. (1996). Angles and distances: Children's and adults' reconstruction and scaling of spatial configurations. Child Development 67, 2763–79.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Westbury, C. & Nicoladis, E. (1998). Meaning in children's first words: Implications for a theory of lexical ontology. In Greenhill, A., Hughes, M., Littlefield, H. & Walsh, H. (eds) Proceedings of the 22nd Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development, 768–78. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar