Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-mkpzs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-27T09:29:56.382Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Early verb constructions in French: adjacency on the left edge

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  21 October 2015

EDY VENEZIANO*
Affiliation:
Université Paris Descartes – CNRS, and Laboratory MoDyCo (UMR 7114) & LPPS (EA 4057)
EVE V. CLARK*
Affiliation:
Stanford University
*
Addresses for correspondence: Edy Veneziano, Université Paris Descartes, Institut de Psychologie, 71 Avenue Edouard Vaillant, Office 4075, 92100 Boulogne Billancourt, France. e-mail: edy.veneziano@parisdescartes.fr
Eve V. Clark, Department of Linguistics, Stanford University, Stanford, CA 94305, USA; e-mail : eclark@stanford.edu

Abstract

Children acquiring French elaborate their early verb constructions by adding adjacent morphemes incrementally at the left edge of core verbs. This hypothesis was tested with 2657 verb uses from four children between 1;3 and 2;7. Consistent with the Adjacency Hypothesis, children added clitic subjects first only to present tense forms (as in il saute ‘he jumps’); modals to infinitives (as in faut sauter ‘has to jump’); and auxiliaries to past participles (as in a sauté ‘has jumped’). Only after this did the children add subjects to the left of a modal or auxiliary, as in elle veut sauter ‘she wants to jump’, or elle a sauté ‘she has jumped’. The order in which these elements were added, and the development in the frequencies of the constructions, all support the predictions of the Adjacency Hypothesis for left edge development in early verb constructions.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2015 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Ågren, M. & van de Weijer, J. (2013). Input frequency and the acquisition of subject–verb agreement in number in spoken and written French. Journal of French Language Studies 23, 311–33.Google Scholar
Aksu-Koç, A. (1988). The role of input vs. universal predispositions in the emergence of tense–aspect morphology: evidence from Turkish. First Language 18, 255–80.Google Scholar
Armon-Lotem, S. & Berman, R. A. (2003). The emergence of grammar: early verbs and beyond. Journal of Child Language 30, 845–77.Google Scholar
Ashkenazi, O. (2015). Input–output relations in the early acquisition of Hebrew verbs. Unpublished PhD dissertation, The Jaime and Joan Constantiner School of Education, Tel Aviv University.Google Scholar
Belikova, A., Kupisch, T., Özçelik, Ö. & Sadlier-Brown, E. (2009). Fillers as functional categories: evidence from German–English bilingual acquisition. In Crawford, J., Otaki, K. & Takahashi, Masahiko (eds), Proceedings of the 3rd Conference on Generative Approaches to Language Acquisition in North America (GALANA 2008), 112. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Proceedings Project. Online: <www.lingref.com>, document #2301.Google Scholar
Bloom, L., Lifter, K. & Hafitz, J. (1980). Semantics of verbs and the development of verb inflection in child language. Language 56, 386412.Google Scholar
Bowerman, M. (1973). Structural relations in children's utterances: syntactic or semantic? In Moore, T. E. (ed.), Cognitive development and the acquisition of language, 197213. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Carstairs-McCarthy, A. (1992). Current morphology. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Chouinard, M. M. & Clark, E. V. (2003). Adult reformulations of child errors as negative evidence. Journal of Child Language 30, 637–69.Google Scholar
Christensen, L. (2010). Early verbs in child Swedish – a diary study on two boys. Nordlund 30, Småskrifter från Nordiska språk, University of Lund.Google Scholar
Clark, E. V. (1987). The principle of contrast: a constraint on language acquisition. In MacWhinney, B. (ed.), Mechanisms of language acquisition, 133. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Clark, E. V. (1990). The pragmatics of contrast. Journal of Child Language 17, 417–31.Google Scholar
Clark, E. V. (1993). The lexicon in acquisition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Clark, E. V. & Bernicot, J. (2008). Repetition as ratification: how parents and children place information in common ground. Journal of Child Language 35, 349–71.Google Scholar
Clark, E. V. & de Marneffe, M.-C. (2012). Constructing verb paradigms in French: adult construals and emerging grammatical contrasts. Morphology 22, 89120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Culbertson, J. & Legendre, G. (2008). Qu'en est-il des clitiques sujet en français oral contemporain? In Durand, J., Habert, B. & Laks, B. (eds), Actes du 1er Congrès Mondial de Linguistique Française, 2651–62. Paris: EDP Sciences.Google Scholar
Cutler, A., Hawkins, J. A. & Gilligan, G. (1985). The suffixing preference: a processing explanation. Linguistics 23, 723–58.Google Scholar
Demuth, K. & Tremblay, A. (2008). Prosodically-conditioned variability in children's production of French determiners. Journal of Child Language 35, 99127.Google Scholar
de Villiers, J. G. (1985). Learning how to use verbs: lexical coding and the influence of the input. Journal of Child Language 12, 587–95.Google Scholar
Diesendruck, G. & Markson, L. (2001). Children's avoidance of lexical overlap: a pragmatic account. Developmental Psychology 27, 630–41.Google Scholar
van Dijk, M. & van Geert, P. (2005). Disentangling behavior in early child development: interpretability of early child language and its effect on utterance length measures. Infant Behavior and Development 28, 99117.Google Scholar
Dressler, W. U., Kilani-Schoch, M., Gargarina, N., Pestal, L. & Pöchtrager, M. (2006). On the typology of inflection class systems. Folia Linguistica 40, 5174.Google Scholar
Dye, C. (2011). Reduced auxiliaries in early child language: converging observational and experimental evidence from French. Journal of Linguistics 47, 301–39.Google Scholar
Ellis, N. C. & Sagarra, N. (2011). Learned attention in adult language acquisition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 33, 589624.Google Scholar
Estigarribia, B. (2010). Facilitation by variation: right-to-left learning of English yes/no questions. Cognitive Science 34(1), 6893.Google Scholar
Gathercole, V. C. M., Sebastián, E. & Soto, P. (1999). The early acquisition of Spanish verbal morphology: across-the-board or piecemeal knowledge? International Journal of Bilingualism 3, 133–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodman, J. C., Dale, P. S. & Li, P. (2008). Does frequency count? Parental input and the acquisition of vocabulary. Journal of Child Language 38, 515–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grevisse, M. & Goosse, A. (2008). Le Bon Usage: Grammaire Française, 14th ed. Bruxelles: De Boeck, Duculot.Google Scholar
Harner, L. (1980). Comprehension of past and future reference revisited. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 29, 170–82.Google Scholar
Hickey, T. (1991). Mean length of utterance and the acquisition of Irish. Journal of Child Language 18, 553–69.Google Scholar
Hirsh-Pasek, K. & Golinkoff, R. M. (1999). The origins of grammar: evidence from early language comprehension. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Höhle, B. & Weissenborn, J. (2003). German-learning infants’ ability to detect unstressed closed-class elements in continuous speech. Developmental Science 6, 122–7.Google Scholar
Hupp, J. M., Sloutsky, V. M. & Culicover, P. W. (2009). Evidence for a domain-general mechanism underlying the suffixation preference in language. Language and Cognitive Processes 24, 876909.Google Scholar
Jordens, P. (2002). Finiteness in early child Dutch. Linguistics 40, 687765.Google Scholar
Kilani-Schoch, M. (2000). Early verb development in two French-speaking children. Zentrum für Allgemeine Sprachwissenschaft Papers in Linguistics 18, 7997.Google Scholar
Klein, W. (2012). The information structure of French. In Krifka, M. & Musan, R. (eds), The expression of information structure, 94125. Berlin: Mouton.Google Scholar
Küntay, A. & Slobin, D. I. (2002). Putting interaction back into child language: examples from Turkish. Psychology of Language and Communication 6, 514.Google Scholar
Leeman-Bouix, D. (2005). Grammaire du Verbe Français: Des Formes aux Sens––Modes, Aspects, Temps, Auxiliaires. Paris: A. Colin.Google Scholar
Le Goffic, P. (1997). Les Formes Conjuguées du Verbe Français. Paris: Ophrys.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (2000). The CHILDES Project: tools for analyzing talk, 2 vols., 3rd ed. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Miller, P. & Monachesi, P. (2003). Les pronoms clitiques dans les langues romanes. In Godard, D. (ed.), Les Langues Romanes : Problèmes de la Phase Simple, 67123. Paris: CNRS.Google Scholar
Mithun, M. (1989). The acquisition of polysynthesis. Journal of Child Language 16, 285312.Google Scholar
Naigles, L. R. & Hoff-Ginsberg, E. (1995). Input to verb learning: evidence for the plausibility of syntactic bootstrapping. Developmental Psychology 31, 827–37.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Naigles, L. R. & Hoff-Ginsberg, E. (1998). Why are some verbs learned before other verbs? Effects of input frequency and structure on children's early verb use. Journal of Child Language 25, 95120.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
New, B., Pallier, C., Ferrand, L. & Matos, R. (2001). Une base de données lexicales du français contemporain sur internet: LEXIQUE. Année Psychologique 101, 447–62.Google Scholar
Parker, M. D. & Brorson, K. (2005). A comparative study between mean length of utterance in morphemes (MLUm) and mean length of utterance in words (MLUw). First Language 25, 365–76.Google Scholar
Peters, A. M. & Menn, L. (1993). False starts and filler-syllables: ways to learn grammatical morphemes. Language 69, 742–77.Google Scholar
Pizzuto, E. & Caselli, M. C. (1994). The acquisition of Italian verb morphology: in a cross- linguistic perspective. In Levy, Y. (ed.), Other children, other languages: issues in the theory of language acquisition 137–87. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Pye, C. (1983). Mayan telegraphese: intonational units in the development of Quiché Mayan. Language 59, 583604.Google Scholar
Richards, B. J. & Robinson, W. P. (1993). Environmental correlates of child copula verb growth. Journal of Child Language 20, 343–62.Google Scholar
Rojas-Nieto, C. (2011). Developing first contrasts in Spanish verb inflection: usage and interaction. In Arnon, I. & Clark, E. V. (eds), Experience, variation, and generalization: learning a first language, 5372. Amsterdam: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Rowland, C. F. & Fletcher, S. L. (2006). The effect of sampling on estimates of lexical specificity and error rates. Journal of Child Language 33, 859–77.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salazar Orvig, A., Marcos, H., Morgenstern, A., Hassan, R., Leber-Marin, J. & Parès, J. (2010). Dialogical factors in toddlers’ use of clitic pronouns. First Language 30, 375402.Google Scholar
Shady, M. & Gerken, L. A. (1999). Grammatical and caregiver cues in early sentence comprehension. Journal of Child Language 26, 163–75.Google Scholar
Shipley, E. F., Smith, C. S. & Gleitman, L. R. (1969). A study in the acquisition of language: free responses to commands. Language 45, 322–42.Google Scholar
Slobin, D. I. (1973). Cognitive prerequisites for the development of grammar. In Ferguson, C. A. & Slobin, D. I. (eds), Studies of child language development, 175208. New York: Holt Rinehart & Winston.Google Scholar
Slobin, D. I. (1985). Crosslinguistic evidence for the language-making capacity. In Slobin, D. I. (ed.), The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition, vol. 2, 1157–256. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Soderstrom, M., White, K. S., Conwell, E. & Morgan, J. L. (2007). Receptive grammatical knowledge of familiar content words and inflection in 16-month-olds. Infancy 12, 129.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (1992). First verbs: a case study of early grammatical development. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. & Stahl, D. (2004). Sampling children's spontaneous speech: How much is enough? Journal of Child Language 31, 101–21.Google Scholar
Valian, V. (2006). Young children's understanding of present and past tense. Language Learning and Development 2, 251–76.Google Scholar
Veneziano, E. (1988). Vocal–verbal interaction and the construction of early lexical knowledge. In Smith, M. D. & Locke, J. L. (eds), The emergent lexicon: the child's development of a linguistic vocabulary, 109–47. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Veneziano, E. (1999). Early lexical, morphological and syntactic development in French: some complex relations. International Journal of Bilingualism 3, 183217.Google Scholar
Veneziano, E. (2003). The emergence of noun and verb categories in the acquisition of French. Psychology of Language and Communication 7, 2336.Google Scholar
Veneziano, E. (2005). Effects of conversational functioning on early language acquisition: when both caregivers and children matter. In Bokus, B. (ed.), Studies in the psychology of child language: essays in honor of Grace Wales Shugar, 4769. Warsaw: Matrix.Google Scholar
Veneziano, E. & Parisse, C. (2010). The acquisition of early verbs in French: assessing the role of conversation and of child-directed input. First Language 30, 287311.Google Scholar
Veneziano, E. & Sinclair, H. (2000). The changing status of ‘filler syllables’ on the way to grammatical morphemes. Journal of Child Language 27, 461500.Google Scholar
Veneziano, E., Sinclair, H. & Berthoud, I. (1990). From one word to two words: repetition patterns on the way to structured speech. Journal of Child Language 17, 633650.Google Scholar